Ownership and memory: the ‘Me’ is in Remembering, not in Knowing

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Do you have a good memory?. Process of memory Encoding – must be able to encode info properly – otherwise – why bother? You must store it correctly –
Advertisements

‘It’s Mine!’ Children’s memory bias for self-owned items Sheila Cunningham David Turk Neil Macrae Project funded by the European Research Council UNIVERSITY.
Background The Self: Strongly influences cognition and behaviour [1,2,3] Directs our attention [1] Triggers elaboration of information [2,3] Is this elaboration.
Method Participants 36 healthy participants (19 females) aged from 17 to 24 years (mean = 20; SD = 1,67) Material Participants were randomly allocated.
PERCEPTION DALEEP PARIMOO.
Word Imagery Effects on Explicit and Implicit Memory Nicholas Bube, Drew Finke, Darcy Lemon, and Meaghan Topper.
1 Defining “Self” The way people think and feel about themselves (Brown, 1998). The way people think and feel about themselves (Brown, 1998). The human.
Abstract Human beings are a social species and the human face is arguably the most pertinent aspect of social interaction and communication (Wilhelm et.
Consumer Information Processing MKT 846 Professor West.
The Self in a Social World Copyright © 2005 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Social Psychology by David G. Myers 8 th Edition The Self in a Social World.
SOCIAL COGNITION 1970s, label ‘social cognition’ (arises out of earlier work on attitudes, attribution, person perception) ‘…The social cognition approach.
Evolution Universals v. Diversity. Battle of Universals and Cultures Human universals: Search for unifying parameters of functioning –Emphasizes biology.
Self-Protective Memory of Interpersonal Events Margaret Wile, Angela Neal, Christine Coyne, and Edward Lemay Department of Psychology, University of New.
INTRODUCTION TO CLOA  Read about the history of cognitive psych – green book pg  In blue text, read about the principles of CLOA on pages
INFORMATION PROCESSING SCHEMA/SCRIPTS. SCHEMA - DEFINITION ã 1) Abstract Or Generic Knowledge Structure ã 2) Stored In Memory ã 3) That Specify The Defining.
Cognitive Psychology, 2 nd Ed. Chapter 1. Defining Cognitive Psychology The study of human mental processes and their role in thinking, feeling, and behaving.
Brain Electrical Activity (ERPs) during Memory Encoding and Retrieval Investigators: C. Trott, D. Friedman, W. Ritter, M. Fabiani, J.G. Snodgrass.
Does Social Neuroscience Contribute to social cognition?
Subjective reports of remembering Practical Methods 2 C82 MPR Dr Richard J. Tunney Room: 304
Cognitive Processes PSY 334 Chapter 7 – Human Memory: Retention and Retrieval May 16, 2003.
MEMORY AND ESP An Overview of Research in Light of the First Sight Model Jim Carpenter.
1 Long-Term Memory  Introduction  STM versus LTM  Episodic Memory  Semantic Memory  Procedural Memory  Encoding in Long-Term Memory  Depth of Processing.
C Pearson Allyn & Bacon Encoding and Retrieval Processes in Long-Term Memory Chapter 6.
Three forms of consciousness in retrieving memories Autonoetic Consciousness Self-Knowing Remembering Presenter: Ting-Ru Chen Advisor: Chun-Yu Lin Date:
Learning and Memory How do experiences produce relatively permanent changes in behavior? What are the principles behind the two types of associative learning,
Encoding Specificity Memory is improved when information available at encoding is also available at retrieval.
The Self-Reference Effect on Memory in Early Childhood Sheila J. Cunningham and David J. Turk University of Aberdeen, Scotland University of Aberdeen INTRODUCTION.
Social Cognition. Social Cognition is the mental process associated with the ways in which people perceive and react to other individuals and groups.
Mirjam Brady-Van den Bos University of Aberdeen Forever young? Self-memory biases are impervious to ageing.
 What are confabulators? › Have memories for events that have not been actually experienced suggesting a vivid subjective experience of false memories.
Irwin/McGraw-Hill Welcome!Welcome! Consumer Behavior and Marketing Strategy Copyright © 2001 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All rights reserved.
Consumer Markets And Industrial Market. The aim of marketing is to meet and satisfy target customer’s needs and wants better than competitors. Marketers.
Help for Thursday’s final (Internal Assessment A and B)
Strengths of the cognitive approach:
mind 1) Computation What problem was the system designed to solve?
Incidental self-reference effects in memory Sheila J. Cunningham University of Abertay Dundee, Scotland.
QUANTITATIVE MODELS OF MEMORY The value of explicit models –Precision of thinking –Explanatory power –Interval- or ratio-scale predictions –The macho factor.
Memory.
Exploring Social Psychology by David G. Myers 7th Edition
Perseveration following a temporal delay in the Dimensional Change Card Sort. Anthony Steven Dick and Willis F. Overton Temple University Correspondence.
Chapter 7 Social Perception and Attribution An Information Processing An Information Processing Model of Perception Model of Perception Stereotypes: Perceptions.
Minimal ownership: cognitive effects and underlying mechanisms Mirjam van den Bos University of Aberdeen, UK.
Conclusions  Results replicate prior reports of effects of font matching on accurate recognition of study items (Reder, et al., 2002)  Higher hits when.
Ch. 1 Consumer Behavior vs. Marketing Strategy
Brunning – Chapter 5 Retrieval Processes. Encoding Specificity Tulving & Osler (1968) –Encoding is enhanced when conditions at retrieval match those present.
Experiment 2: N = Factor Repeated Measures ANCOVA  Span Factor Score Covariate: Shortened Complex Span Tasks. 8  Conditions: Negative-Arousing.
A world of my own: The emergence of ownership effects in childhood. Sheila J. Cunningham Project funded by the European Research Council UNIVERSITY OF.
What is Psychology? Pages What is Psychology? Give me your own personal definition of Psychology.
Long Term Memory LONG TERM MEMORY (LTM)  Variety of information stored in LTM:  The capital of Turkey  How to drive a car.
CHAPTER NINE Learning, Memory, and Product Positioning McGraw-Hill/Irwin Copyright © 2004 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All rights reserved.
Levels of processing theory - Craik and Lockhart (1972).
Emma Delhaye1 & Christine Bastin1
Models of Memory Psychology 3717.
MEMORY The ability to store and retrieve information over time.
Categorical and coordinate spatial relations from different viewpoints in an object location memory task Ineke J. M. van der Ham, Jessie Bullens, Maartje.
Psychology The Study of the Mind
Applying the self in learning
Cognitive Processes PSY 334
Catherine Barsics & Serge Brédart
Word Imagery Effects on Explicit and Implicit Memory
The ability to store and retrieve information over time.
Long-Term Memory Introduction Encoding in Long-Term Memory
Models of Memory Psychology 3717.
The effect of aging on associative memory for
Direct and Generative Construction of Future thoughts:
IB Psychology Today’s Agenda: Turn in: Nothing
Introduction to Psychology
Neural Correlates Underlying The Effect of Value on Recognition Memory
Episodic retrieval of visually rich items and associations in young and older adults: Evidence from ERPs Kalina Nennstiel & Siri-Maria Kamp Neurocognitive.
Presentation transcript:

Ownership and memory: the ‘Me’ is in Remembering, not in Knowing Mirjam van den Bos University of Aberdeen 2nd September 2009

Self-reference effect Memory advantage for information that has been encoded with reference to the self How can it be explained: - self as superordinate schema (Rogers, Kuiper, & Kirker, 1977) - ‘any well-differentiated person will do’ (Bower & Gilligan, 1979) - level of processing effect (Ferguson, Rule, & Carlson, 1983) - organization & elaboration (Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986; Klein & Loftus, 1988; Symons & Johnson, 1997 ) Most of us will know what we mean by saying ‘I have a self’. The self is a fundamental construct that guides key aspects of behaviour. Most empirical work has focused on the manner in which self modulates memory function and has revealed that information is better retrieved when it is encoded with reference to the self in an explicit way. For example, your memory for the adjectives ‘intelligent, reliable, friendly’ is better if you’ve asked yourself ‘am I…..’ than if you’ve tried to think of whether, let’s say, Gordon Brown has got these characteristics. This self-reference effect has been explained in several ways. Some have argued that the self is a unique schema that influences encoding and retrieval. Others have argued that there is not such thing as a unique self-schema, but that you get memory enhancement with any well-differentiated person, for example one’s mother. Ferguson and colleagues showed that making an evaluative judgement has the same effect as self-reference and concluded that level of processing was the crucial factor. In the same vein, Klein and colleagues argued that semantic encoding and self-referent encoding would lead to the same results if the amount of organization and elaboration are controlled for.

Remembering and Knowing Conway and Dewhurst (1995) ‘self-relevant information is important and needs to be available for recollective experience’ ‘self-schemas enable integration with episodic memory’ Distinction between Remembering and Knowing (Tulving, 1985) Conway and Dewhurst recognised that it is important for studies dealing with self-memory biases to make a distinction between two different types of memory experience. In particular, they argued that self-relevant information is important to us and we need to be able to retrieve this information in rich elaborative detail (whereas information about others is not as important and we may need to know that only in a shallow way). They further argued that it is self-schemas that enable self-relevant information to become integrated with episodic memory. The distinction between remembering and knowing provides an excellent way of investigating this issue. In the remember-know paradigm, participants are asked to respond remember to recognised items if they have a specific recollection of having seen it in the study phase, that is, when they can retrieve sensory aspects of the event or thoughts and feelings they had at the time. In other words, they re-experience the event. Participants are asked to respond know if they recognise the item on the basis of a strong feeling of familiarity but have no recollective experience of it being presented.

(Conway & Dewhurst, 1995) Participants judge adjectives (self, other, valence) Remember-Know paradigm Self-Reference Recollection Effect (SRRE) Conway, Dewhurst, Pearson, & Sapute (2001) In Conway and Dewhursts experiments, participants judged adjectives either for their self-desciptiveness, or how well they described a distant other, or they rated the valence of the adjectives. In the second part of the experiment, participants performed a Remember Know task. They found that there were no reliable effects of self-referencing on overall recognition, but consistent and large effects were observed in recognition accompanied by recollective experience (but not in recognition accompanied by a feeling of familiarity). Conway and colleagues (2001) later termed this effect the Self Reference Recollection Effect (SRRE).

Activation of self-concept in a less explicit way Association between Self and owned objects (Belk, 1988; Beggan, 1991) Beggan (1992): owned objects more favourable Shopping paradigm (Cunningham, Turk, Macdonald, & Macrae, 2008) What does it tell us? What does it NOT tell us? The problem is that in research mentioned so far, self-referential mental activity has been triggered through explicit instruction; specifically, directed evaluative processing (‘are you enigmatic?’). However, what’s become clear over the centuries that the self can and does guide cognition in an implicit, unobtrusive manner. This then gives rise to an important functional question. Is it possible to get patterns of memory performance that show that the self is involved, but without the necessity of explicit self-referencing? One possible route for investigating this issue took me to the literature on the self and owned objects. Belk and Beggan showed that possessions sometimes are used to define self, as extensions of self or to make up for characteristics that one lacks. Beggan also showed that objects were seen as more favourable by their owners than by non-owners and he assumed that this was because the objects were regarded as extensions of the self and people tend to regard their own traits and abilities as more positive than those of others. On the basis of this, Cunningham and colleagues hypothesized that the impact of ownership might extend beyond evaluative processing, namely to memory, in a similar way as the self-reference effect. In Cunningham et al.’s (2008) Shopping Paradigm, two participants (one genuine participant and one confederate) were asked to imagine they had each won a basket full of shopping items, represented by cards with photos of shopping items. Each card also showed a colour cue, corresponding to one of the baskets (red or blue). The experimenter handed the cards to the participants one by one and participants were asked to put the cards in the basket with the corresponding colour. Participants were asked to imagine that they owned the items in their baskets. A surprise memory test showed that memory performance was better for self-owned items than for other-owned items. What this tells us, is that the self-concept indeed can be activated without explicit evaluation. However, what it doesn’t tell us is HOW participants recognised the items. A simple ‘yes’ doesn’t tell us much. It could be that all self-owned items for some reason unknown to the participant felt highly familiar and that’s why they pressed the yes button. Or it could be that they had specific recollections of seeing the item on the screen and this evidence made them respond yes. It’s important to know how the self-owned items were recognised, as this tells us much about what happens when you are told that you own something. Therefore, it is necessary to make the distinction between remembering and knowing, as Conway and Dewhurst did. What we wanted to find out was whether the effect they found, namely the memory enhancement for remember responses, would also occur in the ownership paradigm, in which the self-concept is activated in a much less explicit way.

Current experiment Stimuli: 150 images: - 50 self-owned targets - 50 other-owned targets - 50 distractors at test Single-factor (Ownership) within-subjects design To this end, we created a stimulus set of 150 photos of shopping items for sale in a large supermarket. 50 of these were assigned to self, 50 assigned to a fictive other and the remaining 50 were used as distractors at test. So, the within-subjects factor Ownership had two levels: self-owned and other-owned.

Participants were told that they were taking part in a shopping experiment and they had to imagine that they and a fictive other student called John had each won their own basket with shopping items. So, two baskets were presented on the screen and they were told, for example: the red basket is yours, the blue basket is Johns. (but the colour of the self-owned basket was counterbalanced across participants) The items were then presented in a new random order for every participant. Each item remained on screen for 1.5 seconds, after which a coloured border appeared around the item.

If participant owned the red basket, they were told that every time a red border appeared, this item was theirs and they had to put this item into their own basket by pressing the red button on the keyboard.

If participant owned the red basket, they were told that every time a red border appeared, this item was theirs and they had to put this item into their own basket by pressing the red button on the keyboard.

On another trial, a blue border might appear, which meant that this item belonged to the other person and they had to put this item into John’s basket by pressing the blue button on the keyboard.

Test: 2-step RKG 1: ‘yes’ or ‘no’ 2: Remember (specific memory, with details) Know (strong feeling of familiarity) Guess After all of the 100 shopping items had been sorted, a surprise memory test followed. Participants first had to make a yes no response as to whether they recognised the item, and after each yes response they had to indicate the basis of their response: remember, know or guess.

Prediction: ownership effect in recognition accompanied by recollective experience (similar to SRRE) We predicted that memory performance would be better for objects that were assigned to self, but only for recognition accompanied by recollective experience. This is similar to the self-reference recollection effect found by Conway and Dewhurst, but then under conditions that didn’t explicitly force participants to make an evaluative judgement.

Results Ownership: F(1,27) = 6.389, p = .018 The two ownership conditions had no separate false-alarm rates, therefore I’ll only focus on the hit rates. First of all, a significant effect of ownership was observed on the overall hit rates: more self owned items than other owned items were recognised Ownership: F(1,27) = 6.389, p = .018

(Results) Ownership x Response Type F(1,27) = 11.422, p = .002 Then, we applied a two-factor ANOVA to remember and know hit rates, and, we found our very much hoped for interaction effect! This interaction effect was such that an ownership effect was observed only for remember responses, so for recognition accompanied by recollective experience, but no ownership effect was observed for know responses. Ownership x Response Type F(1,27) = 11.422, p = .002 Remember: F(1,27) = 10.444, p = .003 Know: F(1,27) = 2.444, p = .130

Discussion How do the present data relate to Conway and Dewhurst’s SRRE? How about other studies that only examined ‘overall’ data? So how do the present data relate to what has been observed by other researchers? Well, as you may recall, Conway and Dewhurst found no reliable effect of self-referential encoding on the overall data. They did observe this memory bias in the remember responses, but not in the know responses. In the present experiment, support was found for this latter finding: a self-memory bias, in this case an ownership effect, was found for remember responses, but not for know responses. However, we also found an ownership effect for the overall data. This suggests that if the contribution of recognition accompanied by recollective experience to overall recognition is relatively large, as in the present study, an ownership effect can be detected on overall recognition. That is, the finding of an ownership effect on overall recognition in the present experiment can be regarded as a variation on the SRRE: it can easily be seen that as the relative contribution of remember responses to overall recognition increases, it is likely that overall recognition performance will be better in the self-owned conditions than in the other-owned condition. Previous studies examining self-memory bias, as reported by Symons and Johnson (1997) only analysed old-new (overall) recognition data, so it is likely that these studies would have observed greater effects had they made a distinction between remembering and knowing.

Spontaneous elaboration Arousal? Ecological function? (Discussion) Spontaneous elaboration Arousal? Ecological function? So what does this tell us? Previous studies, which used trait judgement tasks, explicitly forced participants to engage in self-referencing and elaboration, otherwise you cannot answer the question ‘does this trait describe you?’. In contrast, in the present study, all participants did was dividing a set of shopping item pictures into a self and other category, which does not require any elaboration. So…. the fact that an ownership effect emerged in recognition accompanied by recollective experience, implies that elaboration of the study items must have occurred spontaneously. In other words, even under conditions where the self-construct is activated only to a minimal extent, potential self-relevant information is encoded in such a way that it's easily retrievable in great detail. So how does this spontaneous elaboration happen? One possibility is through arousal that occurs when you receiving something. Previous research has suggested that memory performance is better for material that has been encoded under arousal conditions. However, it’s possible also to feel high arousal by someone else receiving something you would want (so you should have a better memory for these items too). Maybe arousal is not the best explanation, or at least, not the only explanation. Cunningham and colleagues emphasized the ecological advantage in preferentially encoding items that you own. The present data have shown that not all types of recognition are equally well suited to support this ecological function. Specifically,it could be argued that an undifferentiated feeling of familiarity towards previously seen items does not bear a great ecological advantage, as one can be equally familiar with items that belong to someone else as with items that belong to self. Specific recollection, whereby one re-experiences the event in rich, elaborate detail and where one remembers ones own role in the event is necessary to distinguish items that are previously experienced and belong to self from items that also were previously experienced but do not belong to self.

Thank you

References Beggan, J. K. (1991). Using what you own to get what you need: The role of possessions in satisfying control motivation. [Special Issue]. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 6, 129-146. Beggan, J. K. (1992). On the social nature of nonsocial perception: The mere ownership effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 229-237. Belk, R. W. (1988). Possessions and the extended self. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 139-168. Bower, G. H., & Gilligan, S. G. (1979). Remembering information related to one's self. Journal of Research in Personality, 13, 420-432. Conway, M. A.,& Dewhurst, S. A. (1995). The self and recollective experience. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, 1-19. Conway, M. A., Dewhurst, S. A., Pearson, N., & Sapute, A. (2001). The self and recollection reconsidered: How a ‘failure to replicate’ failed and why trace strength accounts of recollection are untenable. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 673-686.

(references) Cunningham, S. J., Turk, D. J., MacDonald, L. M., & Macrae, C. N. (2008). Yours or Mine? Ownership and memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 312–318. Ferguson, T. J,, Rule, G. R., & Carlson, D. ( 1983 ). Memory for personally relevant information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 251-261. Klein, S. B., & Kihlstrom, J. E (1986). Elaboration, organization, and the self-reference effect in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 115, 26-38. Klein, S. B., & Loftus, J. (1988). The nature of self-referent encoding: The contribution of elaborative and organizational processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 5-11. Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N.A., & Kirker, W.S. (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 677-688. Symons, C. S., & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: A meta- analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 371–394. Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology, 25, 1-12.

Raw data Means and Standard Deviations of Overall Recognition, Remember Hit Rate (HTR) and Know HTR, by Ownership Conditions.   Ownership Self Other Overall HTR X SD .781 .209 .746 .192 Remember HTR X .561 .221 .479 .235 Know HTR X .178 .128 .218 Guess HTR X .042 .066 .050 .060

(Raw data) Means and Standard Deviations of Remember, Know and Guess False-Alarm Rates.   False-Alarm Rates Remember Know Guess X SD .009 .018 .042 .064 .026 .040