Classical Conditioning

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Facebook Group: The group is called: Psych281 Spring08 Available only to University of Alberta network Sorry to be rude but… Please don’t add me as a friend.
Advertisements

Pavlovian (Classical) Conditioning. Example: “Bell” and Food CS US URCR Later Trials CS US UR First Few Trials Time CS = bell US = food UR = salivation.
Chapter 4 – Classical Conditioning: Mechanisms Important characteristics of the CS and US –1) Novelty of CS and US Latent Inhibition –association account.
Lectures 5&6: Pavlovian Conditioning (Basic Concepts & Generality)
Classical Conditioning. A. Definitions 1.Classical conditioning: learning that takes place when originally neutral stimulus comes to produce a conditioned.
Factors Influencing Respondent & Operant Learning: Part 2 Lesson 10.
Chapter 3: Pavlovian Conditioning: Foundations Pavlovian Conditioning or Classical Conditioning Ivan Pavlov –Early 1900s –A Russian physiologist digestive.
Extinction of Learned Behavior Lesson 14. Extinction of Reinforced Behavior n Behavior changes relatively permanent l Modifiable if situation changes.
Psychology 485 September 28,  Introduction & History  Three major questions: What is learned? Why learn through classical conditioning? How does.
Introduction to Psychology, 7th Edition, Rod Plotnik Module 9: Classical Conditioning Module 9 Classical Conditioning.
Conditioned Inhibition
Chapter 3: Pavlovian Conditioning: Foundations
Module 9 Classical Conditioning. 3 Kinds of Learning l Classical Conditioning n Kind of learning in which a neutral stimulus acquires the ability to produce.
Module 9 Classical Conditioning MR. McKinley First a quick video… games/videos/pavlovs-bell.htm
Inhibitory Pavlovian Conditioning Stimuli can become conditioned to signal the absence of a US— such learning is called Inhibitory Conditioning CS+ = excitatory.
Lecture 20: Extinction (Pavlovian & Instrumental) Learning, Psychology 5310 Spring, 2015 Professor Delamater.
Lectures 7&8: Pavlovian Conditioning (Determining Conditions) Learning, Psychology 5310 Spring, 2015 Professor Delamater.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 3 – Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 5 – The Role of Conditioning in Behavior.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning
Copyright © 2005 Pearson Education Canada Inc. Learning Chapter 5.
PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 (Cont.) Indirect Conditioning Applications of Conditioning.
Classical Conditioning: The Elements of Associative Learning
PSY402 Theories of Learning Wednesday January 15, 2003.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 3 – Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)
Classical Conditioning
Psychology 2250 Last class Definition of learning
Principles of Behavior Change Classical Conditioning.
Learning Theories Goal  How do we learn behaviors through classical conditioning?
Psychology 001 Introduction to Psychology Christopher Gade, PhD Office: 621 Heafey Office hours: F 3-6 and by apt. Class WF 7:00-8:30.
CHAPTER 4 Pavlovian Conditioning: Causal Factors.
Chapter 6 Learning.
Learning Ms. Simon Do Now: Define Learning. Definition Learning is a relatively permanent change in an organism’s behavior due to experience.
Psychology 2250 Last Class Characteristics of Habituation and Sensitization -time course -stimulus-specificity -effects of strong extraneous stimuli (dishabituation)
Psychology of Learning EXP4404 Chapter 3: Pavlovian (Classical) Conditioning Dr. Steve.
Table of Contents CHAPTER 6 Learning. Table of ContentsLEARNING  Learning  Classical conditioning  Operant/Instrumental conditioning  Observational.
Innate Behavior Patterns Reflex Tropism –kinesis (undirected) –taxis (directed) Fixed Action Pattern –species-specific; unlearned; goes to completion Reaction.
Classical Conditioning Underlying Processes and Practical Application.
Lecture 2: Classical Conditioning. Types of learning Habituation and sensitization Classical (Pavlovian) conditioning Instrumental (Operant) conditioning.
Experimental Evidence  Rats drink little saccharin water at first but increase over time.  Loud tones (110 db) produce different responses depending.
Innate Knowledge (what an organism is born with) Experience leads to changes in knowledge and behavior Learning refers to the process of adaptation Of.
Factors Influencing Conditioning  CS and US Intensity, and Attention to the CS  Temporal relationship  Predictiveness  Preparedness  Redundancy 1.
Unit 7: Learning. Behaviorism Belief that behavior is learned –genetics has little to do with it. “nurture-only” belief, mainstream1960s and 70s.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 – Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)
Chapter 6 Learning and Behavior Learning n A more or less permanent change in behavior that results from experience.
Pavlovian or Classical Conditioning Psychology 3306.
Learning Part I Learning Classical Conditioning
Module 9 Classical Conditioning. THREE KINDS OF LEARNING Learning –A relatively enduring or permanent change in behavior that results from previous experience.
Blocking The phenomenon of blocking tells us that what happens to one CS depends not only on its relationship to the US but also on the strength of other.
PSY402 Theories of Learning Friday January 17, 2003.
Dr. Steven I. Dworkin Classical Conditioning: Underlying Processes and Practical Applications Chapter 5.
Classical Conditioning A learning procedure in which subjects make associations between a natural stimulus and a neutral stimulus Ivan Pavlov Tuning fork/salivation.
Table of Contents Chapter 6 Learning. Table of Contents Learning –Classical conditioning –Operant/Instrumental conditioning –Observational learning Ivan.
PRINCIPLES AND APPLICATIONS OF CLASSICAL CONDITIONING Chapter 4 1.
Learning & Memory JEOPARDY. The Field CC Basics Important Variables Theories Grab Bag $100 $200$200 $300 $500 $400 $300 $400 $300 $400 $500 $400.
Basic Learning Processes Robert C. Kennedy, PhD University of Central Florida
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 3 – Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)
ATTRIBUTES OF LEARNING AND CLASSICAL CONDITIONING.
Chapter 6 LEARNING. Learning Learning – A process through which experience produces lasting change in behavior or mental processes. Behavioral Learning.
Learning Long lasting, relatively permanent change in behavior due to experience.
Rescorla-Wagner Model  US-processing model  Can account for some Pavlovian Conditioning phenomena: acquisition blocking unblocking with an upshift conditioned.
Classical Conditioning
Unit 4: Memory & Learning
PSY 402 Theories of Learning
Classical Conditioning and prediction
Factors Influencing Respondent & Operant Learning
PSY 402 Theories of Learning
PSY 402 Theories of Learning
Classical Conditioning
Presentation transcript:

Classical Conditioning Chapter 3 Classical Conditioning

Ivan Petrovich Pavlov (1849-1936) Classical (Pavlovian) conditioning Medical physiologist Digestion Human/animal differences Conditioned reflexes en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:Ivan_Pavlov_(Nobel).png en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:One_of_Pavlov%27s_dogs.jpg

Terminology Unconditional stimulus (US) Unconditional response (UR) Stimulus that elicits the innate reflex (e.g., food) Unconditional response (UR) Reflex action that occurs in response to US (e.g., salivation) Conditional stimulus (CS) Any stimulus that doesn’t originally elicit the UR (e.g., bell) Conditional response (CR) The action elicited by the CS (e.g., salivation)

Conditioning and Awareness Awareness of conditioning not required for learning

Innate US-UR is an innate stimulus-behaviour “Reflex” Hardwired Stereotypic pattern of behaviour

Example: Bell and Food CS US UR CR Later Trials CS US UR First Few Trials Time CS = bell US = food UR = salivation CR = salivation

Processes Acquisition Extinction Acquiring a CR E.g., pair CS with US Reduce/eliminate a CR E.g., present CS without US

Measuring Conditioning Sometimes difficult to measure CR e.g., if CS & US close together, CR & UR can overlap Test trial (probe trial) Give CS alone Intensity Does CR intensity increase with experience?

Example: Eyeblink Conditioning CS (tone) US (airpuff) Airpuff on eye Blink UR vs. CR eyeblinks UR blink faster than CR blink CR (blink) UR (blink)

Example: Taste Aversion Very strong Very persistent Usually conditioned after one presentation Experiment Rats fed novel food (CS) Injected with lithium chloride (US) Choice: novel food or regular food Chose regular food

Higher-Order Conditioning CSs and USs can be associated (First-order) CSs can be associated with other CSs Second-order conditioning

First-Order Conditioning tone (CS1) food (US) salivation (CR)

Second-Order Conditioning light (CS2) tone (CS1) salivation (CR) Risk of extinction? salivation (CR) tone (CS1) food (US)

CS+ and CS- CS+ (excitatory CS) CS- (inhibitory CS) CS predicts occurrence of US Activates behaviour related to US CS- (inhibitory CS) CS predicts non-occurrence of US Suppresses behaviour related to US

PAVLOV’S PROCEDURE Trial Type A Trial Type B CS+ CS- US Randomize trial type presentation NEGATIVE CONTINGENCY PROTOCOL CS- US Context cues serve as CS+

Testing for CS- CS- produces absence of CR No CR You’ve produced CS- Haven’t learned anything How to measure nothing… Summation test Measure CR with CS+ Compound stimulus of CS+ & CS-; measure CR Retardation of acquisition Trained CS- and novel stimulus; pair both with novel US for same number of trials Measure CR for both Prior learning of CS- inhibits learning new association

Short Delay Conditioning Strongest and most rapid Simple autonomic responses: 5-30 seconds Quick skeletal responses: 0.5 seconds CS US or

Long Delay Conditioning Other distracting stimuli? Timing estimation required CS US or

Trace Conditioning From “memory trace” Must remember CS Other stimuli interfere trace interval CS US

Simultaneous Conditioning Weaker than short delay CS can’t signal onset of US Not predictive CS US

Backward Conditioning Ignores order; US comes first CS has no predictiveness Might become CS- CS US

Influences in Classical Conditioning

CS-US Contiguity Closeness together in time and/or space Usually, more learning if greater contiguity between CS & US Type of conditioning may influence this e.g., eyeblink vs. taste aversion

CS-US Contingency If-then situation X iff Y Consistency of pairing CS and US Greater contingency, greater learning

Stimulus Features Nature of stimulus affects its conditioning ability Intensity Novelty

Compound Stimuli Two+ simple CSs presented at the same time Paired with US

Overshadowing Salience Exclusive regulation of CR by most salient CS in compound stimuli

Latent Inhibition Repeatedly present neutral stimulus (N) Pair N with US Harder to condition N as CS CS- or habituation

Blocking CS1 -- US CS1 and novel stimulus (CS2) with US CS1 --> CR CS2 --> no or very weak CR

Textbook Error: p. 77 “But suppose we eat two foods, one spicy and the other bland. If we then become sick, thanks to blocking we are likely to develop an aversion to the spicy food -- even though it may have been the bland food that caused our illness.”

Sensory Preconditioing Pair two neutral stimuli repeatedly Pair one with US repeatedly until CR produced Test other stimulus CR produced

Number of CS-US Pairings Acquisition curve Non-linear Asymptote asymptote CR Strength Conditioning Trials

Intertrial Interval ITI Time between each CS-US pairing (i.e., between trials) Generally, around 30 seconds effective

Extinction of CR

Extinction CS without US --> Extinction Weakening and stopping of CR Not forgetting A type of conditioning CS paired with absence of US

Spontaneous Recovery After extinction, let time pass Present CS again (no US) Temporary, small return of CR Shows extinction is not forgetting

Relearning/Reacquisition Effect Extinguish CR Recondition with CS-US pairing Fewer trials required

Putting it Together Strength of CR Trials/Time Spontaneous Recovery Acquisition Extinction Reacquisition Strength of CR CS&US CS alone CS alone CS&US Trials/Time

Theories of Classical Conditioning Associationism, Stimulus Substitution, Preparedness, Rescorla-Wagner

Associationism Linking together of: Contiguity, similarity, contrast Events Memories Actions and consequences Contiguity, similarity, contrast Central to study of learning and behaviour

Ebbinghaus’ Memory Experiments Nonsense syllables E.g., ZOG, PAF, TOB One subject Recite from memory Savings E.g., if 10 trials initially, then after a delay 3 more trials, savings = (10-3)/10 = 7/10 = 70%

Major Findings List length Effects of repetition Effects of time Overlearning Effects of time Role of contiguity Backwards associations Forgetting Curve Time between study and relearning 20min 1hr 8.8hr 1day 2days 6days 31days 100 75 50 25 Percent Savings

Classical Conditioning Innate US-UR reflex pathway CS is associated with the US Through the associative process, CR is produced

Stimulus Substitution Theory Pavlov CR and UR produced by same neural region CS takes on properties of US Substitution CR should be the same as UR

Example: Sign Tracking Response not required US often food Stimulus (CS) indicates US availability Subject “tracks” the sign more and more CS takes on properties of US Pigeon autoshaping Longbox autoshaping F = CS F = US

Biological Predispositions Burns & Domjan (2000) Timberlake & Grant (1975)

Problems with SST CS not a complete substitute for US e.g., eyeblink differences Magnitudes CSs produce different responses Omissions and additions Compensatory conditional responses

Preparatory Response Theory Learn responses that prepare organism for US occurrence Sometimes CR same as UR, sometimes different

Example: Drug Tolerance Neurophysiological dependencies Siegel (1975) Contextual stimuli act as CSs Compensatory CR Morphine

Contextual Stimuli Theory Rats on hotplate Between-groups study Independent variables: Morphine or placebo Location of injection (Home or Injection room) Dependent variable: time to lift feet

Results Control (placebo): 13 sec. Exp. Gr. 1 (morphine): 24 sec. (day 1) to 13 sec. (day 4) Injection room gives contextual cues Compensatory CR Exp. Gr. 2 (morphine): Day 1-3 injection room: 24 --> 13 sec. latency Day 4 home room: 28 sec. latency

Interpretation US: Morphine UR: Pain reduction CS: Injection room CR: Pain sensitivity CS prepares rats for morphine injection Body homeostasis

Rescorla-Wagner Theory Contiguity account Associative strength CS acquires limited amount of associative strength on any one trial

Three Factors in Theory Maximum associative strength Difference between current and maximum strength Number of additional CSs

Rescorla-Wagner Equation DVn = c( - Vn-1) DVn: change in associative strength for CS on one trial c: represents salience of CS and US; a constant (0.0-1.0) : maximum associative strength (magnitude of UR) Vn-1: associative strength already accrued by CS

Acquisition Phase Example: set c = 0.25,  = 10.0 Vn-1 starts at 0.0 For the first trial Vn-1 = V1-1 = V0 For the second trial Vn-1 = V2-1 = V1

Total associative strength Vn (or “VTotal”) after two trials: First CS-US pairing: DVn = c( - Vn-1) DV1 = 0.25(10.0 - 0.0) = 2.5 Second CS-US pairing: DV2 = 0.25(10.0 - 2.5) = 1.88 Total associative strength Vn (or “VTotal”) after two trials: V1 + V2 = 2.5 + 1.88 = 4.38 Third CS-US pairing: DV3 = 0.25(10.0 - 4.38) = 1.41

Acquisition Phase  Trial DVn Vn (VTotal) 0 0.00 0.00 1 2.50 2.50 Trials Associative Strength (Vn) 0 0.00 0.00 1 2.50 2.50 2 1.88 4.38 3 1.41 5.79 DV3 4 1.05 6.84 DV2 5 0.79 7.63 6 0.59 8.22 DV1 7 0.45 8.67 8 0.33 9.00 9 0.25 9.25 10 0.19 9.44

Extinction Example: After first extinction trial: Set c = 0.25,  = 0.0 After first extinction trial: DVn = c ( - Vn-1) = 0.25(0.0 - 10.0) = -2.5

Extinction Trials Associative Strength (Vi)  = 0.0 DV1 DV2 DV3

Blocking Learned CS blocks subsequent CSs Example CS = tone, novel CS = light c = 0.25,  = 10.0 Completed 8 trials with just tone, V8 = 9.0 DVn = 0.25(10.0 - 9.0) Only 1 unit of associative strength left to split between the tone and the light Ultimately, Vtone=9.5 and Vlight=0.5