INFANTS’ PERCEPTION OF FACE-AFFECT RELATIONS IN MULTIMODAL EVENTS Melissa A. Shuman & Lorraine E. Bahrick Florida International University Introduction.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Role of communication experience in facial expression coding in preschool children Vera Labunskaya a a Academy of psychology and pedagogy of Southern Federal.
Advertisements

Electrodermal Measures of Face Recognition Iowa State University of Science and Technology Alison L. MorrisDanielle R. Mitchell Nichole Stubbe Anne M.
Chapter 5: Entering the Social World
Fundamentals of Lifespan Development SEPTEMBER 17 – PHYSICAL DEVELOPMENT IN INFANCY AND TODDLERHOOD.
T HE EFFECT OF E THNICITY ON V ISUAL L ANGUAGE D ISCRIMINATION Cathy Lin 4 th year directed study student in Psychology and Linguistics With Drs. Janet.
Age Differences in Emotion Recognition of Briefly Presented Faces Lisa Emery, Kory Morgan, Kaitlyn Pechanek & Caitlin Williams Reprints may be obtained.
Does anyone have concerns about the child’s functioning with regard to the outcome area? D OES THE CHILD EVER FUNCTION IN WAYS THAT WOULD BE CONSIDERED.
Figure 1 Mean Visual Recovery (and SD) to a novel object for trials where the object was used correctly vs. incorrectly in a moving and static display.
5-Month-Old Infants Match Facial and Vocal Expressions of Other Infants Mariana Vaillant and Lorraine E. Bahrick Florida International University Abstract.
Flashbulb Memories? Memories for Events Surrounding September 11th Elizabeth Arnott David Allbritton Stephen Borders DePaul University Presented at the.
Participants  Thirty-five children completed both waves of data collection (18 female, 17 male)  1 male was diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder between.
Copyright © 2009 Pearson Education Canada7-1 Chapter 7: Social Behaviour and Personality in Infants and Toddlers 7.1 Emotions 7.2 Relationships with Others.
The Effects of Enhanced Milieu Teaching on Children with Down Syndrome
“THE PILL” AND ITS EFFECTS ON WOMEN’S JEALOUSY By: Nicole Ostlund, Mikayla Kemp, and Matt Meneses Cobey, K.D., Pollet, T.V., Roberts, C.S., & Buunk, A.P.
Development Through the Lifespan Chapter 4 Physical Development in Infancy and Toddlerhood This multimedia product and its contents are protected under.
Level 1 and Level 2 Auditory Perspective-taking in 3- and 4- Year -Olds Abstract Presented at the Psychology Undergraduate Research Conference, Atlanta,
Preschool-Age Sound- Shape Correspondences to the Bouba-Kiki Effect Karlee Jones, B.S. Ed. & Matthew Carter, Ph.D. Valdosta State University.
Positive Emotion in Language Production: Age Differences in Emotional Valence of Stories Elise Rosa and Deborah Burke Pomona College The Linguistic Inquiry.
Answers to Group Questions Brownell, Svetlova, & Nichols (2009)
Abstract The current literature on children’s recognition of faces typically uses static faces as opposed to dynamic, moving faces (e.g., Brace, Hole,
Background Infants and toddlers have detailed representations for their known vocabulary items Consonants (e.g., Swingley & Aslin, 2000; Fennel & Werker,
Chapter 10: Basic Sensory and Perceptual Processes.
Year Review Nancy Rader May 13, esearch Emotion and Working Memory Temperament Infant Perception Attention and Early Language.
Neural Activation and Attention Bias to Emotional Faces in Autism Spectrum Disorders S.J. Weng, H. Louro, S.J. Peltier, J. Zaccagnini, L.I. Dayton, P.
Method Participants Participants were 68 preschoolers, between the ages of 29 and 59 months of age. The sample was comprised of 32 male participants and.
Pavlovian, Observational and Instructed Fear Learning: Emotional Responses to Unmasked and Masked Stimuli Andreas Olsson, Kristen Stedenfeld & Elizabeth.
Early Arbitrary Object Memory May Set the Stage for Episodic Memory in Toddlers Frances Balcomb, Nora S. Newcombe, Katrina Ferrara, Jule Grant, Sarah M.
Infant Discrimination of Voices: Predictions from the Intersensory Redundancy Hypothesis Lorraine E. Bahrick, Robert Lickliter, Melissa A. Shuman, Laura.
Attachment “The formation of a strong, reciprocal emotional bond between an infant and a primary caregiver”
Audiovisual Temporal Synchrony Directs Selective Listening in Four-Month-Old Infants Lorraine E. Bahrick, Melissa A. Shuman, & Irina Castellanos Department.
Predictors of interactive competence Do individual differences in associative learning (Reeb-Sutherland) or imitation of social gestures (Paukner) facilitate.
Growing up Bilingual: One System or Two? Language differentiation and speech perception in infancy.
Social Orienting Impairment in Autism: Relations Among Look Length, Disengagement, and Symptom Severity Lorraine E. Bahrick, James T. Todd, Mariana Vaillant-Molina,
Three-month-old Infants Recognize Faces in Unimodal Visual but not Bimodal Audiovisual Stimulation Lorraine E. Bahrick 1, Lisa C. Newell 2, Melissa Shuman.
The development of timing Daniel Messinger, Marygrace Yale, Alan Cobo-Lewis, Alan Fogel, Meg Venezia, Susan Acosta, Danielle Thorp, Peter Mundy, & Tricia.
Cognition 7e, Margaret MatlinChapter 13 The Development of Language Language in Infants rate of acquisition – approx 7 words/day, birth-6 vocabulary size.
Infants’ Discrimination of Speech and Faces: Testing the Predictions of the Intersensory Redundacy Hypothesis Mariana C. Wehrhahn and Lorraine E. Bahrick.
Intersensory Perception and Attention Disengagement in Young Children with Autism Lisa C. Newell 2, Lorraine E. Bahrick 1, Mariana Vaillant-Molina 1, Melissa.
The Development of Basic Indices of Attention to Bimodal and Unimodal Social Events Across Infancy Lorraine E. Bahrick, Barbara M. Sorondo, Irina Castellanos,
1 Cross-language evidence for three factors in speech perception Sandra Anacleto uOttawa.
Abstract Bahrick and Lickliter (2000) recently proposed an intersensory redundancy hypothesis that states that early in development information presented.
Method Introduction Results Discussion Different Neurocognitive Abilities Moderate the Relationship between Affect Perception and Community Functioning.
Basic Cognitive Processes - 2
Abstract Prior research has demonstrated that young infants are able to perceive the affordance, or the potential for action, provided by the physical.
Double Jeopardy in Coping with Discrimination: Differences between White and Visible Minority Women Mindi Foster, Wilfrid Laurier University Introduction.
Bosch & Sebastián-Gallés Simultaneous Bilingualism and the Perception of a Language-Specific Vowel Contrast in the First Year of Life.
Bahrick, L. E. (1995). Intermodal origins of self-perception. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research. Advances in Psychology Series.
Infant Perception of Object-Affect Relations Mariana Vaillant-Molina and Lorraine E. Bahrick Florida International University Presented at the Society.
The Role of Intersensory Redundancy in the Typical Development of Social Orienting Across Infancy: A New Hypothesis for Autism Lorraine E. Bahrick, James.
Development of Basic Indices of Attention to Nonsocial Events Across Infancy: Effects of Unimodal versus Bimodal Stimulation Lorraine E. Bahrick, James.
The Role of Multisensory Information in Infant Attention to Faces of Speakers Versus the Rhythm of Speech Lorraine E. Bahrick, Mariana Vaillant-Molina,
Perceptions of Father Involvement on Adult Psychological Well-Being Adult Psychological Well-Being Conducted by Olivia Lane and Shakeria Burney; Supervised.
The Development of Face Perception in Dynamic, Multimodal Events: Predictions from the Intersensory Redundancy Hypothesis Lorraine E. Bahrick, Robert Lickliter,
Infants’ Detection of the Affordances of Everyday Objects Katryna Anasagasti, Lorraine E. Bahrick, and Laura C. Batista Florida International University.
Intersensory Redundancy Facilitates Infants’ Perception of Meaning in Speech Passages Irina Castellanos, Melissa Shuman, and Lorraine E. Bahrick Florida.
Reinforcement Look at matched picture after sound ends & it moves 10 trials (5 of each pairing) 2 or 4 blocks (2 pairs of words, 2 pairs of swoops) Participants.
The Development of Infants’ Sensitivity to the Orientation of Object Motion: Predictions of the Intersensory Redundancy Hypothesis Lorraine E. Bahrick.
CHAPTER 6 Socioemotional Development in Infancy Lecture prepared by: Dr. M. Sawhney.
Autism Traits in Typical Individuals Moderate Mimicry Responses to Happy, But Not Angry, Expressions Larissa C. D'Abreu, Daniel N. McIntosh Department.
Copyright © 2005 Pearson Education Canada Inc.7-1 Chapter 7: Social Behaviour and Personality in Infants and Toddlers 7.1 Emotions 7.2 Relationships with.
Gaze cues in mother-child dyads Heather Bell and Meredith Meyer University of Oregon INTRODUCTION RESULTS CONCLUSIONS METHODS REFERENCES ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS.
Emotion Knowledge in Maltreated Preschoolers
Alison Burros, Kallie MacKay, Jennifer Hwee, & Dr. Mei-Ching Lien
Chapter 7: Social Behavior and Personality in Infants and Toddlers
Susan Geffen, Suzanne Curtin and Susan Graham
The Development of Emotional Interactions Across the Senses:
Infancy and Childhood.
Dense Regions of View-Invariant Features Promote Object Recognition
Chapter 7: Social Behaviour and Personality in Infants and Toddlers
Significance of attractiveness
Presentation transcript:

INFANTS’ PERCEPTION OF FACE-AFFECT RELATIONS IN MULTIMODAL EVENTS Melissa A. Shuman & Lorraine E. Bahrick Florida International University Introduction The perception of faces and affective information emerge early in infancy. Newborns discriminate between the face of their mother and a stranger (Bushnell, 2001; Sai, 2005), and 2- ( month-olds discriminate among the faces of unfamiliar women (Bahrick, Lickliter, Vaillant, Shuman, & Castellanos, 2004). By the age of five-months, infants discriminate among emotional expressions and match audible and visible affective expressions (see Walker-Andrews, 1997 for review). When do infants detect the relation between an emotional expression and the face of the person conveying the expression (e.g. Mary is happy but Sally is angry)? This relationship is arbitrary and may change over time and across contexts. Little research has investigated infants’ detection of arbitrary multimodal relations. Research suggests that detection of arbitrary relations involving faces is evident between the ages of 3- and 6-months (Bahrick, Hernandez-Reif, & Flom, 2005; Brookes, Slater, Quinn, Lewkowicz, Hayes, & Brown, 2001). Thus, the current study investigated five-month-old infants’ perception and memory for the arbitrary relation between the face of a person and the affective expression that they conveyed. This information is important for social interaction, understanding communicative intent and understanding the affective state of individuals in differing contexts. Method Habituation Phase: Discrimination Test: Twelve infants (final N to be 24) were habituated (in an infant controlled procedure) to alternating videos of two women speaking, one with a happy emotional expression and the other with an angry expression (see Figure 1 for an example of one condition). Following habituation, two test trials depicted a switch in the pairing of the face-affect relationship. The same two women were shown speaking but the one who was angry was now happy and the one who was happy was now angry (see Figure 2). Visual recovery to the switch in face-affect pairing was measured. It was expected that infants would show significant visual recovery to the switch in the face-affect pairing if they could detect a relationship between the face of the individual speaking and the affective expression they conveyed. Intermodal Matching: Memory Test: In order to measure memory for the face-affect relationship, infants then participated in an intermodal matching procedure 2-minutes after the habituation phase. Two still images of the familiarized faces were presented side-by-side with neutral expressions (see Figure 3). A vocal emotional expression (either happy or angry) was played in the voice of an unfamiliar woman speaking the same phrase as before through a centrally located speaker. Two blocks of second trials were presented with lateral positioning counterbalanced across trials. The proportion of total looking time (PTLT) was measured to the still, neutral face that was previously paired with the vocal emotional expression heard. It was predicted that infants would show a significant PTLT to the face that was previously paired with the vocal emotional expression if they remembered the face-affect pairing. Results Habituation Phase: Discrimination Test: Preliminary results (N=12) indicate no evidence of a significant visual recovery to the switch in face-affect pairing thus far (M= 3.05, SD= 7.17, t(11) = 1.47; p >.05). Intermodal Matching: Memory Test: In the intermodal matching procedure, however, infants showed a significant PTLT to the silent neutral face that was previously paired with the happy expression (M=.58, SD=.09; t(11) = 2.99; p=.01) but not to the face that was previously paired with the angry expression (M=.48, SD=.14; t(11) = -.40; p>.05; see Figure 4). Additionally, a trend in the results suggests that infants tend to look more at the neutral face that was previously paired with the happy expression across all trials (M=.55, SD=.09; t(11) = 1.86, p=.09). Conclusions These findings suggest that 5 ½-month-old infants detect the arbitrary relationship between an unfamiliar person’s face and the emotional expression they convey and use this information later to preferentially explore the person previously associated with the happy expression. Despite the lack of significant findings in the habituation procedure, infants significantly matched the neutral face of the person who had previously displayed a happy expression with the happy vocal expression previously heard. Since the faces in the intermodal matching procedure display neutral expressions, any matching must be based on information acquired during the habituation phase. These results suggest that 5 ½-month-old infants detect and remember face-affect relations and use this information to guide their looking preferences across a 2-minute delay. This study contributes to the literature on infant perception of audiovisual emotional expressions and their detection of arbitrary intermodal relations in naturalistic contexts. References Bahrick, L.E., Hernandez-Reif, M., & Flom, R. (2005). The development of infant learning about specific face-voice relations. Developmental Psychology, Bahrick, L.E. & Lickliter, R., Vaillant, M., Shuman, M.A., & Castellanos, I. (2004). Infant discrimination of faces in the context of dynamic, multimodal events: Predictions from the intersensory redundancy hypothesis. Poster presented at International Conference on Infant Studies. Chicago, IL. Brookes, H., Slater, A., Quinn, P.C., Lewkowicz, D.J., Hayes, R., & Brown, E. (2001). Three-month-old infants learn arbitrary auditory-visual pairings between voices and faces. Infant and Child Development.Special Issue: Face Processing in Infancy and Early Childhood, 10(1-2), Bushnell, I.W.R. (2001). Mother’s face recognition in newborn infants: Learning and memory. Infant and Child Development: Special Issue: Face Processing in Infancy and Early Childhood, 10(1-2), Sai, F. (2005). The role of the mother’s voice in developing mother’s face preference: Evidence for intermodal perception at birth. Infant and Child Development, 14(1), Walker-Andrews, A.S. (1997). Infants’ perception of expressive behaviors: Differentiation of multimodal information. Psychological Bulletin, 121, Figure 1: Habituation Phase Figure 2: Discrimination Test Figure 3: Intermodal Matching: Memory Test Figure 4: Mean proportion of total looking time (and standard deviation) to the face that was previously paired with the vocal emotional expression heard. Presented at the Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting, March, 2007, Boston, MA. This research was supported by grants NIMH R01 MH 62226, NICHD R03 HD , and NSF CSLC SBE to the second author. The first author was supported by NIH/NIGMS grant R25 GM Requests for reprints should be sent to the first author at Proportion of total looking time *p <.05