Infant Perception of Object-Affect Relations Mariana Vaillant-Molina and Lorraine E. Bahrick Florida International University Presented at the Society.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Attachment and Close Relationships: A Life Span Perspective Levitt, M. J. (1991). Attachment and close relationships: A life-span perspective. In J. L.
Advertisements

Chapter 5: Entering the Social World
INFANTS’ PERCEPTION OF FACE-AFFECT RELATIONS IN MULTIMODAL EVENTS Melissa A. Shuman & Lorraine E. Bahrick Florida International University Introduction.
Human Development Dancing Baby 1.
Chapter 4: Physical Development: Body, Brain, and Perception Perceptual Development By Kati Tumaneng (for Drs. Cook & Cook)
Figure 1 Mean Visual Recovery (and SD) to a novel object for trials where the object was used correctly vs. incorrectly in a moving and static display.
Chapter 6 Perception.
5-Month-Old Infants Match Facial and Vocal Expressions of Other Infants Mariana Vaillant and Lorraine E. Bahrick Florida International University Abstract.
Teachstone.com © 2013 Teachstone Training, LLC. All rights reserved. CLASS-Based Professional Development Strategies: Working with Teachers of Toddlers.
King Saud University College of nursing Master program.
Research with Infants PSY 415. General Issues Sampling –Time-consuming –Expensive –Not representative? Attrition –Fussiness –Drowsiness/sleep.
Copyright © 2009 Pearson Education Canada7-1 Chapter 7: Social Behaviour and Personality in Infants and Toddlers 7.1 Emotions 7.2 Relationships with Others.
Precursors to theory of mind? Deciding whether something is animate or inanimate Potential Cues to animacy –Action at a distance –Self-propelled –Biological.
Level 1 and Level 2 Auditory Perspective-taking in 3- and 4- Year -Olds Abstract Presented at the Psychology Undergraduate Research Conference, Atlanta,
Preschool-Age Sound- Shape Correspondences to the Bouba-Kiki Effect Karlee Jones, B.S. Ed. & Matthew Carter, Ph.D. Valdosta State University.
Abstract The current literature on children’s recognition of faces typically uses static faces as opposed to dynamic, moving faces (e.g., Brace, Hole,
Chapter 6 Early Cognitive Foundations: Sensation, Perception, and Learning.
Components of Emotion: Facial expressions Physiological factors (e.g., heart rate, hormone levels) Subjective experience/feelings Cognitions that may elicit.
Chapter 10: Basic Sensory and Perceptual Processes.
Chapter 10 Emotional Development. Emerging Emotions The Function of Emotions Experiencing and Expressing Emotions Recognizing and Using Others’ Emotions.
Year Review Nancy Rader May 13, esearch Emotion and Working Memory Temperament Infant Perception Attention and Early Language.
Chapter 6 Perception.
Orientation. Available for order and download from the Texas Early Learning Council. English Spanish Vietnamese.
Social & Emotional Development Chapter 10. Processes in Social Behavior -Social Behavior as Operant Interactions H : H Bert's Contingency: H Sees Ernie.
Method Participants Participants were 68 preschoolers, between the ages of 29 and 59 months of age. The sample was comprised of 32 male participants and.
Pavlovian, Observational and Instructed Fear Learning: Emotional Responses to Unmasked and Masked Stimuli Andreas Olsson, Kristen Stedenfeld & Elizabeth.
WestEd.org Infant & Toddler Group Care Social-Emotional Development.
Infant Discrimination of Voices: Predictions from the Intersensory Redundancy Hypothesis Lorraine E. Bahrick, Robert Lickliter, Melissa A. Shuman, Laura.
Audiovisual Temporal Synchrony Directs Selective Listening in Four-Month-Old Infants Lorraine E. Bahrick, Melissa A. Shuman, & Irina Castellanos Department.
LEXICAL LEARNING AND GENERALIZATION IN CHILDREN WITH DOWN SYNDROME Abstract LEXICAL LEARNING AND GENERALIZATION IN CHILDREN WITH DOWN SYNDROME Elbouz M.
Social Orienting Impairment in Autism: Relations Among Look Length, Disengagement, and Symptom Severity Lorraine E. Bahrick, James T. Todd, Mariana Vaillant-Molina,
Three-month-old Infants Recognize Faces in Unimodal Visual but not Bimodal Audiovisual Stimulation Lorraine E. Bahrick 1, Lisa C. Newell 2, Melissa Shuman.
Chapter 11: Emotional Development Module 11.1 Emerging Emotions Module 11.2 Temperament Module 11.3 Attachment Children and Their Development, 3/e by Robert.
WestEd.org California’s Infant/Toddler Learning & Development Foundations.
Expecting the worst often leads to poor outcomes. This process is particularly true in close relationships, as those who are most sensitive to rejection.
Infants’ Discrimination of Speech and Faces: Testing the Predictions of the Intersensory Redundacy Hypothesis Mariana C. Wehrhahn and Lorraine E. Bahrick.
Intersensory Perception and Attention Disengagement in Young Children with Autism Lisa C. Newell 2, Lorraine E. Bahrick 1, Mariana Vaillant-Molina 1, Melissa.
Chapter 4 The Development of Memory Basic laws of habituation and operant conditioning are sufficient for understanding behavioral relations called “memory.”
The Development of Basic Indices of Attention to Bimodal and Unimodal Social Events Across Infancy Lorraine E. Bahrick, Barbara M. Sorondo, Irina Castellanos,
1 Cross-language evidence for three factors in speech perception Sandra Anacleto uOttawa.
Chapter five.  Language is a communication tools whose development depends on the prior development of communication.  Language is a social tool.* 
Abstract Bahrick and Lickliter (2000) recently proposed an intersensory redundancy hypothesis that states that early in development information presented.
Lecture Outline Components of Emotions Theories of Emotional Development Emotional Milestones Identifying Others’ Emotions and Understanding the Causes.
Basic cognitive processes - 1 Psych 414 Prof. Jessica Sommerville.
Basic Cognitive Processes - 2
Abstract Prior research has demonstrated that young infants are able to perceive the affordance, or the potential for action, provided by the physical.
Contents 16.4 Imitation of intended but incomplete acts 16.5 Imitation from Television 16.6 Changing motivations to imitate in development 16.7 Echolalia.
What factors influence movement or action? Biomechanical (e.g., size, shape, mass, strength, flexibility, coordination of body/body parts) Environmental.
Bahrick, L. E. (1995). Intermodal origins of self-perception. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research. Advances in Psychology Series.
The Role of Intersensory Redundancy in the Typical Development of Social Orienting Across Infancy: A New Hypothesis for Autism Lorraine E. Bahrick, James.
Development of Basic Indices of Attention to Nonsocial Events Across Infancy: Effects of Unimodal versus Bimodal Stimulation Lorraine E. Bahrick, James.
Texas Infant, Toddler, and Three-Year-Old Early Learning Guidelines Training - Revised November 2015 Texas Infant, Toddler, and Three-Year-Old Early Learning.
The Role of Multisensory Information in Infant Attention to Faces of Speakers Versus the Rhythm of Speech Lorraine E. Bahrick, Mariana Vaillant-Molina,
Perceptions of Father Involvement on Adult Psychological Well-Being Adult Psychological Well-Being Conducted by Olivia Lane and Shakeria Burney; Supervised.
The Development of Face Perception in Dynamic, Multimodal Events: Predictions from the Intersensory Redundancy Hypothesis Lorraine E. Bahrick, Robert Lickliter,
Infants’ Detection of the Affordances of Everyday Objects Katryna Anasagasti, Lorraine E. Bahrick, and Laura C. Batista Florida International University.
Perception By: Sandra Rodriguez  Perception creates an object from sensory stimulation.  Our perceptual processes determine that certain features go.
Intersensory Redundancy Facilitates Infants’ Perception of Meaning in Speech Passages Irina Castellanos, Melissa Shuman, and Lorraine E. Bahrick Florida.
Processing Faces with Emotional Expressions: Negative Faces Cause Greater Stroop Interference for Young and Older Adults Gabrielle Osborne 1, Deborah Burke.
The Development of Infants’ Sensitivity to the Orientation of Object Motion: Predictions of the Intersensory Redundancy Hypothesis Lorraine E. Bahrick.
WP6 Emotion in Interaction Embodied Conversational Agents WP6 core task: describe an interactive ECA system with capabilities beyond those of present day.
REFERENCES Harvard University Emily Cogsdill & Mahzarin Banaji New discoveries in the development of face-trait inferences: Early attribution and behaviors.
& Results: Parenting & Line Judgments ► Parents’ autonomy scores are significantly.
Copyright © 2005 Pearson Education Canada Inc.7-1 Chapter 7: Social Behaviour and Personality in Infants and Toddlers 7.1 Emotions 7.2 Relationships with.
Methods Identifying the Costs of Auditory Dominance on Visual Processing: An Eye Tracking Study Wesley R. Barnhart, Samuel Rivera, & Christopher W. Robinson.
Emotion Knowledge in Maltreated Preschoolers
Adults’ Gestures During Object Naming and Preverbal Infants’ Word Mapping: Does Type of Motion Matter? Lakshmi J. Gogate & Dalit J. Matatyaho State University.
Susan Geffen, Suzanne Curtin and Susan Graham
CEN3722 Human Computer Interaction Displays
Title of Project School Name Miami, FL Introduction Problem Statement
Presentation transcript:

Infant Perception of Object-Affect Relations Mariana Vaillant-Molina and Lorraine E. Bahrick Florida International University Presented at the Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting, April, 2005, Atlanta, GA. This research was supported by NIMH grant RO1 MH to the second author. Requests for reprints should be sent to the second author at Abstract According to the intersensory redundancy hypothesis (IRH) infants’ perception of amodal information such as rhythm and tempo is facilitated when information is presented redundantly across more than one sense modality. In contrast, perception of modality specific information such as color or pitch is facilitated in unimodal stimulation (Bahrick & Lickliter, 2000, 2002). Previous research indicates that young infants perceive the amodal properties of rhythm and tempo better in bimodal (audiovisual) stimulation than in unimodal stimulation (Bahrick, Flom, Lickliter, 2002; Bahrick & Lickliter, 2000, 2002). The present study investigates infants’ perception of the relation between the motion of a novel toy and the contingent affective expression it elicits in an actress. Information about affect is amodal and conveyed by temporal and intensity patterning common to auditory and visual stimulation. Consistent with the IRH, it is expected that detection of affect and thus affect-object relations should be enhanced under bimodal audiovisual stimulation as compared with unimodal stimulation. Infants were habituated to videos of two moving toys eliciting an actress’ emotional expressions. One of the moving toys elicited an excited/happy expression and the other toy elicited a fearful/disgust expression. During the test trials, the object-affect pairing was reversed. Visual recovery to the change in object-affect pairing was the dependent measure. Results supported predictions of the IRH and demonstrated that infants were able to relate the toys and their actions to particular emotional expressions when the presentation was bimodal audiovisual but not when it was unimodal visual. These findings indicate that by 5 ½-months infants detect and relate the contingent affective responses of unfamiliar adults to moving objects in bimodal audiovisual stimulation. Introduction It is important for infants to perceive the affective meaning of expressions and relate the expressions to particular contexts or objects in order to guide their actions toward objects and events in appropriate ways. Most social interactions involving affective expressions occur in multimodal, dynamic, contingent contexts. Research has demonstrated that by 5 to 7 months of age infants are capable of discriminating several emotional expressions and matching facial and vocal affective expressions (Walker-Andrews, 1997). Research has also demonstrated that infants are sensitive to contingencies in early development (Bahrick & Watson, 1985; Rochat, 2001). Furthermore, research on social referencing has demonstrated that by 12 months, infants can use another’s emotional expressions to guide their behavior toward an ambiguous object or context. However, few studies have explored the precursors of this ability in multimodal contexts. One possible basis for learning to relate an affective expression and an object is detection of the contingency between the behavior of the object and the affective expression it elicits. The present study investigated whether 5 ½-month-old infants could detect the relationship between an individual’s emotional expression and the object to which it referred, when the expressive behavior was contingent upon the object’s motion. Further, infants experienced the events under bimodal (audiovisual) or unimodal (visual-silent) conditions. Consistent with predictions of the IRH, it was predicted that infants would detect the contingent affect-object relation better in bimodal than in unimodal stimulation. Stimuli Filmed events of a woman responding contingently either with a happy/excited or a fearful/disgust expression to intermittent movements of a toy horse and a toy robot (see Figure 1) were used as events. The actress looked at the toy and immediately after movements of the toy, the actress responded with the affective expression while saying “oh it moves, look it moved!” The bimodal audiovisual condition portrayed dynamic films that included the soundtrack. The unimodal condition was identical to the bimodal one except that no soundtrack accompanied the visual presentation. Procedure Seventeen 5 ½-month-old infants viewed two alternating events in an infant controlled habituation procedure. Infants were habituated to alternating films of an actress responding with a contingent happy expression to a moving toy horse and a contingent fearful expression to a moving toy robot or the reverse pairing (counterbalanced). Infants’ looking time to the films was recorded by trained observers. Once infants were habituated (a decreased in looking time by 50% of the initial looking time), two test trials were presented. The test trials were identical to those of habituation except that the object-affect pairing was reversed. Thus, if an infant had been habituated to the actress responding happily to the toy robot, and fearfully to the horse toy, then they saw the actress responding fearfully to the robot toy and happily to the horse toy during the test trials. Visual recovery to the change in affect-object pairing was the dependent measure. It was predicted that infants in the bimodal audiovisual condition would perceive the object relation and therefore would show a significant visual recovery to the test trials. In contrast, the infants in the unimodal silent condition were not expected to show visual recovery to the change in affect-object pairing. Results Preliminary results (see Figure 2) found that infants in the bimodal condition (N=11) demonstrated significant visual recovery to the change in object-affect pairing, according to a single sample t-test (M= , SD= ), t(10)= 2.571, p= Infants in the unimodal condition (N=6) did not show significant visual recovery to the change in object-affect pairing (M= , SD= 2.993), t(5) = -.244, p> 0.1. Additionally, a two sample t-test demonstrated that infants’ visual recovery to a change in the object-affect pairing in the bimodal condition was significantly greater than the infants’ visual recovery in the unimodal condition, t(16)= 2.599, p= Conclusions These results indicate that by 5 ½-months of age, infants can perceive the relation between an objects’ motion and an individual’s contingent emotional expressions in bimodal, dynamic, audiovisual displays, but not in unimodal, visual displays. The superiority of bimodal over unimodal perception of object-affect relations is consistent with predictions of the IRH and likely reflects the fact that perception of affect and social referencing emerges first in a multimodal redundant context. These findings also suggest that the failure of young infants to social reference in previous studies (e.g., Mumme & Fernald, 2003), was not due to an inability to relate a person’s emotional expression to an object. Indeed, the present results demonstrate that infants as young as 5 ½-months can perceive object-affect relations when the individual’s affective expression is contingent upon the object’s motion. This ability is fundamental to the development of social referencing. Visual Recovery (Seconds) Bimodal Unimodal Figure 2 Figure 1 Visual recovery (and SD) to a change in object-affect pairing References Bahrick, L.E., & Lickliter, R. (2000). Intersensory redundancy guides attentional selectivity and perceptual learning in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 36, Bahrick, L.E. & Lickliter, R. (2002). Intersensory redundancy guides early perceptual and cognitive development. In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 30 (pp ). New York: Academic Press. Bahrick, LE., Flom, R., & Lickliter, R. (2002). Intersensory redundancy facilitates discrimination of tempo in 3-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 41, Bahrick, L.E. and Watson, J.S. (1985). Detection of intermodal proprioceptive-visual contingency as a potential basis of self-perception in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 21, Mumme, D. L., & Fernald, A. (2003). The infant as onlooker: Learning from emotional reactions observed in a television scenario. Child Development, 74, Rochat, P. (2001). Social contingency and infant development. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic. Special Cognitive and Interactional Foundations of Attachment, 65,