Key points for lecture 4 Forgetting: good or bad?

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Key points for lecture 4 Forgetting: good or bad?
Advertisements

Core Features of Episodic Memory l (1) Memory for specific events from your past l (2) Involves retrieving the bound together contents and context (what.
PS2015 Lecture 2 Cognitive Models of Memory. Cognition Lecture 2 l Key issues where cognitive psychology parts from common sense »1. Deterministic (by.
Cognitive Science Jose Tabares Psych 202B January 23, 2006.
When Memories Go Wrong What happens when your memory of an event does not correspond to what actually happened? –In what ways can our decisions get warped.
Memory II Reconstructive Memory Forgetting. Observe this crime scene.
Memory part I Memory Distortions Eyewitness Testimony Lineup Studies.
Cognitive Psychology, 2 nd Ed. Chapter 7. Reconstructive Retrieval Refers to schema-guided construction of episodic memories that alter and distort encoded.
BHS Memory and Amnesia Memory and Reality.
Readings 25 & 26. Reading 25: Classic Memory and the eye-witness Experiment 1 Experiment 2 Conclusion Reading 26: Contemporary Misinformation Effect Memory.
Memory Prepared by Michael J. Renner, Ph.D.
 The misinformation effect refers to incorrect recall or source attribution of an item presented after a to-be-remembered event as having been presented.
Memory Games Can We Improve Memory?. Common Cents Only one of the images of a penny on the following slide is correct. Which one is it?
WHEN, WHY, AND HOW SCIENCE RESEARCH IS REPORTED IMRAD.

Chapter12 Reconstructive Processes in Memory(II) by Luan Feng.
References Arndt, J. & Hirshman, E. (1998). True and false recognition in MINERVA2: Explanation from a global matching perspective. Journal of Memory and.
- Recall / Recognition - - Forgetting.  Identify several memory retrieval processes.  Explain the processes involved in forgetting.
EYEWITNESS MEMORY and INTERFERENCE Importance of retrieval conditions –Note Encoding Specificity effects The Misinformation Effect –Loftus, Miller & Burns.
Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology
THREE MEMORY PROCESSES  Encoding – making a mental representation to be placed into memory (meaningful association)  Storing – placing encoded information.
HOLT, RINEHART AND WINSTON P SYCHOLOGY PRINCIPLES IN PRACTICE 1 Chapter 7 MEMORY Section 1: Three Kinds of MemoryThree Kinds of Memory Section 2: Three.
MEMORY & INTELLIGENCE. MEMORY: The input, storage, and retrieval of what has been learned or experienced.
Preparing to teach OCR GCSE (9-1) Geography B (Geography for Enquiring Minds) Planning, constructing and introducing your new course.
Getting you thinking: Extension: Read the ‘Apply your knowledge’ section on p55. Discuss the task with your neighbour.
Research Introduction to the concept of incorporating sources into your own work.
Preparation and practice are essential for success in your examination
Chapter One: Observation Skills
Neural representation and decoding of the meanings of words
Forgetting, Memory, construction, and improving memory
PeerWise Student Instructions
Monday Moment w/Mama Boas
Chapter One: Observation Skills
Human MEMORY.
Forensic Science.
Chapter 21 More About Tests.
CS101 Introduction to Computing Lecture 24 Design Heuristics
The Components of the Phenomenon of Repetition Suppression
Level 4 Counselling: Catherine Drewer
Learning Law Orientation: August 16, 2006.
4.3 Classic Evidence: Loftus and Palmer (1974)
Rationalism versus Empiricism
Retrieval Failure Theory of Forgetting
Post event discussion (PED) and EWT
Brainstorm… What is learning? How would you define it?
Recap of Aristotle So Far…
Office of Education Improvement and Innovation
Lesson 5. Lesson 5 Extraneous variables Extraneous variable (EV) is a general term for any variable, other than the IV, that might affect the results.
Section 1: Memory Classifications and Processes
Recap of Aristotle So Far…
Working Memory Model Tulving, 1972
Do we directly perceive objects? (25 marks)
On your whiteboard: What is innatism? Give two examples to support it
Implicit Memory SNU CSE Son, Suil.
4.3 Classic Evidence: Loftus and Palmer (1974)
Module 11 Types of Memory.
Introduction When searching for a new mattress, you have to make sure you know where to go to find the best one. The mattress you sleep on is going to.
Copyright © Allyn & Bacon 2007
Chapter 12 Power Analysis.
Brainstorm… What is learning? How would you define it?
Year 11 GCSE Revision.
Confidence Intervals for Proportions
Memory (Cognition) AP Psychology Essential Task:
Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology
Confidence Intervals for Proportions
The cognitive area.
Bruce & Young’s model of face recognition (1986)
Reconstructing Memory
Module 11 Types of Memory.
Unit 2: Memory (Cognition)
Presentation transcript:

Key points for lecture 4 Forgetting: good or bad? How does the Deese / Roediger / McDermott (DRM) paradigm work? Which factors increase or decrease false memory? What are the actual underlying (cognitive) causes of false memory? Reisberg, Chapter 7.

When Memories Go Wrong What happens when your memory of an event does not correspond to what actually happened? In what ways can our decisions get warped by an inaccurate memory? Are we always aware when this happens? The readings from Reisberg for today’s lecture are in Chapter 7 (or chapter six if you are using the first edition). The chapter ranges quite widely over a number of topics, not all of which are directly relevant to today’s lecture. So to help you, pay particular attention to the following topics from the chapter :- ‘Remembering what was said versus remembering gist’ ‘The Deese-Roediger-McDermott Paradigm’ ‘Interference: blurring of similar episodes’ ‘Rembering versus knowing’

Forgetting Is Normal! And desirable! The Case of “S” (Luria, 1968) Perfect memories, photographic memories and so on, are few and far between. For the vast majority of us, forgetting things gradually over time is a normal part of life. And in some cases, being able to forget is very welcome - as I briefly mentioned a couple of lectures ago, patients who suffer post traumatic stress disorders would very much like to be able to forget the horrible events of their past but they simply cannot. So having the ability to recall everything perfectly, in other words never being able to forget anything, is not something that you should necessarily want to have. Reisberg briefly mentions a report from the Russian Neuropsychologist Luria, about a fellow called 'S', who couldn’t forget anything that happened to him.

The Case of “S” “S” did not benefit a great deal from having a ‘perfect’ memory. Impaired ability to abstract general knowledge from his experiences. Related to his inability to forget specific details of each event? Almost the opposite of Varga-Khadem’s amnesic children. ‘S’ found it extremely difficult to construct general knowledge, and reason about the world. But, he seemed to have perfect access to all the raw data of his experiences. He could state in minute detail just about everything that had ever happened to him, and Luria was able to verify that this was the case. But despite this, S wasn’t able to abstract any general knowledge out of all of this data. Reisberg mentions Luria's report that S was so incapable of abstracting out knowledge from his experiences, that he had difficulty understanding that two different dogs, seen on different occasions, were both DOGS, could both be thought of as instances of a 'DOG'. And it seems that 'S's problem in creating general knowledge was related to his inability to forget the specific details that distinguished between each and every DOG that he had seen. So in a sense, 'S' could find something novel in every experience, and very few things would have been just familiar to him, i.e. a repetition of the same kind of event or object. In a way, 'S's predicament is the opposite to the one afflicting the amnesic children reported by Varga-Khadem and colleagues I mentioned last week. These kids seemed to have lost all episodic details, and were reliant on familiarity information as a basis for their memory judgements.

Episodic vs. Semantic Memory Baddeley’s Metaphor Our general knowledge is represented in a distinct ‘semantic’ Memory

Episodic & Semantic Components of Autobiographical Memory Parker et al. (2006), Neurocase 12: pages 35-39. (pdf available via my webpages) Case A.J. : ‘…highly superior semantic autobiographical memory’

Sources of Error in Normal Memory Forgetting. A natural feature of our memory? Recollection and familiarity may have to trade off against one another all the time. How might their interaction distort our memory of the past, and mislead our judgement? In normal people forgetting goes on all the time, which I guess you could view as a kind of error, but only if our memory was meant to be perfect. What I mean is, the fact that we forget doesn’t mean that our memories are not working properly. There may have been no need for our memories to have evolved in that way. And in fact we have seen in the case of 'S' that perfect, total recall, can interfere with cognitive functioning in other domains. So our ability to reason and categorise things might be a positive and natural consequence of forgetting. In normal people who don’t have either a photographic memory, or amnesia caused by brain damage, episodic memory and familiarity are both working, they both interact and trade off against one another, probably all the time. A great deal of recent attention in the cognitive literature on memory has focussed on how this interaction might introduce bias or distortions into all, or potentially all, of our judgements and decisions. And one reason why this area of research has attracted such interest is because of the serious consequences that distortions of memory can have in real life, in all sorts of domains. This is particularly true within the legal system, where you own future, or that of someone else, might depend upon 1) the accuracy of your memory, 2) your confidence in your memory, and 3) the validity that the court assigns to your testimony.

The Weight of Eyewitness Evidence An estimated 77,000 people annually in the USA are charged solely on the basis of eye witness evidence. Around three quarters of English cases result in conviction due to eye witness testimony (of which half were based on a single eye witness).

Introducing Distortions into Memory Force subjects to experience very similar kinds of episodes, which become hard to discriminate from one another Manipulating the familiarity of retrieval cues Two of the principle ways of confusing memory are as follows. You can get your subjects to remember lots of episodes that have very similar kinds of content. This makes the episodes difficult to discriminate from one another, and produces problems for your episodic memory as it tries to keep the representation of each individual episode distinct in your memory. A second manipulation is to vary the retrieval cues so that some of them are really familiar to the subject, even though they don’t belong to any actual episodes from the subjects past. And you can then examine whether the familiarity is fooling the subjects memory. In fact, one of the most powerful ways of distorting memory in the lab works by combining these two manipulations, making episodic memory and familiarity fight against one another.

The Deese (1959) Recall Task SOUR CANDY SUGAR BITTER GOOD TASTE TOOTH NICE HONEY SODA CHOCOLATE HEART CAKE TART PIE QUEEN ENGLAND CROWN PRINCE GEORGE DICTATOR PALACE THRONE CHESS RULE SUBJECTS MONARCH ROYAL LEADER REIGN Two of the study lists used by Deese in his original recall intrusion experiments.

The Deese (1959) Recall Task Deese constructed his lists using word association norms. Each item in a list is a strong associate of a particular TARGET word. Deese found high levels of recall intrusions by these unpresented TARGET items.

Roediger and McDermott (1995) Modified and extended Deese’s basic result. Employing recall and recognition tasks Use of the Remember / Know (R/K) procedure. The full reference is Roediger, HL, and McDermott, KB. (1995) Creating false memories: remembering words not presented in lists, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. Volume 21, pages 803-814.

The Remember / Know Procedure Ask subjects to report on their experiences while recognising. Do they ‘Remember’ any episodic details? Or do they just ‘know’ the information was encountered at study?

Recognition Test List PLACE SWEET TABLE PARTY GENERAL MEMORY CONSENSUS KING COMPUTER TREE FERRET BURGLAR BOTTLE The lure items from the two study lists earlier are shown here in red. The rest of the items are ‘new’.

Roediger and McDermott (1995) Percent Recognition Roediger and McDermott's subjects didn’t just find the target word familiar, they were prepared to state that they could recollect episodic details from a study episode where the word was presented. In fact, just as many TARGET items got a remember response as did the genuinely studied items. It looked like there was basically no difference in the level, or the kind of experience, associated with TRUE or FALSE memories. Now this is a really remarkable result. These subjects were college students, bright, educated people, paying close attention to these very simple materials who, moreoever, were warned that the experimenters were trying to trick their memory, so they should be on their guard. Yet they are claiming to remember little events that never happened.

Some Factors that increase or decrease DRM False Memory Increase: the number of associates presented for study Increase: the strength of association between study list items and their TARGET Decrease: (in young people) multiple study-test cycle. Decrease: the ‘distinctiveness heuristic’ A brief summary of some further results investigating the DRM paradigm. For a good review and discussion of the paradigm, you SHOULD make a point of reading the following paper:- Dodson, CS., Koutstaal, W., and Schacter, DL. (2000) Escape from illusion: reducing false memories. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, volume 4, number 10, pages 391-397.

‘Distinctiveness Heuristic’ Two study conditions Words from the DRM lists Words from the DRM lists paired with a picture False recognition was almost absent when words had been paired with pictures The ability to recollect picture information was ‘diagnostic’ for studied items. More recently, other researchers, notably Daniel Schacter and his colleagues, have adapted and extended the basic DRM paradigm to investigate these two accounts in more detail (you can read about this work in the Dodson et al review in Trends in Cognitive Sciences). Schacter and colleagues used pictures instead of words, finding that if you study lots of pictures, lets say of different cats, or different cars, and you then do a recognition test which contains pictures you’ve studied along with new cats and new cars, you false alarm like crazy, just like in the original DRM paradigm with words. So the DRM findings are not just peculiar to words and their associations. The level of false memory, however, drops dramatically if the subjects are given different instructions at test. So if the subjects are allowed to make a three way distinction between 'new' pictures, pictures that are 'new but from the same category as old items, and old pictures that were actually studied, they false alarm much less often. So false memory responses drop if you adopt a retrieval ‘orientation’, where you have to make a more fine grained discrimination between the test items. In other words, where you have to retrieve specific details from the study episodes, rather than just relying on the familiarity of the test items - familiarity alone could not support correct responding in this retrieval orientation.

A triple whammy! 3 Reasons for ‘DRM’ False Memory (1) Implicit associative responses subjects themselves generate the target items while studying each list. Then experience ‘source confusions’ at test (2) Familiarity of ‘lure’ items But what about the ‘Remember’ responses? (3) A loss of encoding specificity More than one explanation of these effects has been given. A popular one is that during the study phase, the subjects might think of the target item themselves, which is reasonable given that the study items were picked precisely because of their ability to bring the target item to mind through word association. So if you see more associates at study, or if they are very strong associates, then the target may be more likely to come to mind. And then what might happen at test, according to this account, is that the subjects get confused about the source of their memories, they know that they experienced the word at study, its just that they forget that it wasn’t actually on the list, they just generated it themselves. So they are confused about the proper source of their memory. They have recollected episodic details, that’s why they make R responses, but they get the fine details wrong. And the finding that false memories decrease with multiple study test cycles could be because you are simply getting more opportunities to encode specific features of the items on the study list marking them out more strongly.

The puzzle raised by false memory Within the consensus view, how is it possible to recollect events that never took place? That is, what might cause Source errors? Familiarity-based confusions? Loss of encoding specificity?

CMF Explanations for DRM False Memory The hippocampal formation Pattern separation failure at encoding Pattern completion failures at retrieval Therefore: source errors, & loss of encoding specificity The frontal lobes Strategic control over memory Failure to adequately focus on cues and/or monitor retrieval The entire ‘association’ neocortex Represents very similar content across a succession of episodes

Summary Judgements are most accurate when they are made on the basis of information whose source has been recollected. But if retrieval instructions allow it, judgements may be based, by default, upon potentially less accurate familiarity. The impression from all these studies is that you have to almost force subjects to interrogate their memories for information that is diagnostic, information that specifies the source of the memory. And if the retrieval task instructions don’t emphasise this, then the subjects appear to rely on an easier, default strategy, responding on the basis of familiarity. But in the laboratory at least, you can get past this strategy if the retrieval instructions emphasise the particular feature of the episode which is diagnostic of its source. You can also get round the use of familiarity if the encoding episode has really distinctive information that the subject can use to guide their responses. So the two important conclusions coming out of this work for the applied setting is not to let your eye witnesses rely on any easy, default use of familiarity, and second, to believe what they say only if they have recollected the source of the information they are telling you, and can state the source explicitly. But of course, even this isnt sufficient, if they confuse the source of their memories, then they will get their decisions wrong, like the DRM subjects who made remember responses to the lure items, possibly because they generated the items themselves instead of perceiving them on the study lists. And if your single eyewitness gets the source wrong there is unfortenately nothing that you can do, to verify their story independently. So to my mind at least, the importance or weight that is given to eye witness testimony, has to be balanced by this appreciation that witnesses' confidence in their recollections could all too easily be misplaced.