Week 2b. Categories and features CAS LX 522 Syntax I.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Syntax Lecture 2: Categories and Subcategorisation.
Advertisements

Morphology.
CAS LX 522 Syntax I Week 2a. Categories.
Nouns and Countability. Outline Introduction Conceptual Semantics Chierchia’s approach Natural Semantic Metalanguage Conclusion.
Morphology Nuha Alwadaani.
Morphology Chapter 7 Prepared by Alaa Al Mohammadi.
Week 1b. Morphosyntactic features ch CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
Types of Phrases.
Lecture -3 Week 3 Introduction to Linguistics – Level-5 MORPHOLOGY
Episode 5a. TP CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Modals Pat might eat lunch. Pat might eat lunch. Modals: might, may, can, could, shall, should, will, would,
Week 3a. Categories, features, natural classes, and morphology. CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
Episode 8a. Passives and remaining issues CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
Week 3b. Just a little bit more phology. CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
Week 2a. Morphosyntactic features, part II. Ch. 2, 4.2- CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
Installment 12a. Commentary, and the beginning of wh-movement ( ) CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
PARTS OF SPEECH 1 The principles of the traditional classification of the English vocabulary 2 Notional and functional parts of speech. 3 The field structure.
S.T.E.P. (Structured Tutoring for English Placement)
Unit One: Parts of Speech
HELPING VERBS (Aka auxiliary verbs).
Week 1b. Morphosyntactic features ch CAS LX 522 Syntax I.
THE PARTS OF SYNTAX Don’t worry, it’s just a phrase ELL113 Week 4.
Emergence of Syntax. Introduction  One of the most important concerns of theoretical linguistics today represents the study of the acquisition of language.
Embedded Clauses in TAG
CAS LX 502 Semantics 3a. A formalism for meaning (cont ’ d) 3.2, 3.6.
Non-finite Verbs What is a non-finite verb?
Verb Forms and Related Matters
Lecture 9: The Gerund.  The English gerund is an intriguing structure which causes a particular problem for X-bar theory  [His constantly complaining.
Dr. Monira Al-Mohizea MORPHOLOGY & SYNTAX WEEK 12.
Verbals and Verbal Phrases
© Child language acquisition To what extent do children acquire language by actively working out its rules?
Morphology A Closer Look at Words By: Shaswar Kamal Mahmud.
Verbals. A gerund is a verbal that ends in -ing and functions as a noun. The term verbal indicates that a gerund, like the other two kinds of verbals,
Linguistics The ninth week. Chapter 3 Morphology  3.1 Introduction  3.2 Morphemes.
Albert Gatt LIN3021 Formal Semantics Lecture 4. In this lecture Compositionality in Natural Langauge revisited: The role of types The typed lambda calculus.
M ORPHOLOGY Lecturer/ Najla AlQahtani. W HAT IS MORPHOLOGY ? It is the study of the basic forms in a language. A morpheme is “a minimal unit of meaning.
Rules, Movement, Ambiguity
WORDS The term word is much more difficult to define in a technical sense, and like many other linguistic terms, there are often arguments about what exactly.
What tense is that verb? Naming verb tenses
◦ Process of describing the structure of phrases and sentences Chapter 8 - Phrases and sentences: grammar1.
Parts of Speech Melinda Norris Start. How to navigate through this tutorial At the bottom of each page, you will see buttons that allow you to move to.
1 Some English Constructions Transformational Framework October 2, 2012 Lecture 7.
Language and Cognition Colombo, June 2011 Day 2 Introduction to Linguistic Theory, Part 3.
1 LIN 1310B Introduction to Linguistics Prof: Nikolay Slavkov TA: Qinghua Tang CLASS 11, Feb 9, 2007.
Basic Syntactic Structures of English CSCI-GA.2590 – Lecture 2B Ralph Grishman NYU.
Phrase Definition review. Consists of an appositive and any modifiers the appositive has.
2 pt 3 pt. 4 pt 5pt 1 pt 2 pt 3 pt 4 pt 5 pt 1 pt 2pt 3 pt 4pt 5 pt 1pt 2pt 3 pt 4 pt 5 pt 1 pt 2 pt 3 pt 4pt 5 pt 1pt Parts of speech PunctuationVerbal's.
GRAMMAR REVIEW OF FIRST SEMESTER
Chapter 5 English Syntax: The Grammar of Words. What is syntax? the study of the structures of sentences combining words to create ‘all & only’ ‘well-formed’
ACT REVIEW. RUN-ONS A complete sentence contains a subject, a verb, and a complete thought. If any of the three is lacking, the sentence is called a.
Verbals and Verbal Phrases. What is a Verbal A verbal is a verb that acts as a noun, adjective, or adverb. A verbal is a verb that acts as a noun, adjective,
Week 3. Clauses and Trees English Syntax. Trees and constituency A sentence has a hierarchical structure Constituents can have constituents of their own.
King Faisal University جامعة الملك فيصل Deanship of E-Learning and Distance Education عمادة التعلم الإلكتروني والتعليم عن بعد [ ] 1 جامعة الملك فيصل عمادة.
GERUND Научный руководитель– Агаева Алия А.. The –ing Forms in English.
Verbals. Gerunds, infinitives, and participles, are words that originate from verbs. They can be confusing because they are like verbs and at the same.
Lecture 2: Categories and Subcategorisation
Introduction to Linguistics
Non-finite forms of the verb
Gerunds Participles Infinitives
Morphology Morphology Morphology Dr. Amal AlSaikhan Morphology.
Beginning Syntax Linda Thomas
Lecture -3 Week 3 Introduction to Linguistics – Level-5 MORPHOLOGY
Unit One: Parts of Speech
عمادة التعلم الإلكتروني والتعليم عن بعد
Syntax of the English Language
Word Classes and Affixes
VERBS PART 2.
Traditional Grammar VS. Generative Grammar
Structure of a Lexicon Debasri Chakrabarti 13-May-19.
Introduction to English morphology
Presentation transcript:

Week 2b. Categories and features CAS LX 522 Syntax I

Where we were… Lexical categories: Lexical categories: N: nounA: adjectiveAdv: adverb N: nounA: adjectiveAdv: adverb V: verbP: preposition V: verbP: preposition Functional categories: Functional categories: I: inflection/aux/modalD: determiner I: inflection/aux/modalD: determiner C: complementizerPRN: Pronoun C: complementizerPRN: Pronoun

Not all nouns are the same We’re trying to describe syntactic behavior of words, and we tried to put words into categories based on differences and similarities in behavior (distribution). We’re trying to describe syntactic behavior of words, and we tried to put words into categories based on differences and similarities in behavior (distribution). But we already know that there are differences even between members of the same category, for example count vs. mass nouns. But we already know that there are differences even between members of the same category, for example count vs. mass nouns.

Count and Mass We could just say, fine, we have two categories: Count, and Mass. We could just say, fine, we have two categories: Count, and Mass. [ Mass furniture], [ Mass freedom] [ Mass furniture], [ Mass freedom] [ Count chair], [ Count pinnacle] [ Count chair], [ Count pinnacle] But then we miss the fact, essentially, that they’re all nouns. E.g., what do adjectives modify? But then we miss the fact, essentially, that they’re all nouns. E.g., what do adjectives modify? Comfortable furniture makes me happy. Comfortable furniture makes me happy. Comfortable chairs make me happy. Comfortable chairs make me happy.

Commonalities and differences Basically, mass nouns have something in common with count nouns (namely, they’re nouns), while also having differences (count nouns are countable, mass nouns are not). Basically, mass nouns have something in common with count nouns (namely, they’re nouns), while also having differences (count nouns are countable, mass nouns are not). Nouns have the property of being a noun. Nouns have the property of being a noun. Count nouns have the property of being countable, mass nouns do not. Count nouns have the property of being countable, mass nouns do not.

Features A “feature” is a fancy name for “property”, and is used to express these similarities and differences. Features are generally binary [+F] or [-F]. A “feature” is a fancy name for “property”, and is used to express these similarities and differences. Features are generally binary [+F] or [-F]. Count nouns have the feature [+count]. Count nouns have the feature [+count]. Mass nouns have the feature [-count]. Mass nouns have the feature [-count]. Both have the feature [+N]. Both have the feature [+N]. So things that are true of nouns we can say are true of words with the feature [+N]. Things that are true of count nouns are true of things that are [+N, +count]. So things that are true of nouns we can say are true of words with the feature [+N]. Things that are true of count nouns are true of things that are [+N, +count].

Proper and common nouns We can similarly distinguish proper nouns (names) from common nouns (types). We can similarly distinguish proper nouns (names) from common nouns (types). Boston, Chomsky, September Boston, Chomsky, September park, linguist, month park, linguist, month Proper nouns don’t occur with determiners (or if they do, they are interpreted as if they were common nouns: I met every Chomsky at the picnic, I go to classes every September, the Boston I remember was cleaner than this. Proper nouns don’t occur with determiners (or if they do, they are interpreted as if they were common nouns: I met every Chomsky at the picnic, I go to classes every September, the Boston I remember was cleaner than this.

[±Common] So, Boston is a [+N, -Common] and park is a [+N, +Common]. But they are both [+N]. So, Boston is a [+N, -Common] and park is a [+N, +Common]. But they are both [+N]. These “secondary” features define subclasses of categories, and are sometimes referred to as subcategorial features. These “secondary” features define subclasses of categories, and are sometimes referred to as subcategorial features.

Feature matrices We can in fact encode many of the grammatical properties words can have as features, which will be useful in formulating our theory. The features will be anything that our grammatical rules/generalizations can “refer to”. We can in fact encode many of the grammatical properties words can have as features, which will be useful in formulating our theory. The features will be anything that our grammatical rules/generalizations can “refer to”. The dog [+N, +Count, -Plural] is hot. The dog [+N, +Count, -Plural] is hot. The dogs [+N, +Count, +Plural] are hot. The dogs [+N, +Count, +Plural] are hot. The soup [+N, -Count, -Plural] is hot. The soup [+N, -Count, -Plural] is hot. The scissors [+N, -Count, +Plural] are hot. The scissors [+N, -Count, +Plural] are hot.

Feature matrices The dog [+N, +Count, -Plural] is hot. The dog [+N, +Count, -Plural] is hot. The dogs [+N, +Count, +Plural] are hot. The dogs [+N, +Count, +Plural] are hot. The soup [+N, -Count, -Plural] is hot. The soup [+N, -Count, -Plural] is hot. The scissors [+N, -Count, +Plural] are hot. The scissors [+N, -Count, +Plural] are hot. The auxiliary be shows plural agreement: it is are when the subject is [+Plural] and is when the subject is [-Plural]. It doesn’t refer to (care about) [±Count]. The auxiliary be shows plural agreement: it is are when the subject is [+Plural] and is when the subject is [-Plural]. It doesn’t refer to (care about) [±Count].

Adjectives and adverbs Adjectives and adverbs are a lot alike. Most adjectives have an adverb form, and can in nonstandard speech in fact be used as adverbs. They both can be modified by very. Adjectives and adverbs are a lot alike. Most adjectives have an adverb form, and can in nonstandard speech in fact be used as adverbs. They both can be modified by very. Suggests that maybe this is more like the difference between mass and count nouns than like the difference between nouns and verbs— perhaps [±ADV] is a subcategorial feature. Suggests that maybe this is more like the difference between mass and count nouns than like the difference between nouns and verbs— perhaps [±ADV] is a subcategorial feature. quick: [+A, -ADV]quickly: [+A, +ADV] quick: [+A, -ADV]quickly: [+A, +ADV]

Verbal features Like for nouns, we can think of the different forms that verbs take as being differentiated by features: Like for nouns, we can think of the different forms that verbs take as being differentiated by features: (Note: I’m diverging a bit from Radford here, but I’m right.) (Note: I’m diverging a bit from Radford here, but I’m right.) He has shown improvement [+V, +Participle, -Past, +Perfect] He has shown improvement [+V, +Participle, -Past, +Perfect] He had shown improvement [+V, +Participle, +Past, +Perfect] He had shown improvement [+V, +Participle, +Past, +Perfect] He is showing improvement [+V, +Participle, -Past, -Perfect] He is showing improvement [+V, +Participle, -Past, -Perfect] He showed improvement [+V, -Participle, +Past] He showed improvement [+V, -Participle, +Past] He shows improvement [+V, -Participle, -Past, +3sg] He shows improvement [+V, -Participle, -Past, +3sg] You show improvement [+V, -Participle, -Past, -3sg] You show improvement [+V, -Participle, -Past, -3sg] So, -s is usually [-Participle, -Past, +3sg], -en is [+Participle, +Perfect], -ed is [+Past], -ing is [+Participle, -Perfect]. So, -s is usually [-Participle, -Past, +3sg], -en is [+Participle, +Perfect], -ed is [+Past], -ing is [+Participle, -Perfect].

Crosscategorial features Consider what un can attach to. Consider what un can attach to. untie, unfold, unwrap, unpack untie, unfold, unwrap, unpack unhappy, unfriendly, undead unhappy, unfriendly, undead *uncity, *uncola, *unconvention *uncity, *uncola, *unconvention *unupon, *unalongside, *unat *unupon, *unalongside, *unat Basically, it applies to reversible verbs and adjectives, but not to nouns or prepositions. How can we state that? Basically, it applies to reversible verbs and adjectives, but not to nouns or prepositions. How can we state that?

Crosscategorial features Suppose that nouns and verbs are the most basic categories. A noun is a noun and not a verb, and verb is a verb and not a noun. Suppose that nouns and verbs are the most basic categories. A noun is a noun and not a verb, and verb is a verb and not a noun. Noun: [+N, -V]. Noun: [+N, -V]. Verb: [-N, +V]. Verb: [-N, +V]. A conceptual reason to separate nouns and verbs is that verbs are basically predicates— they attribute some property to the noun. Nouns are basically arguments, to be assigned properties by verbs. A conceptual reason to separate nouns and verbs is that verbs are basically predicates— they attribute some property to the noun. Nouns are basically arguments, to be assigned properties by verbs.

Crosscategorial features Looked at this way, adjectives are kind of “verby” in that they are also attributing properties. Looked at this way, adjectives are kind of “verby” in that they are also attributing properties. It’s hard to make that really precise, but we have a more concrete syntactic similarity between verbs and adjectives too: both can take un-, while nouns and prepositions cannot. It’s hard to make that really precise, but we have a more concrete syntactic similarity between verbs and adjectives too: both can take un-, while nouns and prepositions cannot.

Supercategories Chomsky (1970) proposed that we explain this by supposing that [±N] and [±V] are the two basic features that determine the four lexical categories (N, V, A, P). Chomsky (1970) proposed that we explain this by supposing that [±N] and [±V] are the two basic features that determine the four lexical categories (N, V, A, P). N: [+N, -V]V: [-N, +V] N: [+N, -V]V: [-N, +V] P: [-N, -V]A: [+N, +V] P: [-N, -V]A: [+N, +V] Given that, what does un attach to? Given that, what does un attach to?

Russian Case Other languages can give us evidence of this as well. For example, Russian nouns (all nouns) are marked for Case (like English pronouns are: me vs. I), but when they are modified by an adjective, the adjective is also marked for case. Other languages can give us evidence of this as well. For example, Russian nouns (all nouns) are marked for Case (like English pronouns are: me vs. I), but when they are modified by an adjective, the adjective is also marked for case. What gets marked for Case in Russian? What gets marked for Case in Russian? Krasivaya dyevushka vsunula chornuyu koshku v pustuyu korobku beautifulgirlputblackcatinemptybox ‘The beautiful girl put the black cat in the empty box’

Functional and lexical That takes care of N, V, A, P, but what about our functional categories? That takes care of N, V, A, P, but what about our functional categories? In fact, the functional categories (C, I, D, PRN) each seem a little like a lexical category. In fact, the functional categories (C, I, D, PRN) each seem a little like a lexical category. Auxiliaries seem a lot like verbs (have, be, do), and inflect like verbs do. Auxiliaries seem a lot like verbs (have, be, do), and inflect like verbs do. Complementizers and infinitival to seem a bit like prepositions (e.g., for, to). Complementizers and infinitival to seem a bit like prepositions (e.g., for, to). Pronouns are kind of nouny. Pronouns are kind of nouny. Determiners are a bit adjectivey. Determiners are a bit adjectivey.

[+F]? Perhaps we can add a third binary feature, [±F] to capture this: Perhaps we can add a third binary feature, [±F] to capture this: [-F][+N][-N] [+V]AV [-V]NP[+F][+N][-N][+V]DAux [-V]PRN C, I

Grammatical category So what, then is a grammatical category? So what, then is a grammatical category? A grammatical category is a set of elements which have the same value(s) for a given set of grammatical features. A grammatical category is a set of elements which have the same value(s) for a given set of grammatical features. Category labels like “N”, or “Aux” are really just shorthand for feature matrices like [+N, -V, -F], or [-N, +V, +F]. Notationally convenient. Category labels like “N”, or “Aux” are really just shorthand for feature matrices like [+N, -V, -F], or [-N, +V, +F]. Notationally convenient.

The morphology of be Consider the following. How can we write an efficient set of rules to give us the correct form of be? Consider the following. How can we write an efficient set of rules to give us the correct form of be? [-Past][+Past] [-Pl][+Pl][-Pl][+Pl] [+1]1amarewaswere [-1, +2] 2arearewerewere [-1, -2] 3isarewaswere

The morphology of be Ok, let’s test it out. Ok, let’s test it out. I [+1, -2, -Pl, -Past, -N, +V, +F] ecstatic. I [+1, -2, -Pl, -Past, -N, +V, +F] ecstatic. They [-1, -2, +Pl, +Past, -N, +V, +F] leaving. They [-1, -2, +Pl, +Past, -N, +V, +F] leaving. Y’all [-1, +2, +Pl, +Past, -N, +V, +F] late. Y’all [-1, +2, +Pl, +Past, -N, +V, +F] late. In general, we might suppose that’s all there is to auxiliary be: a bundle of properties (auxiliary be: -N, +V, +F; subject agreement: ±1, ±2, ±Pl; tense: ±Past) and some rules to pronounce the bundles. In general, we might suppose that’s all there is to auxiliary be: a bundle of properties (auxiliary be: -N, +V, +F; subject agreement: ±1, ±2, ±Pl; tense: ±Past) and some rules to pronounce the bundles.

Separating the underlying from the pronounced This is a worthwhile point to remember. There is an underlying feature bundle, the auxiliary is an auxiliary be, with a tense, and some agreement features. We know the rules about pronouncing those features. Sometimes two feature bundles end up being pronounced in the same way. This is a worthwhile point to remember. There is an underlying feature bundle, the auxiliary is an auxiliary be, with a tense, and some agreement features. We know the rules about pronouncing those features. Sometimes two feature bundles end up being pronounced in the same way. In a real sense, the are in You are happy and the are in We are happy are different words, that just happen to be pronounced the same way. But they could have been pronounced distinctly. In a real sense, the are in You are happy and the are in We are happy are different words, that just happen to be pronounced the same way. But they could have been pronounced distinctly.

                      