Embedded Clauses in TAG

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Grammar Formalisms Spring Term 2004
Advertisements

Verb and Verb Phrase (V)
Lecture 4: The Complementiser System
Lexical Functional Grammar : Grammar Formalisms Spring Term 2004.
NP Movement Passives, Raising: When NPs are not in their theta positions.
Unit 5 Finite Verbs or Verb Phrases. What is a verb phrase? A verb phrase is a phrase whose nucleus is a verb which can be a finite V/VP or a non-finite.
Syntactic analysis using Context Free Grammars. Analysis of language Morphological analysis – Chairs, Part Of Speech (POS) tagging – The/DT man/NN left/VBD.
Dr. Abdullah S. Al-Dobaian1 Ch. 2: Phrase Structure Syntactic Structure (basic concepts) Syntactic Structure (basic concepts)  A tree diagram marks constituents.
Finite Clauses.
Grammatical Relations and Lexical Functional Grammar Grammar Formalisms Spring Term 2004.
Statistical NLP: Lecture 3
Complement Structures: Equi and Raising HPSG WS 2007/08 Janina Kopp
Lecture 6: Verbs with Clausal Arguments
Installment 10b. Raising, etc CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
Week 8. Midterm debrief CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Midterm results Mean: 88 Mean: 88 Median: 93 Median: 93 A A- B+ B B-
Syntax Lecture 3: The Subject. The Basic Structure of the Clause Recall that our theory of structure says that all structures follow this pattern: It.
Week 14b. PRO and control CAS LX 522 Syntax I. It is likely… This satisfies the EPP in both clauses. The main clause has Mary in SpecIP. The embedded.
Week 6a. Case and checking (with a little more  -Theory) CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
The students will be able to know:
AuxiliariesAuxiliaries. Auxiliaries A verb used to add a functional or grammatical meaning to a clause in which it appears. Functions in a supporting.
Clauses and Moods by Prashanth Kamle
Context Free Grammars Reading: Chap 12-13, Jurafsky & Martin This slide set was adapted from J. Martin, U. Colorado Instructor: Paul Tarau, based on Rada.
1 LIN 1310B Introduction to Linguistics Prof: Nikolay Slavkov TA: Qinghua Tang CLASS 14, Feb 27, 2007.
Lecture 19 Passive Voice Objectives: 2. Passive voice
1.Syntax: the rules of sentence formation; the component of the mental grammar that represent speakers’ knowledge of the structure of phrase and sentence.
ASPECTS OF LINGUISTIC COMPETENCE 4 SEPT 09, 2013 – DAY 6 Brain & Language LING NSCI Harry Howard Tulane University.
IV. SYNTAX. 1.1 What is syntax? Syntax is the study of how sentences are structured, or in other words, it tries to state what words can be combined with.
Finite and nonfinite clauses: function subordinate clause finitethatsubjunctive non- subjunctive whsubjunctive non- subjunctive othersubjunctive non- subjunctive.
The verb of a sentence expresses an action or simply states a fact. Verbs that simply state a fact are often called state of being verbs or verbs of existence.
More Syntax Introduction to the Study of Syntax 2: Advanced Phrase Structure, Recursion, and Complex Clauses tt.
October 15, 2007 Non-finite clauses and control : Grammars and Lexicons Lori Levin.
Bare phrase structure Null subjects Null auxiliaries Sept. 17, 2010 – Day 9 Introduction to Syntax ANTH 3590/7590 Harry Howard Tulane University.
Context Free Grammars Reading: Chap 9, Jurafsky & Martin This slide set was adapted from J. Martin, U. Colorado Instructor: Rada Mihalcea.
Clause Types A descriptive tangent into the types of clauses Note: much of this discussion is based on Radford, Andrew (1989) Transformational Grammar.
Rules, Movement, Ambiguity
Linguistic Theory Lecture 5 Filters. The Structure of the Grammar 1960s (Standard Theory) LexiconPhrase Structure Rules Deep Structure Transformations.
1 LIN 1310B Introduction to Linguistics Prof: Nikolay Slavkov TA: Qinghua Tang CLASS 16, March 6, 2007.
Syntax I Checklist Grammars and Lexicons Fall Term 2004.
 Chapter 8 (Part 2) Transformations Transformational Grammar Engl 424 Hayfa Alhomaid.
Syntax Lecture 6: Missing Subjects of Non-finite Clauses.
Verb phrases Main reference: Randolph Quirk and Sidney Greenbaum, A University Grammar of English, Longman: London, (3.23 – 3.55)
1 Some English Constructions Transformational Framework October 2, 2012 Lecture 7.
Finite Clauses. Types of Sentences Simple Compound Complex Compound Complex.
English 10 From Writer’s Inc. & Mrs. Eberts
Basic Syntactic Structures of English CSCI-GA.2590 – Lecture 2B Ralph Grishman NYU.
3.3 A More Detailed Look At Transformations Inversion (revised): Move Infl to C. Do Insertion: Insert interrogative do into an empty.
Objectives: 1.A classification of verbs 2. Transitive verbs, intransitive verbs and linking verbs 3. Dynamic verbs and stative verbs 4. Finite and non-finite.
September 26, : Grammars and Lexicons Lori Levin.
X-Bar Theory. The part of the grammar regulating the structure of phrases has come to be known as X'-theory (X’-bar theory'). X-bar theory brings out.
Expanding verb phrases
Week 3. Clauses and Trees English Syntax. Trees and constituency A sentence has a hierarchical structure Constituents can have constituents of their own.
Week 12. NP movement Text 9.2 & 9.3 English Syntax.
Week 10 X-bar syntax: More on Clauses English Syntax.
Natural Language Processing Vasile Rus
Embedded Clauses in TAG
Week 10 X-bar syntax: More on Clauses
Lecture 3: Functional Phrases
Statistical NLP: Lecture 3
Lexical Functional Grammar
Behavioral Properties of Subjects: matrix coding as subject
Lecture 7: Missing Subjects of Non-finite Clauses
Part I: Basics and Constituency
HAVE as VERB & HELPING VERB
Verbs.
Finite Clauses.
Lecture 8: Verb Positions
TREE ADJOINING GRAMMAR
ENG 3306 Raising and Control I.
Presentation transcript:

Embedded Clauses in TAG

Embedded Clauses Matrix Clause Embedded Clause COMP NP VP We think that they have left. COMP NP VP S V S-bar NP VP Embedded Clause

Linguistic Background Constraints Semantic roles

Embedded Clauses: Constraints Main verbs are subcategorized for The complementizer (that, for, to, etc.) Non-finite for-to We hoped for there to be no trouble. A word at the beginning of a subordinate clause that identifies it as a complement The morphology of the embedded verb Finite: present or past tense Non-finite: infinitive, present participle, past participle

Examples: Constraints imposed by the main verb on the embedded verb “Say” requires a finite embedded clause: Sam said that Sue saw him. *Sam said Sue to see him. *Sam said that Sue seeing him. “that” is a complementizer that goes with finite clauses. When it comes after a verb, it is optional: Sam said Sue saw him. That he left is a problem. *He left is a problem. “That” is only optional after a verb.

Examples: Constraints imposed by the main verb on the embedded verb “Expect” takes a finite clause or an infinitive, but not a participle: We expect to see him. We expect that we will see him. Modal auxiliary verbs (will, would, may, might, can, could, shall, should, etc) are always finite. *We expect seeing him. Might sound grammatical because “seeing him” can be a noun phrase, and “expect” can occur with a noun phrase: “We expect problems” *We expect seen him.

Finite embedded clauses I believe (that) it is snowing. Say, think, scream Finite with dummy subject It seems that they have left. Finite embedded question I wondered/asked whether/if it was snowing. Finite plus object We told them that it was snowing. Finite plus PP We said to them that it was snowing.

Non-finite embedded clauses Non-finite for-to We hoped for there to be no trouble. Non-finite: Raising to subject They seem (to us) to have left. Appear, continue Non-finite: Subject Equi They tried to leave. Intend, expect, plan, hope Non-finite: Raising to object We believe them to have left. consider Non-finite: Object Equi We persuaded them to leave. Convince, order, force, signaled Non-finite: promise We promised them to leave.

English Auxiliary Verbs Modal verbs: (will, would, can, could, shall, should, may, might, and a few others) Invariant: don’t have a third person singular form. Only occur where you can have present or past tense. Don’t occur in infinitives, gerunds, or participles: I will go. I would go. I said I would go. *I want to can go. Compare: I want to be able to go. *Canning go would make me happy. Compare: Being able to go would make me happy. The next verb must be an infinitive without “to”. I will have gone. I will be going. *I will going/gone/went/goes.

English Auxiliary Verbs “Have” Must be followed by a past participle: I have gone. *I have going/went/goes/go. Progressive “be” Must be followed by a present participle: I am going. *I am goes/went/go. Passive “be” Must be followed by a passive verb: The cookies were devoured. *The cookies were devouring/devours/devour.

Auxiliary verbs as main verbs (for syntax; not for semantics) The auxiliary verb can impose constraints on the main verb. Sam is sleeping/*slept/*sleeps. The main clause has to be finite (has a tense). Sam sleeps/slept. *Sam to sleep. *Sam sleeping. The auxiliary verb carries the tense, not the main verb: Sam is sleeping. *Sam be sleeps.

S Sam is sleeping NP VP V VP S Sam has slept NP VP V VP

Summary of constraints on embedded clauses The main verb determines the tense and morphology of the embedded verb. More than one embedded clause: Each verb determines the tense and morphology of the next one: I think that Sam tried to sleep. “Think” requires “try” to be finite. “Try” requires “sleep” to be infinitive.

The car needs washed. In most dialects of English, “need” takes an infinitive as a complement: The car needs to be washed. Sam needs to sleep. There are a few verbs that take passive participles as complements: We had them arrested by the police. We got them arrested by the police. They were arrested by the police. They got arrested by the police. In Pittsburgh, “need” and “want” can take passive participles as complements: The car needs washed. Do you want pushed?

Semantic Roles Syntax Word order Constituent structure Constraints: agreement, subcategorization, case marking Semantic roles: Sue interviewed Sam. Sue is the interviewer. Sam is the interviewee.

Semantic Roles in Embedded Clauses Sam tried to sleep. Sam is the agent of “try” Sam is the agent of “sleep” “Sam to sleep” is what was tried. Sam seemed to sleep. Sam is the agent of “sleep.” Sam is not an argument of “seem.” “Sam to sleep” is the only argument of “seem”.

Just the facts How many semantic arguments does each verb take: “Try” takes two. “Seem” takes one. Do the main clause and the embedded clause share a subject? Yes. Both “seem” and “try” share their subjects with the embedded verb.

How we know that the semantic role assignments are different with Seem and Try The cat seems to be out of the bag. There seems to be a problem. That seems to be my husband. The doctor seemed to examine Sam. Sam seemed to be examined by the doctor. The cat tried to be out of the bag. *There tried to be a problem. That tried to be my husband. The doctor tried to examine Sam. Sam tried to be examined by the doctor.

Raising to subject S COMP NP VP COMP It seems that they have left. V S-bar NP VP They seem to have left. COMP VP V VP-bar NP VP S

Two ways to represent that “seem” and “leave” share a subject. NP VP V VP-bar Subj they Verb seem Complement subj verb leave VP COMP They seem to have left. S NP VP V S NP VP They seem e to have left.

Comparison Second method: First method: Allow empty strings as terminal nodes in the tree. An empty string needs to take the place of the missing subject of the lower clause. The empty string is linked to the subject of the main clause to show that the main and embedded clauses share a subject. The tree represents: word order, constituent structure, grammatical relations, semantic roles. First method: No empty strings in the tree. The tree represents only word order and constituent structure. Grammatical relations and semantic roles are represented in a separate structure. Structure sharing in the representation of grammatical relations shows that the two verbs share a subject. Is one method simpler than the other? No. Both methods have to represent word order, semantic relations, grammatical relations, and semantic roles. People who argue that one is simpler are usually wrong – they don’t know how to count steps in a derivation.

Two ways to represent that “try” and “leave” share a subject. NP VP V VP-bar Subj they Verb seem Complement subj verb leave VP COMP They try to leave. S PRO is an empty string, but not the same kind of empty string as e  Coindexing indicates that PRO refers to “they”. NP VP V S NP VP They(i) try PRO(i) to leave.

“Seem” type verbs in TAG VP S John to be happy V AP NP VP Adjunction site V VP seem Auxiliary Tree Initial Tree These trees represent the number of arguments for each verb: “Seem” has one argument, represented as a VP. “To be happy” has one argument, “John”.

V VP VP seem S Adjunction site NP VP VP V AP to be happy John

S NP John V VP VP seems S NP VP John to be happy V AP V VP VP seem VP Adjunction VP to be happy V AP This tree shows word order and constituent structure. It also shows that “John” is the subject of “seem.” It doesn’t show that “John” is the subject of “to be happy.”

“Try” type verbs in TAG S S NP VP NP VP TO VP V PRO leave John tried Adjunction site S NP VP TO VP PRO leave S V NP VP John tried Initial Tree Auxiliary Tree These trees show the number of arguments for each verb: “Try” has two arguments. “Leave” has one argument.

S V NP VP John tried S Adjunction site S NP VP TO VP PRO leave

S V NP VP John tried S Adjunction site S NP VP TO VP PRO leave

S NP VP V S NP VP John tried TO VP PRO leave Adjunction is only allowed at the top S node so as not to mess up compositional semantics: After you put together “try to leave” you don’t want to have to take it apart again by inserting another verb like “expected” as in: John tried to expect to leave. Inserting “seem” into the middle of the tree doesn’t require you to disassemble any of the semantic pieces that were already assembled? S V NP VP John tried S NP VP TO VP PRO leave