Models of word production and reaction-time evidence.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Interlanguage IL LEC. 9.
Advertisements

Accessing spoken words: the importance of word onsets
Marslen-Wilson Big Question: “What processes take place during the period that the sensory information is accumulating for the listener” during spoken.
Knowing More than One Language: The Psycholinguistics of Bilingualism Marina Blekher Department of Linguistics.
Chapter 4 Key Concepts.
Psycholinguistic what is psycholinguistic? 1-pyscholinguistic is the study of the cognitive process of language acquisition and use. 2-The scope of psycholinguistic.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production: Models cont.
Language and Cognition Colombo, June 2011 Day 8 Aphasia: disorders of comprehension.
Using prosody to avoid ambiguity: Effects of speaker awareness and referential context Snedeker and Trueswell (2003) Psych 526 Eun-Kyung Lee.
Phonological Priming in Spontaneous Speech Production Katrina Housel H uman L anguage P rocessing L ab.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production: Models of language production.
Albert Gatt LIN1180 – Semantics Lecture 10. Part 1 (from last week) Theories of presupposition: the semantics- pragmatics interface.
Models of Language Language and Cognition Colombo 2011.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics
Autosegmental Phonology
Syntactic Priming in Bilinguals: Effects of verb repetition in an L2-monolingual and cross-lingual setting Sofie Schoonbaert 1, Robert Hartsuiker 1, &
Research presentation Assignment 1 per group: –Prepare a min research presentation of your experiment (power point or overhead, and script of what.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Representing language.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production: Models.
Mental Lexicon Body of knowledge we hold in our minds about words Includes pronunciation, spelling, meaning syntactic roles Recognition of words—whether.
Language Development Major Questions: 1) What is language/what is involved in language? 2) What are the stages of language development? 3) Is language.
Language, Mind, and Brain by Ewa Dabrowska Chapter 2: Language processing: speed and flexibility.
Linguisitics Levels of description. Speech and language Language as communication Speech vs. text –Speech primary –Text is derived –Text is not “written.
Sound and Speech. The vocal tract Figures from Graddol et al.
Chapter Four The Cognitive Approach I: History, Vision, and Attention.
A Modular Approach to STM Allan Baddeley: Articulatory Loop Central Executive Visuospatial Sketchpad The article by Lee Brooks considers a double-dissociation.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production: Theories and models.
Psycholinguistics 05 Internal Lexicon.
Meaning and Language Part 1.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Acquisition: Bilinugalism.
Lecture 1 Introduction: Linguistic Theory and Theories
Generative Grammar(Part ii)
Interactions between Language and Stuttering NU/SFA Workshop for Fluency Specialists July, 1996 J. Scott Yaruss, Ph.D., CCC-SLP University of Pittsburgh.
14: THE TEACHING OF GRAMMAR  Should grammar be taught?  When? How? Why?  Grammar teaching: Any strategies conducted in order to help learners understand,
Communication Disorders
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production & Comprehension: Conversation & Dialog.
Jelena Mirković and Maryellen C. MacDonald Language and Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, University of Wisconsin-Madison Introduction How to Study Subject-Verb.
Retrieval of Lexical-Syntactic Features in Tip-of-the-Tongue States Michele Miozzo and Alfonso Caramazza Presented by Ping Yu.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics
WORD SEMANTICS 4 DAY 29 – NOV 4, 2011 Brain & Language LING NSCI Harry Howard Tulane University.
Explaining second language learning
+ Treatment of Aphasia Week 12 April 1 st, Review Involvement of semantic and phonological stages in naming. Differentiating features of naming.
1 Speech Perception 3/30/00. 2 Speech Perception How do we perceive speech? –Multifaceted process –Not fully understood –Models & theories attempt to.
Reading. How do you think we read? -memorizing words on the page -extracting just the meanings of the words -playing a mental movie in our heads of what.
Age of acquisition and frequency of occurrence: Implications for experience based models of word processing and sentence parsing Marc Brysbaert.
Phonological Encoding II Producingconnectedspeech.
Morphology A Closer Look at Words By: Shaswar Kamal Mahmud.
Levels of Language 6 Levels of Language. Levels of Language Aspect of language are often referred to as 'language levels'. To look carefully at language.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Conversation & Dialog: Language Production and Comprehension in conjoined action.
Recent Models of Stuttering Western Illinois University February 7, 1997 J. Scott Yaruss, Ph.D., CCC-SLP University of Pittsburgh.
The effects of working memory load on negative priming in an N-back task Ewald Neumann Brain-Inspired Cognitive Systems (BICS) July, 2010.
Sahin, Pinker, Cash, Schomer, & Halgren (2009) Sequential processing of lexical, grammatical, and phonological information within Broca’s area.
Comparing the effectiveness of orthographic and phonological cues in the treatment of anomia. Lyndsey Nickels 1, Antje Lorenz 1,2, 1 Macquarie Centre for.
COGNITIVE MORPHOLOGY Laura Westmaas November 24, 2009.
Levels of Linguistic Analysis
Interactivity in lexical access The modularity debate.
SLA Effects of Recasts as Implicit Knowledge Young-ah Do Fall, College English Education.
Theories of Priming II : Types of Primes Timothy McNamara Journal of Experimental Psychology,1994 조 성 식조 성 식.
VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION. What is Word Recognition? Features, letters & word interactions Interactive Activation Model Lexical and Sublexical Approach.
Rob Hartsuiker (Ghent University) Martin Pickering & Nivja de Jong
Cognitive Processes in SLL and Bilinguals:
Phonological Priming and Lexical Access in Spoken Word Recognition
Sentence Production.
‘The most natural way to communicate is simply to speak
Levels of Linguistic Analysis
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics
Phonological Priming and Lexical Access in Spoken Word Recognition
LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND THOUGHT
Fromkin's Utterance Generator
Presentation transcript:

Models of word production and reaction-time evidence

Last week 3 stages to production: Conceptualisation Formulation Articulation Formulation involves lexical retrieval: Semantic/syntactic content (lemma) Phonological content (word-form) Tip of tongue state when lemma is retrieved without word-form being retrieved

This week Levelt et al.’s theory of word production Modularity in language production Experimental evidence for this account Dell’s interactive account Recent experimental evidence that supports interaction Can the modular approach explain these results?

Levelt, Roelofs and Meyer’s (1999) model of word production See two figures: theory outline and description of network Theory outline: multiple levels of representation lack of feedback except via the monitor Network has three strata conceptual stratum lemma stratum word-form stratum

Conceptual stratum Conceptual stratum is not decomposed thus, one lexical concept node for “escort” not decomposed into “walk”, “be with”, “safeguard” instead, conceptual links from “escort” to “safeguard”, etc.

Activation of concepts “pragmatically”, via the intention to communicate something (e.g., describing an object) together with “perspective” (e.g., using “dog” vs. “animal”) but also via spreading activation from other concepts or via direct activation of that concept (e.g., the word “dog”) or perhaps via some random, spontaneous activation

Lexical selection First, lemma activation occurs This involves activating a lemma or lemmas corresponding to the concept thus, concept DOG activates lemma “dog” but also involves activating other lemmas DOG also activates CAT (etc.) to some extent and CAT activates lemma “cat”

Lemma selection Distinguished from activation Only one lemma is selected (in this model) probability of selecting the target lemma (“dog”) is the ratio of that lemma’s activation to the total activation of all lemmas (“dog”, “cat”, etc.) hence competition between semantically related lemmas competitors can be activated in other ways (see below)

Determining grammatical properties Fixing “diacritic parameters” correspond to grammatical properties of the word include grammatical category (noun, verb, etc.) include number, person, tense, mood for verbs include number, person, gender, count/mass status for nouns DOGS leads to selection of “dog” lemma plus plural number and noun category parameters Note: function words can be selected on purely syntactic grounds e.g., that in John saw that is activated by saw

Morpho-phonological encoding (and beyond) (a very rough sketch) The lemma is now converted into a phonological representation called “word-form” (or “lexeme”) If “dog” lemma plus plural (and noun) are activated Leads to activation of morphemes dog and s Morpheme = smallest meaningful unit of the language

Other late stages (in speaking) Accessing metrical shape basically, syllable structure and stress pattern escort has two syllables and is stress-final Accessing segmental make-up basically the phonemes that make up the word e.g., /d/, /o/, /g/ Then a phonetic representation is constructed This specifies the articulatory task that will produce the word Finally, the word is articulated

A modular account claim that processes are encapsulated, so that the output to a module depends only on its inputs cf. The Modularity of Mind (Fodor, 1983) but Fodor didn’t really consider language production in production, modularity amounts to the claim that later processes cannot affect earlier processes

Levelt et al.’s model allows no feedback during encoding critically, no feedback from word-form stratum to lemma stratum contrasts with Dell’s account (see below)

Also, only one lemma activates a word form if “dog” and “cat” lemmas are activated, they compete to produce a winner at the lemma stratum Only the “winner” activates a word form the word forms for the “losers” aren’t accessed hence, it doesn’t allow “cascading” activation

A rift between lemma and word form Levelt et al.’s model assumes a clear divide only one word (lemma) can “cross the divide” between lemma and wordform strata and no feedback possible Accords well with TOT and anomia data sometimes the divide cannot be crossed

Self-monitoring listening to yourself Correcting yourself Critical role in Levelt et al.’s model “external loop” monitoring what is said internal loop” monitoring the phonological representation (probably) used to explain results that appear to be due to feedback or cascading activation

Picture-word interference Participants name basic objects as quickly as possible e.g., picture of a dog Distractor words are embedded in the object e.g., the word cat participants are instructed to ignore these words

A stroop-like effect Basic finding that semantically related words can interfere with naming e.g., the word cat in a picture of a dog However, form-related words can speed up processing e.g., the word dot in a picture of a dog

Experiments manipulate timing: picture and word can be presented simultaneously or one can slightly precede the other We draw inferences about time-course of processing and test word-production models

Schriefers, Meyer, and Levelt (1990) Auditory presentation of distractors hence, definitely phonological (not orthographic) effect Conditions unrelated word SHIP phonologically relatedDOT alliterative (i.e., same beginning) semantically related CAT TARGET: DOG

SOA (Stimulus onset asynchrony) manipulation -150 ms (word …150 ms … picture) 0 ms (i.e., synchronous presentation) +150 ms (picture …150ms …word)

Results Semantic effect: -150 ms Word … 150 ms … Picture  inhibition (related slower than control) 0 ms, +150 ms Word = Picture; Picture …150 ms … Word  no effect

Results Phonological effect: ms: Word … 150 ms … Picture  no effect 0 ms, +150 ms Word = Picture; Picture …150 ms … Word  facilitation (related faster than control)

Separate semantic and phonological stages? Early semantic inhibition Late phonological facilitation Fits with the assumption that semantic processing precedes phonological processing No overlap suggests two discrete stages in production an interactive account might find semantic and phonological effects at the same time

Other studies support and extend these results other picture-word experiments (e.g., Meyer & Schriefers, 1991, JEP:LMC) other methods (e.g., Levelt et al., 1991; Van Turrenout et al., 1998) but many other experiments criticise these claims (see below)

Dell’s interactive account Dell (1986) presented the best-known interactive account other similar accounts exist 3 levels of representation semantics (decomposed into features) words phonemes (sounds)

Interactive because information flows “upwards” as well as “downwards” e.g., the semantic features mammal, barks, four- legs activate the word “dog” this activates the sounds /d/, /o/, /g/ these send activation back to the word level, activating words containing these sounds (e.g., “log”, “dot”) to some extent this activation is upwards (phonology to syntax) and wouldn’t occur in Levelt’s account

Evidence: “Mixed” errors Both semantic and phonological relationship to target word Target = “cat” semantic error = “dog” phonological error = “hat” mixed error = “rat” Occur more often than predicted by modular models if you can go wrong at either stage, it would only be by chance that an error would be mixed

Dell’s explanation the semantic features of dog activate lemma “cat” some features (e.g., animate, mammalian) activate “rat” as well “cat” then activates the sounds /k/, /ae/, /t/ /ae/ and /t/ activate “rat” by feedback this confluence of activation leads to increased tendency for “rat” to be uttered Also explains the tendency for phonological errors to be real words Sounds can only feed back to words (non-words not represented) so only words can feedback to sound level

Why might interaction occur? Can’t exist just to produce errors! Perhaps because the same network is used in comprehension So feedback would be the normal comprehension route Dell argues against this because many aphasics have good auditory word recognition yet disturbed phonological encoding

Alternatively, it simply serves to increase fluency in lemma selection advantageous to select a lemma whose phonological form is easy to find

Evidence for interactivity A number of recent experimental findings appear to support interaction under some circumstances (or at least cascading models) Peterson & Savoy (JEP:LMC, 1998) Cutting & Ferreira (JEP:LMC, 1999) Griffin & Bock (JML, 1998) Damian & Martin (JEP:LMC, 1999)

Peterson & Savoy found evidence for phonological activation of near synonyms: Participants slower to say distractor soda than unrelated distractor when naming couch Soda is related to non-selected sofa remember that Levelt et al. assume that only one lemma can be selected and hence activate a phonological form Levelt et al’s explanation: Could be erroneous selection of two lemmas?

Damian and Martin (1999) Extension of Schriefers et al.’s picture- word interference task remember that semantic inhibition occurred early, phonological facilitation occurred late (with no overlap) various methodological changes and developments focus on Experiment 3

The critical difference from Schriefers et al. is the addition of a “semantic and phonological” condition Picture of Apple peach (semantically related) apathy (phonologically related) apricot (sem & phono related) couch (unrelated) (also no-word control, always fast)

Results Relatedness-150 ms0 ms+ 150 ms Unrelated Semantic Phono S & P

Summary of findings early semantic inhibition (- 150 and 0 ms) late phonological facilitation (0 and ms) shows overlap, unlike Schriefers et al. but S & P condition didn’t show early semantic inhibition

This last finding demonstrates that semantic interference is reduced in the simultaneous presence of a phonological relationship (which should facilitate) Thus the finding appears to contradict the “discrete two-step” account of Levelt et al.

Can the two-stage account be saved? Evidence for interaction is hard to reconcile with the Levelt account however, most attempts are likely to revolve around the monitor basically, people sometimes notice a problem and screen it out Levelt argues that evidence for interaction really involves “special cases”, not directly related to normal processing

Summary Levelt et al.’s theory of word production: Strictly modular lexical access Syntactic processing precedes phonological processing Dell’s interactive account: Interaction between syntactic and phonological processing Experimental evidence is equivocal, but increasing evidence that more than one lemma may activate associated wordform

Caramazza’s alternative Caramazza and colleagues argue against the existence of the lemma node instead they propose a direct link between semantic level and lexeme syntactic information is associated with the lexeme Also assumes separate lexemes for written and spoken production This is really a different issue

Much evidence comes from patient data But also evidence from the independence of syntactic and phonological information in TOT states see discussion of Vigliocco et al. also Caramazza and Miozzo (Cognition, 1997; see also replies by Roelofs et al.)