The role of perseverative thinking processes in paranoia Dr. Kate Cavanagh University of Sussex, UK

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Introductory Training Behavioral Therapy Behavioral Therapy helps you weaken the connections between troublesome situations and your habitual reactions.
Advertisements

A transdiagnostic model
Mood Disorders I (Chapter 7) March 7, 2014 PSYC 2340: Abnormal Psychology Brett Deacon, Ph.D.
Psyco 350 Lec #22– Slide 1 Lecture 22 – Psyco 350, B1 Winter, 2011 N. R. Brown.
Formulation and Intervention Kate Hardy, Clin.Psych.D Post Doctoral Fellow Prodromal Assessment, Research and Treatment Team (PART),
AFFECTIVE FACTORS IMPACTING ON ACADEMIC FUNCTIONING Student Development Services: Faculty of Commerce.
Michael Knepp, M.S., Chad Stephens, B.S. & Dr. Bruce Friedman, PhD INTRODUCTION METHODOLOGY One component for diagnosis of generalized anxiety disorder.
So is violence bad for me? Discuss the effects of short-term and long ‑ term exposure to violence.
Chapter 14 Psychological Disorders. Psychopathology.
Aversive intrusive thoughts as contributors to inflated responsibility, intolerance of uncertainty, and thought- action fusion Graham C L Davey, Frances.
Interparental Conflict & Children’s Internalizing Psychopathology: Examining the Role of Children’s Appraisals & Emotions Jennifer K. Hauser & John H.
TYPES OF MENTAL ILLNESS. OVERVIEW DEPRESSION ANXIETY SUBSTANCE ABUSE.
Copyright © 2005 Mosby, Inc. All rights reserved. Slide 0.
TEMPLATE DESIGN © s.com Spirituality and Experiential Avoidance in Social Anxiety Benjamin Ramos, Elizabeth Mejia-Muñoz, Michael.
MENTAL HEALTH Understanding Mental Illness. Defining Mental Illness Clinical definition Clinically significant behavioral problems Clinically significant.
Metacognitive Behaviour Therapy. Teaching aims: Introduction to main principles underpinning Meta cognitive therapy. Be able to identify what distinguishes.
Psychology 100:12 Chapter 13 Disorders of Mind and Body.
Paul Salkovskis Institute of Psychiatry Centre for Anxiety Disorders and Trauma, Maudsley Hospital What is toilet phobia? History,
Cherie Levy Dianne A Vella-Brodrick School of Psychology, Psychiatry and Psychological Medicine, Monash University.
A Trauma-Informed Approach to Diagnosing Children in Foster Care Gene Griffin, J.D., Ph.D.Northwestern University Medical SchoolAugust 28, 2012.
CHAPTER 3 NOTES Mental health – the state of mental well-being in which one can cope with the demands of daily life.
RESILIENCE RESOURCES CONTRIBUTE TO BETTER HEALTH OUTCOMES AMONG RHEUMATOIC ARTHRITIS PATIENTS Kate E. Murray, B.A., Brendt P. Parrish, B.S., Mary C. Davis,
POSTER TEMPLATE BY: om Sex Differences in Associations between Fear of Negative Evaluation (FNE) and Substance Use Lesley A.
Specifying autobiographical information alters emotion activation, but not the way you think it does… Pierre Philippot Université de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve,
Categories of Mental Disorders 1 Child and youth mental health problems can be classified into two broad categories: 1Internalizing problems  withdrawal.
Lab 8 Anxiety Disorders. DSM IV Criteria Generalized Anxiety Disorder A) Excessive anxiety & worry (apprehensive expectation) occuring more days than.
Mental Health By: Mr. Lopez and Mr. Guzzarde. Video Clip Jonah Mowry’s Story.
General Anxiety Disorder (GAD) Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) is an anxiety disorder that is characterized by excessive, uncontrollable and often.
Suicide Risk Assessment. Thoughts, myths, questions about suicide 1.Is suicide a form of manipulation? 2.Will asking about suicide lead to suicidality?
Abnormal Psychology (Chapter 16) Lecture Outline : “Abnormality” and diagnosis Anxiety Disorders (video 92 case)
Introduction to Psychology Mood Disorders November 28, 2011 Mood Disorders November 28, 2011.
ANXIETY DISORDERS Anxiety vs. Fear  anxiety: (future oriented) negative affect, bodily tension, and apprehension about the future  fear: (reaction.
Illness Cognitions, Cognitive Fusion, Avoidance and Self-Compassion as Predictors of Distress and Quality of Life in a Heterogeneous Sample of Adults,
APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY LABORATORY East Tennessee State University Johnson City, Tennessee INTRODUCTION CONTACT:
MMPI-2 Restructured Clinical Scales (RC) Scales William P. Wattles Francis Marion University.
Continuing and Distance Education Introductory Psychology 1023 Lecture 6: Abnormal Psychology Reading: Chapter 14.
Copyright © 2007, 2003 by Mosby, Inc., an affiliate of Elsevier Inc. Chapter 36 Mental Health Problems.
Abnormal Psychology. Medical Studentitis: Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2007 A form of “hypochondriasis” can occur when learning about abnormal psychology.
Chapter 10: Depressive Disorders in Adolescents Megan Jeffreys V. Robin Weersing.
Stress Responses in Children with Chronic Pain and Anxiety Madeleine Jackson, Lynette Dufton, M.S., & Bruce E. Compas, Ph.D. Introduction Method Conclusions.
Review outline in notes
1. Abnormal Behavior * A psychological disorder, causing distress, disability, or dysfunction. Defined symptomatically by the DSM. 2.
Gender differences in symptom reporting: the influence of psychological traits. Laura Goodwin Dr Stephen Fairclough Liverpool John Moores University BACKGROUND.
Chapter 5 Anxiety Disorders. Copyright © 2011 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All rights reserved. Chapter 5 2 Fear: Fight-or-Flight Response.
Chapter 16 Abnormal Psychology “To study the abnormal is the best way of understand the normal.” -William James.
1 Mental Disorders EQ: How can having a mental disorder harm family relationships?
User-Defined Placeholder Text Mental Illness Abnormal Psychology.
LO: To be able to describe and evaluate the Cognitive Treatment for Schizophrenia.
Mental Illness Depression Bipolar Disorder Schizophrenia Anxiety Disorders Eating Disorders Personality Disorders.
Cluster DescriptionMust Score 0 Variance. Despite careful consideration of all the other clusters, this group of service users are not adequately described.
Dr Nelson Clarke 24 th April 2012 Distress vs. Disorder Mental Health and Psycho-social support in Disasters in the Caribbean.
Cognitive Behaviour Therapy (CBT) For Anxiety And Depression.
A Cognitive Behavioral Approach to Social Phobia Allison Brayton Dr. Brett Deacon University of Wyoming.
1. MHFA (Wales) Session 4 (3 hours) What are psychotic disorders (schizophrenia, bipolar disorder)? Symptoms of psychotic disorders. Risk factors for.
Researcher: Witold Orlik
PSY 436 Instructor: Emily Bullock Yowell, Ph.D.
What is Abnormal…anyway?
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy Workshop
Morgan Sissons Victoria University of Wellington
Blessing Marandure, DeMontfort University
Evaluation of Ethiology
Psychological Disorders
Trauma- Stress Related Disorders
A transdiagnostic treatment targeting Intolerance of Uncertainty.
Team 1 $1,000,000 $500,000 $250,000 A key feature of _______ schizophrenia is an odd motor state in which the individual may remain frozen in a stupor.
Clinical Psychology.
and mental health? Study Lead: Katerina Panagaki
Biological Social Learning Cognitive Psychodynamic
The Role of Rumination in Depression
Biological Social Learning Cognitive Psychodynamic
Presentation transcript:

The role of perseverative thinking processes in paranoia Dr. Kate Cavanagh University of Sussex, UK

It’s not what you think it’s the way that you think it

In cognitive psychopathology research focuses on both the content of automatic thoughts, attributions and beliefs But also on the way we respond to these thoughts (cognitive or ‘thinking’ processes) and the implications of this for further thoughts, feelings and behaviour – For example: if the idea ‘I look foolish’ crosses my mind – how I respond to that thought (focus on it, evaluate it, dismiss it or distract from it, argue with it etc) will have an effect on future thought content, how I feel and my behaviour over time.

Outline Repetitive thought and repetitive negative thinking Role of repetitive negative thinking processes in psychopathology Hypothesised role of repetitive negative thinking processes in paranoia Research programme exploring the role of rumination in paranoia – Questionnaire based study (correlation) – Lab based studies testing the causal relationship between rumination and the maintenance of suspicious and mistrustful beliefs in student sample Conclusions, questions and future directions

Colleagues and collaborators Newcastle University, UK: – Rob Dudley, Jennifer Simpson, Bryony McGregor University of Sussex, UK: – Cheontell Barnes, Stacey Hemmings, Deidre Robertson, Cristina Martinelli and Moitree Banerjee.

Repetitive thought The “process of thinking attentively, repetitively or frequently about one’s self and one’s world” – Segerstrom et al. (2003, p.909) e.g. planning, reflection, emotional processing, problem solving, working through, worry, rumination, post-event processing

Repetitive thought May be ‘adaptive’ or ‘maladaptive’ Vary on three dimensions (Watkins, 2008) – Valence of the thought content (positive vs negative – Intrapersonal and situational context (positive vs negative) – Level of construal adopted (abstract vs concrete) + – Level of volition/control (Ehring & Watkins, 2008)

Repetitive thought With ‘constructive’ or ‘unconstructive’ consequences for mental and physical health (Watkins, 2008) Constructive consequencesUnconstructive consequences e.g. recovery from upsetting and traumatic events, adaptive preparation and planning, recovery from depression, uptake of health promoting behaviours e.g. Depression, anxiety, difficulties in physical health

Repetitive negative thought Transdiagnostic process (Harvey et al., 2004) “same process across disorders, which is applied to disorder specific content’ (p. 192; Ehring & Watkins, 2008 Repetitive Passive / Relatively uncontrollable Focused on negative content

Repetitive negative thought Growing evidence of repetitive negative thinking processes in various types of psychopathology

Worry – repetitive thought about “future potential threat, catastrophes, uncertainty and risks” (Watkins, 2008, p. 164) – Cardinal feature of GAD (DSM-IV APA, 1994), – Also associated with OCD, PTSD, depression, insomnia, bipolar disorder (and psychosis)

Rumination – A “mode of responding to distress that involves repetitively and passively focusing on symptoms of distress and the possible causes and consequences of these symptoms” (Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco & Lyubomirsjy, 2008, p. 400) Cardinal feature of depression (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2008) – but also evident in social phobia (Brozovich & Heimberg, 2008), PTSD, GAD and panic (Smith & Alloy, 2008) and associated with binge eating and alcohol abuse (Nolen- Hoeksema et al., 2008) and negative symptoms of psychosis (emotional withdrawal, stereotyped thinking, Halari et al., 2009).

Repetitive negative thought Not just ‘associated’ to a range of disorders But playing a causal role

The role of rumination in depression Evidence of higher levels of rumination in people with depression (and predicts future episodes of depression) Causal role is supported by evidence of the effects of rumination induction in depressed individuals * (versus distraction), which leads to – worsened mood (Donaldson & Lam, 2004) – Increased retrieval of negative memories (Lyubomirsky, Caldwell & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1998) – Distorted interpretation of events and negative expectancies of the future (Lyubomirky & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1995) – Indesciveness (Randenborg et al., 2010) – Withdrawal and inactivity (Martell et al., 2001) All factors that may play a role in maintaining depression

Repetitive Negative Thinking Evidence of RNT in 13 disorders – Depression, PTSD, Social Anxiety Disorder, OCD, Insomnia, eating disorders, pain disorder, hypochondriasis, alcohol use disorder, psychosis and bipolar disorder – Including psychosis, where high levels of RNT, described as worry, have been measured in groups with persecutory delusions (Freeman & Garety, 1999; Startup, Freeman & Garety, 2007) and other presentations of psychosis (Morrison & Wells, 2007) Harvey et al., 2004; Erhing & Watkins, 2008

Perseverative cognition “The repeated or chronic activation of the cognitive representation of one or more psychological stressors” (Brosschot et al., 2006) Description of repetitive negative thinking adopted in research relating to stress-related physiological activation and its health consequences

Summary Significant evidence of a transdiagnostic role for repetitive negative thought (RNT) RNT associated with many Axis 1 disorders Evidence of a causal role for RNT in onset and maintenance of transdiagnostic distress Is RNT relevant to psychosis / paranoia and persecutory delusions?

Repetitive negative thinking processes in persecutory delusions, paranoia and psychosis ‘Preoccupation’ as a cardinal feature of delusional beliefs – e.g. Chadwick & Lowe (1990; belief conviction, belief preoccupation) – E.g. Peters et al. (1999; distress, preoccupation, conviction)

Repetitive thinking processes in psychosis underpins delusional distress “…further appraisal of the experience (e.g. worry and rumination)” Freeman (2007), p451

Repetative thought in psychosis Limited research available High levels of worry found in – Individuals with current persecutory delusions (Freeman & Garety, 1998; Startup, Freeman & Garety, 2008) – And, other presentations of psychoses (Morrison & Wells, 2007) Association between rumination and negative symptoms – A relationship between rumination and measures of emotional withdrawal and stereotyped thinking, in participants with diagnosis of schizophrenia (Harari et al., 2009)

Rumination and psychosis Idea of ‘rumination’ or ‘brooding’ as playing a role in paranoia has clinical face validity – Repetitive thinking about the meaning, causes and consequences of activated threat beliefs and anxious mood But, little research has explored the role of rumination in persecutory or paranoid beliefs

Does rumination play a role in paranoia? A questionnaire study investigating the relationship between rumination and paranoid ideation in a student sample * Two lab-based studies investigating the effect of rumination (versus distraction) on a personal threat belief – 1. autobiographical memory of suspicious or mistrustful event (within subjects design) – 2. in vivo exposure to a ‘paranoia prime’ (between subjects design)

Relevance of non-clinical studies There is a continuum of severity of paranoia in the general population (Os & Verdoux, 2003) – 15-20% regularly engage in paranoid thinking Non-clinical and clinical paranoia are associated with the same risk factors (Freeman, 2007) Presence of non-clinical paranoia increases likelyhood for a subsequent diagnosis of psychosis Non-clinical paranoia is of interest in its own right, but may also inform our understanding of clinical paranoia and persecutory delusions

Is rumination associated with paranoid ideation? Questionnaire Study Simpson, Dudley, McGregor & Cavanagh (under review) 133 student participants from Newcastle University, UK 40 males and 92 females (data missing for one participant) Mean age = 20 (SD=3.9) Ethnicity 91% white British, 7% white other, 2% Asian.

Measures – Fenigstein Paranoia Scale (Fenigstein & Vanable, 1992) ‘I’m sure I have been talked about behind my back’ ‘some people have tried to steal my ideas and take credit for them’ – Rumination Response Scale (Nolen-Hoeksema & Marrow, 1991) ‘think about your shortcomings, faults and mistakes’ ‘write down what you are thinking and analyse it’ – Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson, Clark & Tellegen, 1988) – (plus other measures not reported here)

Result of Pearson’s Correlation and Significance Values between variables (n=129) Paranoia Scale PANAS negative PANAS positive PANAS,negative 0.31** PANAS,positive Ruminative Response Scale 0.46** 0.52**-0.10 Key: PS=Paranoia Scale, PANAS=Positive and Negative Affect Scale, SBQ=Safety Behaviour Questionnaire, RRS=Rumination Response Scale. *p<0.05**p<0.01 Regression analysis indicated that rumination predicts significant additional variance in paranoia when negative mood is accounted for.

Correlational analysis supports the idea that rumination may be associated with paranoid ideation This effect is over and above the processes intrinsic to negative mood Next step, to test hypothesis that this is a causal relationship in the lab…

Does rumination on paranoid ideation increase conviction in paranoid ideas and feelings of paranoia? Simpson, McGregor, Dudley, Cavanagh (under review) 25 University Students – 22 females, mean age 20 (SD=3), 96% white Autobiographical memory procedure – “Bring to mind a time when you felt suspicious or mistrustful of another persons intentions…” – “focus on the events that made you feel suspicious or mistrustful” Within subjects design – With that memory held in mind think about…(counterbalanced) Rumination (e.g. ‘Why people treat you the way they do’?) Distraction (e.g. ‘the layout of your local supermarket aisles’) Based on Rusting & Nolen-Hoeksema (1998) Measures of procedure fidelity and adherence, conviction in threat beliefs, self-reported distress etc

Design Baseline Measures Paranoia Induction: Autobiographical memory Rumination / Distraction Tasks (counterbalanced) Outcome measures

Fidelity checks: memory procedure Prior to Rumination Task Prior to Distraction Task How clearly can you remember the incident? 73.1(21.0)76.8 (21.5) To what extent did you feel mistrustful or suspicious of the other person/people? 60.6 (19.3)67.3 (21.5) To what extent did you feel that you were at threat of physical harm or psychological harm 47.4 (28.3)49.0 (29.9) To what extent did you feel that the person(s) meant to cause harm? 34.9 (27.5)34.4 (27.7)

Thinking tasks Rumination induction – 15 rumination items and asked to spend 8 minutes thinking sequentially about the meaning of the items. – e.g. “Think about why people treat you the way they do” and “Think about how you react to other people Distraction induction – 15 externally-focused distraction items – e.g. “Think about the layout of the local post office” and “Think about a double-decker bus driving down the street” All items were taken from Rusting and Nolen-Hoeksema, (1998) rumination procedure. Matched for instructions and length of task

Adherence checks: thinking tasks Rumination TaskDistraction Task Percentage of time you managed to stay focused on the task 76.0 (12.1)75.3 (15.2) Extent you were able to follow the instructions of the task 87.2 (13.5)89.4 (10.6) Proportion of the time you spent thinking about yourself or other people? 87.4 (8.6)43.0 (31.1) Proportion of the time you spent trying to understand or make sense of things? 70.0 (19.5)45.8 (30.8)

Outcomes (nb. baseline = post-prime) Rumination vs distraction, p<0.05 Baseline vs distraction, p<0.05 Baseline vs rumination, ns Rumination vs distraction, p<0.05 Baseline vs distraction, p<0.05 Baseline vs rumination, ns

Conclusions Rumination appears to be associated with the maintenance of threat beliefs and paranoid feelings following recall of an autobiographical memory characterised by suspicion/mistrust Distraction appears to remediate threat beliefs and paranoid feelings Next step – does rumination have a similar effect on paranoid threat beliefs in vivo?

Martinelli & Cavanagh (in prep) 37 students – 30 females, mean age = 24.5 (SD = 3.9), In vivo paranoia induction – concern about potential harm from an other person Between subjects design – With these events held in mind think about… Rumination (e.g. ‘Why people treat you the way they do’?) Distraction (e.g. ‘the layout of your local supermarket aisles’) Based on Rusting & Nolen-Hoeksema (1998) Measures of state paranoia scale (adapted from Freeman et al), time on task, distress etc

Association between trait paranoia and RNT Trait paranoia and trait perseveration were highly correlated, r = 0.54, p< Supporting the idea that RNT and paranoia are related

Martinelli & Cavanagh (in prep) Baseline Measures Paranoia Induction: In vivo Rumination Outcome measures Distraction Procedure

Induction of paranoia Previous studies have used different protocols to initiate in situ paranoia Prevost et al (2010) – participants told there was a hidden goal to the study, their cerebral activity would be manipulated via electrical currents during the experiment, and were given the impression they were being observed from behind a one-way mirror (paranoia not measured) Ellet et al.(2008) exposure to deprived urban environment (increased paranoia) Ellett & Chadwick (2007) video camera observation during task (increased paranoia – but artifact of self-focus/social anxiety?)

Our paranoia induction procedure – Sit in a dark room for 5 minutes, facing a wall with back to door, after some time someone might come in and might interact with you in a mildly unpleasant way – e.g. pinch you, tickle you, say something unpleasant to you… – Demonstrated to elevate state paranoia in pilot studies.

Measuring state paranoia Freeman et al (2007) State Social Paranoia Scale – designed to capture participants responses to a ‘social’ situation “someone was hostile towards me” “someone had it in for me” “I felt safe…” “everyone was pleasant” etc Freeman et al have found that more than 1/3 rd people report paranoid thoughts even when the social environment is neutral (2008)

Was the paranoia induction effective? Suggests our induction procedure was effective in eliciting ‘paranoia’ in our participants

State paranoia scale No difference between groups after paranoia induction – t(35) = 0.6, ns Time x Condition interaction effect for thought process – F (1,35) = 7.1, p = 0.01 Post-test differences Distraction v Rumination – t(35) = 2.3, p = 0.03 No differences between groups in following instructions, concentration, time on task etc

Findings of Martinelli & Cavanagh study Paranoia induction appeared to raise state paranoia (needs validation/replication) Rumination on this ‘dark-room’ experience was associated with maintenance of feelings of paranoia Distraction was associated with reductions in feelings of paranoia Baseline paranoia was associated with baseline perseverative style, and response to the paranoia induction. Other methodological factors did not account for the effects of rumination on final level of paranoid ideation

Conclusions Rumination appears to be associated with maintenance (elevation?) of threat beliefs and feelings of paranoia associated with exposure to ambiguous/threatening interpersonal situations Distraction appears to remediate paranoid ideas and mood (at least in the short term) This replicates and extends a significant body of research supporting the transdiagnostic role of repetitive negative thinking processes Limitations to these studies in terms of small sample sizes, use of non-clinical examples of suspicious threat beliefs etc

Questions Does this have any implications for our understanding of clinical paranoia/persecutory delusions? How can we reconcile evidence of both repetitive negative thinking and ‘jumping to conclusions’ reasoning biases in relation to paranoia and persecutory delusions? How might we extend our knowledge of paranoia processes in non-clinical samples? How similar are these measured effects to those found in ‘post-event processessing’ in social anxiety? Other questions….

Future research should explore… Bio-psycho-social mechanisms underlying the relationship between rumination and suspicious and mistrustful thinking in non-clinical populations The role of repetitive thought in paranoid ideation in clinical populations The impact of keeping rumination and other types of perseveration in mind when working therapeutically with suspicious and mistrustful thinking and paranoia in clinical settings – E.g. can RNT therapies offer benefit in this domain?

Thank-you Kate Cavanagh DPhil DClinPsych Senior Lecturer in Clinical Psychology Director of the Interactive Wellbeing Lab University of Sussex, UK.