Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Scaling up our instruction Our prior work demonstrates an effective method for teaching the Control of Variables Strategy (CVS), a means of designing simple.

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Scaling up our instruction Our prior work demonstrates an effective method for teaching the Control of Variables Strategy (CVS), a means of designing simple."— Presentation transcript:

1 Scaling up our instruction Our prior work demonstrates an effective method for teaching the Control of Variables Strategy (CVS), a means of designing simple experiments, for students as young as third grade (Strand Cary & Klahr, to appear in special issue of Cognitive Development), Though promising, this brief, one-size-fits-all instruction has some disadvantages: It does not reach all students. Particularly students in low-income schools with large minority populations require explicit scaffolding, repetition, and instruction in multiple domains to master the procedures and concepts of CVS (Li, Klahr, & Siler, 2006). Such adaptations are labor intensive and still fail to reach some students. Many students have beliefs and misconceptions that are very resistant to change. Even when they seem to thoroughly understand CVS, some fail to transfer to new domains or question types. Thus we are in the process of creating the TED computer tutor (for Training in Experimental Design) to provide individualized instruction. Working toward robust knowledge of experimental design: Development of the TED Tutor Mari Strand Cary, Stephanie Siler, Cressida Magaro & David Klahr Carnegie Mellon University A need for instruction Experimental design and inference are critical skills for scientific thinking. Teachers emphasize this by asking students to complete science fair projects and standardized tests include related items. Although the ability to infer causal mechanisms is present to some degree in infancy (Schultz & Sommerville, 2006), the ability to design experiments from which to draw unambiguous causal inferences is lacking for many middle school children (Chen & Klahr, 1999; Klahr & Nigam, 2004). Most students do not attain experimental design and inference skills on their own or in their K-8th grade classes (see Klahr & Li, 2005 for review) Prior research (Strand Cary & Klahr, to appear in special issue of Cognitive Development; Chen & Klahr, 1999) has resulted in a promising technique for teaching experimental design skills. However, some students are left behind. This research was supported by the Department of Education, IES (# R305H060034) Questions? Comments? Please contact MariStrandCary@cmu.eduMariStrandCary@cmu.edu CDS 2007 Training in Experimental Design (TED) Tutor What is TED? An experimental design computer tutor used individually by 4th-8th grade students in full-class setting Used before, during, or after teacher-instruction Individualized, adaptive instruction about simple experimental design, evaluation, and interpretation. Efficient instruction that will be effective for all students Multifaceted instruction leading to long-lasting, generalized knowledge demonstrable in new domains and question formats How will it help all students learn? Instruction, practice, and assessment will be based on individuals’ knowledge and mastery in real time. Ongoing recording and analysis of student actions and formative assessments will enable tutor to do “knowledge tracing” and “model tracing” and adapt to the current “student model” (Anderson, Boyle, Corbett & Lewis, 1990; Corbett & Anderson, 1995) while selecting instruction. Teachers can use student performance information to decide which students to spend more time with or to guide additional full-class instruction (e.g., if common errors arise). How will the tutor adapt to each student? Build on student strengths and current knowledge Focus on student’s weak or missing knowledge Provide “just-in-time” and “just enough” feedback Ask a range of question types Balance domain specific and domain general instruction Adjust pacing and coverage “Fade” scaffolding Introduce new domains (to renew motivation, test understanding, and promote generalization) Require forward and backward reasoning (e.g., design an experiment to answer a specific question; determine the experimental question by looking at a well-designed experiment) How do students incorrectly design simple experiments? VariableRamp 1Ramp 2 SlopeSteepNot steep BallRedYellow SurfaceSmoothRough Starting positionTopMiddle VariableGroup 1Group 2 Studying locationDesk LightingWell litDim Noise levelLoudQuiet Does WHERE YOU STUDY affect your grades ? What is behind these errors? Guessing Carelessness Different goals Different or incorrect experimental logic Misuse of visual representations Completely confounded experiment Two identical set-ups Hold the target variable constant and vary the other variables Partially-confounded experiment Choose combinations of variables such that each set- up is optimally designed. Unconfounded experiment for wrong target variable Some fourth-grade students were doing a project for their science class. They were trying to find the answer to the question “Do beetles choose to live in bright light or in the shade?” The picture below shows how a student set up the experiment to find out if beetles choose to live in bright light or in the shade. Is this a good way to set up the experiment? Why or why not ? For example, identifying and fixing problematic experiments (as often required by confusing standardized test items like the one on the right) seems to be more difficult than designing experiments from scratch. Does the SURFACE affect how far balls roll? To find out whether seeds grow better in the light or dark, you could put some seeds on pieces of damp paper and A.keep them in a warm, dark place B.keep one group in a light place and another in a dark place C.keep them in a warm, light place D.put them in a light or dark place that is cool TED’s Iterative Design Process Version n Pilot testing and Classroom implementation (+ pre, post, and formative assessments) Human tutoring (+ delayed post assessment) Revise model of student thinking, instruction, and interface Teaching CVS with ramps (Screen shots taken from current version which involves small-group instruction by researchers) Ramp familiarizationReviewing CVSSeeking reminder during experiment designExplaining the visual representations Good / Bad References Anderson, J.R., Boyle, C.F., Corbett, A.T. & Lewis, M.W. (1990). Cognitive modeling and intelligent tutoring. Artificial Intelligence, 42 Corbett, A.T. & Anderson, J.R. (1995). Knowledge tracing: Modeling the acquisition of procedural knowledge. User modeling and user- adapted Interaction Chen, Z. & Klahr, D. (1999). All other things being equal: Children’s acquisition of the control of variables strategy. Child Development, 70(5), Klahr, D. & Li, J. (2005). Cognitive research and elementary science instruction: From the laboratory, to the classroom, and back. Journal of science and educational technology, 4. Klahr, D. & Nigam, M (2004). The equivalence of learning paths in early science instruction: Effects of direct instruction and discovery learning. Psychological science, 15(10) Li, J., Klahr, D. & Siler, S. (2006). What Lies Beneath the Science Achievement Gap? The Challenges of Aligning Science Instruction with Standards and Tests. Science Educator, 15, 1-12 Schulz, L. & Sommerville, J. (2006). God does not play dice: Causal determinism and preschoolers’ causal inferences. Child Development, 77(2) Strand Cary, M. & Klahr, D. (to appear in special issue of Cognitive Development)


Download ppt "Scaling up our instruction Our prior work demonstrates an effective method for teaching the Control of Variables Strategy (CVS), a means of designing simple."

Similar presentations


Ads by Google