Does daily limited access to a highly palatable sweet food induce binge eating in laboratory rats? Katie Isbell, Robynn Mackechnie, Pres Garrett, and G.R.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Nonstandard Feeding and Watering. What is nonstandard feeding/watering? Any deviation from standard feeding/watering provided by LAF care staff. Examples:
Advertisements

Do College Students Perceive Healthier Food as Being More Expensive? Evaluating How Perceptions Can Affect Food Choice SIERRA S. HOWRY, A. DEAN MONROE:
Does Texture Affect Appetite? A Study of Sensory Specific Satiety in College-Aged Males Project Leader: Amelia Keaton.
Chowing Down: Do Rats Eat Less When They Eat Slower? Elise Boos Prashant Patel Margaret Strom Biology 104 Section B.
Subjects: Twenty-two male Sprague-Dawley rats were housed individually in plastic cages with bedding on a 12:12 light:dark cycle (lights off at 0900) and.
The Effects of Vicarious Learning with Experienced Rats Jessie Young and Lyndsey Wells Randolph College Lynchburg, VA Introduction Vicarious learning.
Limited Intermittent Access to a Highly Palatable Food Combined with Chronic Mild Stress Induces Overeating in Laboratory Rats Kishan Govind, Robynn Mackechnie,
Examination of the State Dependent Properties of WIN on Spatial Learning and Memory in Rats in the Sand Maze Ashley R. Smith and Gretchen Hanson.
Do low-carbohydrate diets afford a metabolic advantage that causes more weight loss than that which occurs in individuals on conventional, high-carbohydrate,
Research Question In obese individuals who lose more weight on a low- carbohydrate diet versus a conventional diet, what are the underlying mechanisms?
MARION M. HETHERINGTON, MARTIN F. REGAN EFFECTS OF CHEWING GUM ON SHORT- TERM APPETITE REGULATION IN MODERATELY RESTRAINED EATERS By: Eliza Wendel.
Chapter 8. Fig 23.1  “Competition eating” ◦ Eating strategies  Before, during and after competition ◦ “preparation”  Weeks, months prior.
How to use Mood Food Diary: Everyday fill in the food diary by stating what you ate and how you felt an hour after eating Please record your mood, energy.
Project Leader: Katie Kavanagh
Sleep Deprivation Effects on Memory and Anxiety in Mice Eyitemi Opuogen Department of Biology, York College of Pennsylvania Sleep is imperative for a person’s.
E ATING B EHAVIORS By: Patrick Thibodeau. E XPERIMENT 1: S TRESS - INDUCED LABORATORY EATING BEHAVIOR IN OBESE WOMEN WITH BINGE EATING DISORDER. Purpose.
Manipulating the Sensory Properties of Food Enhances Food Intake in Rats Under Simulated Microgravity: a Demonstration of Sensory Specific Satiety with.
Light and Dark Phase Maternal and Paternal Behaviors in the Polygamous House Mouse (Mus musculus domesticus) April Sparkman, Department of Biological Science,
Taurine Supplementation Improves the Utilization of Sulfur-Containing Amino Acids in Consecutive Alcohol Administration Rats Hui-Ting Yang 1, Ching-Chien.
ClearH 2 O Duration Assessment of DietGel 31M & HydroGel Bilsky Laboratory University of New England August 6, 2012.
Title about SSS and food preference (in the form of a question or answer- group did not decide) Tim Beckner, Michelle Bertling, Jai-Sheena Garnett, Allison.
DESIPRAMINE BLOCKS THE DEPRESSIVE EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL IN FEMALE WKY RATS Sheketha R. Hauser 1*, Bruk Getachew 2, Robert E. Taylor 2, Yousef Tizabi 2 1 Department.
Proposal Introduction Presentation By: Torie Lembersky.
The effects of continuous-suckling and supplementation on growth performance of crossbred Katahdin lambs (Birth weight 4.2 ± 0.2 kg) were investigated.
Does Scheduled Intermittent Access to a Highly Palatable Carbohydrate-Rich Food Trigger Binge Eating in Laboratory Rats? Robynn Mackechnie, Kishan Govind,
Twenty male Sprague-Dawley rats previously used for other purposes (Mackechnie et al., 2008 and Govind et al., 2008) were used in this pilot study. Rats.
The Effects of Simulated Microgravity on Sensory Specific Satiety in Rats Using the Hindlimb Unloading Model Bio 250 and 450 classes Presenters: Elizabeth.
Twenty male Sprague-Dawley rats previously used for other purposes (Mackechnie et al., 2008 and Govind et al., 2008) were used in this pilot study. Rats.
EFFECTS OF CHRONIC ALCOHOL ON BEHAVIOR AND ALPHA-2 ADRENOCEPTORS IN TWO RAT STRAINS B. Getachew*, S. R. Hauser, J. R. Das, C. Ramlochansingh, B. Bhatti,
The Hypothalamopituitary-adrenal axis and alcohol preference Matthew J. O’Callaghan, Adam P. Croft, Catherine Jacquot, Hillary J. Little Presented by Muharema.
Rats (Rattus norvegicus) modulate eating speed and vigilance to optimize food consumption: Effects of cover, circadian rhythm, food deprivation, and individual.
Does Texture Affect Appetite
Different types of lights have been used to increase the growth of various plants. Our experiment tested whether LED or fluorescent lights produce plants.
Different types of lights have been used to increase the growth of various plants. Our experiment tested whether LED or fluorescent lights produce plants.
Cortisol,Hunger, and Desire to Binge Eat Following a Cold Stress Test in Obese Women with Binge Eating Disorder Gluck ME, Geliebter A, Hung J, and Yahav.
E ff ects of duloxetine in rats trained to discriminate between 2- and 22- hr food deprivation M.A. Vanden Avond, A.R. Johnson, K.A. Reed, B.R. Gomer, D.J.
The Effect of Sensory Deprivation on Neuroplasticity Neuroplasticity: refers to the brain’s ability to rearrange the connection between its neurons – that.
Tim Beckner, Michelle Bertling, Jai-Sheena Garnett, Allison Thomas,
A GLASS OF RED WINE IS THE EQUIVALENT TO AN HOUR AT THE GYM, SAYS NEW STUDY By: Ryan Milani, Justin Shock, Mattie Crisler, Blake Barbour, and Jacob Dickman.
Chowing Down: Do Rats Eat Less When They Eat Slower? Elise Boos Prashant Patel Margaret Strom Biology 104 Section B.
The Effects of Different Types of Food Reinforcers on Performance in the T-Maze Sarah A. Linderman, Fahima Vakalia, and Laura Revetta Randolph-Macon Woman’s.
Hunger.
Effects of palatability on food, caloric, and macronutrient intake in a three-course meal. Tim Beckner, Michelle Bertling, Jai-Sheena Garnett, Allison.
Friday, October 5 Revising Paragraphs Writing Process Map.
Identification of Brain Regions Activated with Arousal- Induced Clock Resetting in Mice Jonathan E. Schenk, Andrew T. Schultz, and Robert G. Olson with.
Social Stress in Female Sprague- Dawley Rats: Open Field University of Wyoming Spring 2009 Department of Neuroscience Independent study by Zachary Yaple.
Effects of palatability on food, caloric, and macronutrient intake in a three-course meal. Tim Beckner, Michelle Bertling, Jai-Sheena Garnett, Allison.
Difference in Mls poured between the subject and the researcher
Carbon dioxide in carbonated beverages induces ghrelin release and increased food consumption in male rats: Implications on the onset of obesity  Dureen.
Short-term effects of chewing gum on snack intake and appetite
Tim Beckner, Michelle Bertling, Jai-Sheena Garnett, Allison Thomas,
Justin Yates & Michael Bardo
Examination of the State Dependent Properties of WIN on Spatial Learning and Memory in Rats in the Sand Maze   Ashley R. Smith and Gretchen Hanson.
Does slowing the rate of access to a highly palatable food lead to reduced food consumption in laboratory rats? Travis Boyd, Brandon Hunt, Kelsi Koenig.
Tim Beckner, Michelle Bertling, Jai-Sheena Garnett, Allison Thomas,
Vicarious Learning in Laboratory Rats
Does Texture Affect Appetite
Overeating Induced by Injection of the Glutamate Receptor Antagonist, CNQX, within the Nucleus Accumbens of Satiated Rats Project Leader: Jenny McCallister.
Volume 138, Issue 7, Pages e3 (June 2010)
Central Nervous System Mechanisms Linking the Consumption of Palatable High-Fat Diets to the Defense of Greater Adiposity  Karen K. Ryan, Stephen C. Woods,
Sensory Deprivation Unmasks a PKA-Dependent Synaptic Plasticity Mechanism that Operates in Parallel with CaMKII  Neil Hardingham, Nick Wright, James Dachtler,
Sensory Deprivation Unmasks a PKA-Dependent Synaptic Plasticity Mechanism that Operates in Parallel with CaMKII  Neil Hardingham, Nick Wright, James Dachtler,
Inhibitory Interplay between Orexin Neurons and Eating
Volume 44, Issue 3, Pages (October 2004)
Volume 21, Issue 11, Pages (December 2017)
Results from Unrestricted Intake Experiments
Body weight, blood glucose, and iron status in STZ-induced type 1 diabetic rats with or without insulin therapy. Body weight, blood glucose, and iron status.
Tim Beckner, Michelle Bertling, Jai-Sheena Garnett, Allison Thomas,
Rescuing the Overpouring Effect: The impact of Perceived Drinking Situation in a Simulated Alcohol Free Pour Task Meredith Watson, Brianne Ackley, Lucas.
Sequence Experiment Rat given one of six course sequences
Presentation transcript:

Does daily limited access to a highly palatable sweet food induce binge eating in laboratory rats? Katie Isbell, Robynn Mackechnie, Pres Garrett, and G.R. Davis*. Department of Biology, Wofford College, Spartanburg, SC Acknowledgements The authors thank students in the Bio 250 Spring 2007 class at Wofford College for their involvement in this research: Jennifer Almond, Andy Barnes, KC Correll, Krupa Desai, Shana Glenn, Melissa Hayes, Hollis Inabinet, Kelsi Koenig, Jamie Newton, & Emily Scales. * Corresponding author: When rats or humans eat to satiety on one food, they consume very little of that same food offered as a second meal, but will consume a more palatable food offered as a second meal. This form of overeating is called sensory specific satiety (SSS). Another form of overeating is binge eating, defined as the consumption of a larger than normal amount of food within a given time. Episodes of binge eating in humans often follow periods of self-imposed restricted access to a palatable foods (i.e. dieting) and are frequently associated with mood and eating disorders. To understand the mechanisms responsible for binge eating, it would be useful to develop an animal model for which the triggers of binge eating can be investigated under controlled laboratory conditions. Laboratories pursuing an animal model of binge eating (Corwin, 2004; Boggiano et al., 2007) report that ad libitum daily access to a highly palatable food for 2 hours does not result in episodes of binge eating. Perhaps severely restricting daily access to a highly palatable food will trigger binge eating. In this study, we provided laboratory rats with a small quantity (1.1 g) of a highly palatable food (Froot Loops®, FL) every day for at least two weeks. We then measured FL intake when FL were available ad libitum to see if limited daily access resulted in binge eating. Food intake was compared to a group of rats which were not provided daily access to FL. Limited daily access to Froot Loops® triggers binge eating. Subjects: Data were obtained from 23 male Sprague- Dawley rats weighing 346 ± 12 g (range g, age = 102 days) on 1st experimental day. Animals were housed individually in plastic cages with lights on at 7 am and lights off at 7 pm. Diet: Daily ad libitum Teklad 8604 rodent chow and water except on experimental days. Prior to experiments, all rats were given 5 FL in a plastic cup to overcome neophobicity. Groups: Daily Access to 5 FL (~1.1 g provided irregularly during the light period) or No Daily Access to FL. Experimental days: Biweekly for 12 weeks following 18 hour food deprivation (to assure rats eat to satiety during first meal), rats were given a 1st meal of Chow (C) or FL at 8 am Cumulative Food Intake in grams was measured at 60 and 90 minutes. A 2nd meal of C or FL was provided when the 1st meal was removed. Cumulative Food Intake of the second meal was measured at 30 and 60 minutes. Statistics: Repeated Measures ANOVA followed by Student-Newman-Keuls (SNK) post hoc pair-wise testing for significant differences (p< 0.05). Counterbalanced Crossover Experimental Design Hypothesis Introduction Wk2Wk4Wk6Wk8Wk10Wk12 No Daily FL Access (n=11)Daily FL Access C-CFL-CC-FLC-CFL-CC-FL C-CFL-CC-FLC-CFL-C C-FLC-CFL-CC-FLC-C Daily FL Access (n=12)No Daily FL Access C-CFL-CC-FLC-CFL-CC-FL C-CFL-CC-FLC-CFL-C C-FLC-CFL-CC-FLC-C Methods Day of Experiment Results: Satiation in Meal 1 Results: Food Intake in grams by meal Status of Hypothesis Implications Each rat received each meal sequence (C-C, C-FL, and FL-C; counterbalanced) following at least two weeks of Daily Access or No Daily Access to FL. At the end of week 6, FL access was reversed for each group (arrows indicate crossover). 18 hr Food deprivation ending at 8:00 am 8:00 Pre-weighed Meal 1 placed in cage 9:00 Weigh and return Meal 1 9:30 Remove and weigh Meal 1, Place Meal 2 in cage 10:00 Weigh and return Meal 2 10:30 Remove and weigh Meal 2 Food Intake measured in grams for each meal. Rats eat to satiety in the 1st meal: Cumulative Food Intake at 90 minutes (gray bars) is not significantly different from intake at 60 Minutes (black bars.) Rats consume significantly more FL than chow as a 1 st meal. Limited access to FL did not affect intake in the 1 st meal. Histograms with different letters are significantly different (p<0.05). No Daily Access to FLLimited Daily Access to FL A A A A B B 1 st Meal2 nd MealTotal1 st Meal2 nd MealTotal 4.5 ± 0.5 Chow a 0.3 ± 0.1 Chow a 4.8 ± 0.5 Chow+Chow a,○ 3.6 ± 0.5 Chow a 0.4 ± 0.2 Chow a 4.0 ± 0.5 Chow+Chow a,○ 4.3 ± 0.5 Chow a 3.3 ± 0.5 FL b 7.7 ± 1.0 Chow+FL b,■ 3.3 ± 0.4 Chow a, 5.6 ± 0.6 FL b,* 8.8 ± 0.8 Chow+FL b,■ 6.5 ± 0.6 FL b 0.7 ± 0.2 Chow a 7.2 ± 0.7 FL+Chow b,■ 7.5 ± 0.7 FL b 0.3 ± 0.1 Chow a 7.8 ± 0.8 FL+Chow b,■ No Daily Access to FLLimited Daily Access to FL Different letters (a, b) in columns indicate significant differences in values. Different symbols (■, ○) in rows indicate significant differences in total intake in grams. * = significantly different from FL intake in 2 nd Meal in “No Daily Access to FL” group. Row 1: Rats consume very little additional chow as a second meal when satiated on chow in the first meal. This is expected based on sensory specific satiety. Furthermore, limited access to FL does not affect chow intake. Row 2: As expected based on sensory specific satiety, rats satiated on rat chow in the first meal consume a more palatable food (Froot Loops) offered as a second meal. Rats with limited daily access to FL did eat significantly more FL in a second meal (5.6 ± 0.6 g) than did rats without daily access to FL (3.3 ± 0.5 g). However, the total intake in grams for chow and FL was not affected by limiting the daily access to FL. Row 3: Rats consume large quantities of FL in a first meal and very little chow as a second meal (as predicted by sensory specific satiety.) However, limited daily access to FL did not affect the intake of FL in the first meal nor did it affect total intake for both meals. Columns 3 and 6: Total food intake for both meals was greater if FL were offered as either the first or second meal. However, rats that were given limited daily access to a highly palatable food ate no more than animals that were not given FL access daily. Because the energy density of RC (3.3 cal/g) and FL (3.8 cal/g) is similar, when intake is expressed as calories (data not shown) the results follow the same pattern. The only effect of limited daily access to a highly palatable food was a significant increase in the intake of that food offered as a second meal. Although total food intake was greater if FL were offered as either the first or second meal, overall food intake for a two meal sequence was not affected by limited daily access to FL. Thus, these data do not support the hypothesis that limited daily access to a highly palatable food triggers binge eating. Others have reported episodes of binge eating in laboratory rats for which access to a highly palatable food (100% fat) is limited to three days per week (Corwin, 2004) or limited access to high fat/high sugar foods is combined with stress (Boggiano et al., 2007.) In those studies, daily ad libitum access for 2-4 hours to a highly palatable food did not result in binge eating. Our rats receiving daily access to FL were provided only ~1.1 g of FL. It appears that this very limited access to a highly palatable food does not induce craving for that food that would be manifest as an elevated intake of that food when available ad libitum on experimental days. Thus, the conditions of limited daily access to FL in our protocol did not trigger binge eating. It would be interesting to determine in future experiments whether further limiting FL access to only three days per week would trigger episodes of binge eating. 1 st meal of Chow Chow FL Chow Chow FL 2 nd meal to be Chow FL Chow Chow FL Chow