Chapter 10 Aversive Control: Avoidance and Punishment.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Instrumental Learning & Operant Reinforcement
Advertisements

Instrumental Conditioning Also called Operant Conditioning.
Chapter 10 – Aversive Control: Avoidance and Punishment
Chapter 6: Learning. Classical Conditioning Ivan Pavlov A type of learning in which a neutral stimulus acquires the ability to elicit a response. How.
PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 9, Theories and Applications of Aversive Conditioning.
Aversive Conditioning. ReinforcementPunishment Positive contingency Negative contingency Chocolate BarElectric Shock Excused from Chores No TV privileges.
Establishing a Desirable Behavior by Using Escape and Avoidance Conditioning Chapter 13.
Inhibitory Pavlovian Conditioning Stimuli can become conditioned to signal the absence of a US— such learning is called Inhibitory Conditioning CS+ = excitatory.
THEORIES AND APPLICATIONS OF AVERSIVE CONDITIONING Chapter 7 1.
There’s never just one reinforcer Hmm…what to do?
Aversive Control of Behavior: Punishment & Avoidance Lesson 16.
Operant Conditioning. I. Operant Conditioning A type of learning that occurs when we receive rewards or punishments for our behavior A type of learning.
How do animals “know” when a schedule is on extinction?
Lecture 21: Avoidance Learning & Punishment Learning, Psychology 5310 Spring, 2015 Professor Delamater.
PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 8 – Aversive Conditioning.
Avoidance Conditioning Combining Classical and Operant Conditioning Classical and operant conditioning often take place in the same situation. We saw this.
Aversive Control: Avoidance and Punishment
Copyright © 2005 Pearson Education Canada Inc. Learning Chapter 5.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 10 – A Synthetic Perspective on Instrumental Learning.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning
PSY402 Theories of Learning Wednesday, November 19, 2003 Chapter 6 -- Traditional Theories (Cont.)
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 9 – Motivation.
Negative Reinforcement
Operant Conditioning Thomas G. Bowers, Ph.D. Penn State Harrisburg.
OPERANT CONDITIONING DEF: a form of learning in which responses come to be controlled by their consequences.
Learning the Consequences of Behavior
LEARNING HOW DO YOU LEARN BEST??. Ivan Pavlov and the role of Serendipity Russian physiologist studying the digestive system Focusing on what substance.
Learning.
Chapter 6: Learning. Classical Conditioning Ivan Pavlov Terminology –Unconditioned Stimulus (UCS): evokes an unconditioned response without previous conditioning.
Learning Prof. Tom Alloway. Definition of Learning l Change in behavior l Due to experience relevant to what is being learned l Relatively durable n Conditioning.
Chapter 6: Learning. Classical Conditioning Ivan Pavlov Terminology –Unconditioned Stimulus (UCS) –Conditioned Stimulus (CS) –Unconditioned Response (UCR)
Aversive Control Negative Reinforcement Avoidance Learning Escape Learning.
Chapter 6 Learning. Table of Contents Classical conditioning Ivan Pavlov Terminology –Unconditioned Stimulus (UCS) –Conditioned Stimulus (CS) –Unconditioned.
Chapter 6 Learning.
B.F. SKINNER - "Skinner box": -many responses -little time and effort -easily recorded -RESPONSE RATE is the Dependent Variable.
Chapter 6: Learning 1Ch. 6. – Relatively permanent change in behavior due to experience 1. Classical Conditioning : Pairing 2. Operant Conditioning :
4 th Edition Copyright 2004 Prentice Hall5-1 Learning Chapter 5.
Learning Chapter Eight. Definitions Learning-the process by which experience or practice results in a relatively permanent change in an organism’s behavior.
CHAPTER 4 Pavlovian Conditioning: Causal Factors.
Chapter 5 Learning. chapter 5 What is Learning? Occurs whenever experience or practice results in a relatively permanent change in behavior.
LEARNING: PRINCIPLES AND APPLICATIONS Operant Conditioning.
Psychology 2250 Last Class Characteristics of Habituation and Sensitization -time course -stimulus-specificity -effects of strong extraneous stimuli (dishabituation)
Table of Contents CHAPTER 6 Learning. Table of ContentsLEARNING  Learning  Classical conditioning  Operant/Instrumental conditioning  Observational.
PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 6 – Appetitive Conditioning.
Copyright McGraw-Hill, Inc Chapter 5 Learning.
Learning Experiments and Concepts.  What is learning?
SKINNER’S “THEORY” OF INSTRUMENTAL CONDITIONING
Operant conditioning (Skinner – 1938, 1956)
Knowledge acquired in this way.
Chapter 6: Learning.
Rescorla’s Experiment Contingencies in Classical Conditioning Three Phases Phase 1: Avoidance Conditioning: Establish a behavioral measure (operational.
Extinction of Conditioned Behavior Effects of Extinction  the rate of responding decreases  response variability increases  experiment by Neuringer,
Unit 6: Learning. How Do We Learn? Learning = a relatively permanent change in an organism’s behavior due to experience. 3 Types:  Classical  Operant.
Learning Definition: The process of acquiring new and enduring information or behaviors Associative learning is the key Conditioning – the process of.
PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 – Appetitive Conditioning.
Chapter 6 FLASH CARD CHALLENGE!!!
Blocking The phenomenon of blocking tells us that what happens to one CS depends not only on its relationship to the US but also on the strength of other.
Operant Conditioning – Chapter 9 Theories of Learning October 19, 2005 Class #25.
Unit 1 Review 1. To say that learning has taken place, we must observe a change in a subject’s behavior. What two requirements must this behavioral change.
Extinction of Conditioned Behavior Chapter 9 Effects of Extinction Extinction and Original Learning What is learned during Extinction.
Table of Contents Chapter 6 Learning. Table of Contents Learning –Classical conditioning –Operant/Instrumental conditioning –Observational learning Ivan.
PSY402 Theories of Learning Monday February 10, 2003.
4 th Edition Copyright 2004 Prentice Hall5-1 Psychology Stephen F. Davis Emporia State University Joseph J. Palladino University of Southern Indiana PowerPoint.
Chapter 7 The Associative Structure of Instrumental Conditioning.
Operant Conditioning. A type of learning in which the frequency of a behavior depends on the consequence that follows that behavior. The frequency will.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 10 – A Synthetic Perspective on Instrumental Learning.
Basic Learning Processes Robert C. Kennedy, PhD University of Central Florida
Chapter 6 LEARNING. Learning Learning – A process through which experience produces lasting change in behavior or mental processes. Behavioral Learning.
PSY402 Theories of Learning
Classical Conditioning
Presentation transcript:

Chapter 10 Aversive Control: Avoidance and Punishment

Instrumental Conditioning Procedures Positive Reinforcement Response increases Punishment Response decreases Omission Training Response decreases Negative Reinforcement Response increases

 Negative reinforcement – also called escape/avoidance  Avoidance procedures increase the operant response  Punishment procedures decrease the operant response  With both types of procedures, the behavior that develops serves to minimize contact with the aversive stimulus Aversive Control  Critical difference: in avoidance, taking a specific action prevents the aversive stimulus in punishment, refraining from action minimizes contact with the aversive stimulus

Aversive Control Avoidance behavior is sometimes referred to as active avoidance Punishment is sometimes referred to as passive avoidance Both terms emphasize the fact that both avoidance and punishment involve minimizing contact with an aversive stimulus

Avoidance Behavior  origins in Pavlovian conditioning  first experiments conducted by Bechterev (1913) Participants instructed to place a finger on a metal plate A warning stimulus (CS) was then presented, followed by a brief shock (US) The participants quickly lifted their finger off the plate after being shocked After a few trials, they also learned to make the response during the CS This experiment viewed as a standard example of Pavlovian conditioning

Avoidance Behavior In the 1930s people focused on the difference between a standard classical conditioning procedure and a procedure that had an instrumental avoidance component added Brogden, Lipman, & Culler (1938) Tested 2 groups of guinea pigs in a rotating wheel A tone served as the CS and a shock as the US The shock stimulated the animals to run and rotate the wheel For the classical conditioning group, the shock was presented 2 s after the onset of the tone For the avoidance conditioning group, the shock also followed the tone when the animals did not make the CR (a small movement of the wheel) if the avoidance animals moved the wheel during the tone CS before the shock occurred, the scheduled shock was omitted

Brogden, Lipman, & Culler (1938) Results: Figure 10.2 These results showed that avoidance conditioning is different than standard classical conditioning

The Discriminated Avoidance Procedure

Discriminated, or Signalled, Avoidance A warning stimulus (e.g., a light) signals a forthcoming S Aversive (e.g., a shock) If the required response is made during the light (warning stimulus), before the shock (S Aversive ) occurs, the subject avoids the shock. If a response is not made during the warning stimulus of the light, the shock (S Aversive ) occurs, and terminates when the required response is made (i.e., escape).

Discriminated, or Signalled, Avoidance Discriminated avoidance procedures are often conducted in a shuttle box  the shuttle box consists of 2 compartments separated by a barrier  the animal is placed on one side of the apparatus  at the start of the trial, a CS is presented  if the animal crosses to the other side before the shock is presented, then no shock occurs and the CS goes off  after the inter-trial interval, the next trial can be started with the animal in the second compartment  shuttle avoidance  two-way shuttle avoidance or one-way shuttle avoidance (one-way avoidance easier to learn)

The Two-Process Theory of Avoidance Avoidance procedures involve a negative contingency between a response and an aversive stimulus The absence of the aversive stimulus is presumably the reason that avoidance responses are made But, how can the absence of something provide reinforcement for instrumental behavior?

The Two-Process Theory of Avoidance Explains avoidance learning in terms of two necessary processes: First, the subject learns to associate the warning stimulus with the S Aversive – what is this? This is a classical conditioning process; the warning stimulus of the light is the CS, the S Aversive of shock is the US. CS (light)US (shock)UR (fear) CR (fear)

Now, the subject can be negatively reinforced during the warning stimulus; this is the second, operant conditioning process RCS Removes i.e., reduces fear Strengthens Thus the two-process theory reduces avoidance learning to escape learning; the organism learns to escape from the CS and the fear that it elicits. The Two-Process Theory of Avoidance

Support for Two-Process Theory of Avoidance Acquired-Drive Experiments If Two-Process theory is right, then separating the two processes should still lead to successful learning. First, classical conditioning to acquire fear of CS Second, escape training with CS as S Aversive ; will the subject learn a response to escape from just the CS (i.e., US no longer presented)? Two phases to acquired-drive experiments: In the typical avoidance procedure, classical conditioning of fear and instrumental reinforcement through fear reduction occur intermixed in a series of trial

Acquired-Drive Experiment Brown & Jacobs (1949)  tested rats in a shuttle box  in phase 1 (classical conditioning), rats confined to one side of the apparatus and given 22 CS-shock pairings  in phase 2 (instrumental conditioning), rats were placed on one side of the apparatus with the center barrier removed  the CS was presented and remained on until the rat turned it off by crossing to the other side (no shocks presented)  how long the rats took to cross the shuttle box and turn off the CS was measured for each trial

Brown & Jacobs (1949) Results: Figure 10.6 Organisms do learn to escape from the CS, supporting the Two-Process Theory of Avoidance.

Evidence that questions the Two-Process Theory of Avoidance However, the level of fear is not always positively correlated with avoidance If fear motivates and reinforces avoidance responding, then the conditioning of fear and the conditioning of instrumental avoidance behavior should be highly correlated Animals often become less fearful as they become more proficient in performing the avoidance response

Kamin, Brimer, & Black (1963) If the warning signal in an avoidance procedure comes to elicit fear, then presentation of that stimulus in a conditioned suppression procedure should result in suppression of behavior  Rats initially trained to bar-press for food  rats then trained to avoid shock in response to an auditory CS in a shuttle-box  training was continued for separate groups until they avoided the shock on 1, 3, 9 or 27 consecutive trials  the animals were then returned to the Skinner box for bar pressing  the CS that had been used in the shuttle box was periodically presented to see how much suppression of bar pressing it would produce

Kamin, Brimer, & Black (1963) Results: figure 10.7

Kamin, Brimer, & Black (1963) With more extensive avoidance training, response suppression declined Animals trained until they avoided the shock on 27 consecutive trials showed less conditioned suppression to the avoidance CS than those trained to a criterion of 9 consecutive avoidances This suggests that fear, as measured by conditioned suppression, decreases during extended avoidance training However, this decrease in fear is not accompanied by a decrease in the strength of the avoidance response

Asymptotic Avoidance performance Two-process theory predicts that the strength of the avoidance response should fluctuate in cycles  when a successful avoidance response occurs, the shock is omitted on that trial  this is an extinction trial for the conditioned fear response  repetition of the avoidance response (and thus the CS-alone extinction trials) should lead to extinction of fear  as the CS becomes extinguished, there will be less reinforcement resulting from the reduction of fear, and the avoidance response should also become extinguished  however, when the shock is not avoided, the CS is paired with the US  this should reinstate fear to the CS and re-establish the potential for reinforcement through fear reduction, thereby reconditioning the avoidance response

Asymptotic Avoidance performance Thus, two-process theory predicts that after initial acquisition, the avoidance response will go through cycles of extinction and re-acquisition However, this does not always happen Avoidance behavior can be very persistent

Rats given shocks according to a shock-shock (S C S) interval (e.g., a shock every 5 s) unless they make a response to delay the shock according to a response-shock (R C S) interval (e.g., 30 s). Free-operant avoidance  also called nondiscriminated avoidance or Sidman avoidance  shock postponement procedure  no warning signal Problem for two-process theory?