Sided with Omaha but no Twist: Three Logics of Alyawarra Kinship Woodrow W. Denham, American University, Dubai, U. A.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Kinship Diagramming ANTH 321: Kinship and Social Organization
Advertisements

Chapter 21 Marital Residence and Kinship
Kinship and Descent. Chapter Outline  What are descent groups?  What functions do descent groups serve?  How do descent groups evolve?
interpreting kinship through marriage
Kimberly Martin, Ph.D. ANTH 250: Issues in Anthropology.
Section 1.3 Experimental Design © 2012 Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved. 1 of 61.
Section 1.3 Experimental Design.
Chapter 10 Kinship and Descent
Kinship Patterns.  Kinship is recognized differently around the world, resulting in different patterns.  Anthropologists typically use diagrams to illustrate.
Chapter 21 Kinship and Descent. Chapter Outline  What are descent groups?  What functions do descent groups serve?  How do descent groups evolve?
Lineage and Descent ANTH 321: Kinship and Social Organization Kimberly Martin,Ph.D.
Kinship and Descent Lolita Nikolova Reference: Haviland et al
Women and Families. What Is a Family? A family is a group of people who are connected to one another by consanguineal, affinal or fictive kin ties.
What is Anthropology? Anthropology is the broad study of humankind around the world and throughout time. Anthropology is the broad study of humankind.
Unit 4 – Theory and Methods in Cultural Anthropology
1 McGraw-Hill © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. O v e r v i e w Families, Kinship and Descent This chapter introduces students to the anthropological.
Families, Kinship and Descent
Rescuing Morgan from Oblivion Classifcatory Kinship Systems and Social Roles and Obligations: W.H.R. Rivers.
Chapter One: The Science of Psychology
Marriage and Family How do Anthropologists study Marriage and Family?
Kinship Diagrams. Unit Learning Objectives Differentiate between nuclear & extended families. Distinguish between family orientation & family procreation.
Statistics: Unlocking the Power of Data Lock 5 Hypothesis Testing: Hypotheses STAT 101 Dr. Kari Lock Morgan SECTION 4.1 Statistical test Null and alternative.
Fig Theory construction. A good theory will generate a host of testable hypotheses. In a typical study, only one or a few of these hypotheses can.
An Introduction to Visual Analysis Katy Gregg & Desiree Paulin Seponski QUAL 8420 March 26, 2009.
Qualitative Research.
FOCUS GROUPS ANALYSIS OF QUALITATIVE DATA
Chapter 21 Kinship & Descent. Chapter Preview What Is Kinship? What Are Descent Groups? What Functions Do Kin-ordered Groups Serve?
Kinship and Descent Chapter 10. What Is Kinship? Kinship is a social network of relatives within which individuals have rights and obligations. Kinship.
Cultural Anthropology
Power of coherence of a Turkish Nomad Clan From fieldwork to anthropological theory, 2 Ulla Johansen and Douglas R. White University of Köln University.
Chapter One: The Science of Psychology. Ways to Acquire Knowledge Tenacity Tenacity Refers to the continued presentation of a particular bit of information.
Experimental Design 1 Section 1.3. Section 1.3 Objectives 2 Discuss how to design a statistical study Discuss data collection techniques Discuss how to.
Chapter 8: Kinship and Marriage
Types of Research (Quantitative and Qualitative) RCS /11/05.
Role of Statistics in Geography
PPA 502 – Program Evaluation Lecture 2c – Process Evaluation.
Quantitative and Qualitative Approaches
The Practice of Statistics, 5th Edition Starnes, Tabor, Yates, Moore Bedford Freeman Worth Publishers CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies 4.2Experiments.
Marriage & Kinship. Kinship  Relationship between any entities that share a genealogical origin, through either biological, cultural, or historical descent.
The Practice of Statistics, 5th Edition Starnes, Tabor, Yates, Moore Bedford Freeman Worth Publishers CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies 4.2Experiments.
Chapter 9 Three Tests of Significance Winston Jackson and Norine Verberg Methods: Doing Social Research, 4e.
12.1.  Most universal institution is the family  Make up of the family varies from culture to culture  All families follow similar organizational patterns.
Cultural Anthropology
Kinship and Descent Unit 4A.
The Practice of Statistics, 5th Edition Starnes, Tabor, Yates, Moore Bedford Freeman Worth Publishers CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies 4.2Experiments.
The Practice of Statistics, 5th Edition Starnes, Tabor, Yates, Moore Bedford Freeman Worth Publishers CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies 4.2Experiments.
Section 1.3 Experimental Design.
Starter: Kinship Diagrams
What is Anthropology? Anthropology. What do you think of when you think of Canada??? What is Culture?
The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education Chapter One: The Science of Psychology.
Chapter 10, Kinship Key Terms. Kinship Culturally defined relationship established on the basis of blood ties or through marriage. Consanguineal Related.
Chapter 10 Kinship and Descent. What We Will Learn  Why have cultural anthropologists spent so much time studying kinship?  What are the various functions.
Chapter 10 Kinship and Descent
Section 1.3 Objectives Discuss how to design a statistical study Discuss data collection techniques Discuss how to design an experiment Discuss sampling.
Kinship and Descent Unit 4A.
1 9 C H A P T E R FAMILIES, KINSHIP, AND DESCENT 19-2.
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Kimberly Martin, Ph.D. ANTH 250: Issues in Anthropology
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Types of Research (Quantitative and Qualitative)
Introduction to Qualitative Research
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Presentation transcript:

Sided with Omaha but no Twist: Three Logics of Alyawarra Kinship Woodrow W. Denham, American University, Dubai, U. A. Douglas R. White, University of California, Irvine Cultural Structures and Distributed Cognition Session 2: Ethnographic Cases for Potential Formalization. Cultural Structures and Distributed Cognition Chair: Dwight Read Anthropological Sciences Meeting, New Orleans 2002 Friday 1:30-1:45

Air photo of Gurlanda Camp 1972.

The Fieldwork Experiment This paper derives from an experiment in quantitatively oriented fieldwork conducted by Denham in Central Australia thirty years ago. The experiment was designed explicitly to explore differences between what Aborigines actually did and what they said they did when anthropologists interviewed them. The attempt to discover what they did rested on observing and recording their activities, then using computers to seek and extract patterns in the coded data that would not have appeared in traditional ethnographic data.

Domestic activities.

Denham fieldwork ’71-2: data  File01 Genealogies and vital statistics records – one focus here  File02 Census data records  File03-20 digitized maps of 17 Alyawarra camps  File21 Meteorological records days  File22 Kinship term applications egos x 225 alters = 23,400 applications – the other focus here  File23 Sleeping Group Compositions records  File24 Residential Group Compositions records  File25 Task Group Compositions records  File26-27 Observation distribution records  File28-78 Behavior Observations - 41,809 records of numerically coded data derived from 200 hours of observations spanning 86 days  Documents pages of field notes and historical letters  Photographs portraits, 90 B+W prints, 230 color slides, 7 air photos  Other graphics - 3 Alyawarra Territory Dreaming maps,17 camp maps, 12 residence plans, 30 sketches  Genealogical diagrams containing all people belonging to the 17 countries represented in the research population  Audio recordings - 14 hours raw, 77 minutes edited

Some members of the research population.

This person’s use of kinship terms for others numbered is recorded here while they view other cards containing each alter’s photograph as mounted on alter’s card in the area indicated by the arrow Alyawarra fieldwork: one of the methods used is Photodeck Cards for data collection on kin-term usage

Our reanalysis Our new analysis examines the kin-term usage data elicited with the Photodeck cards on each of 104 egos x 225 alters, in relation to a network analysis of regular equivalence in marriage patterns between lineages using the genealogical and marriage data on all 264 individuals in the region studied, plus 113 ancestors. Lineages are treated as regular-equivalent if they have equivalent relations to equivalent sets of other lineages (White and Reitz 1983). We examined these data in the light of discrepant logics noted by Denham, Chad McDaniel, and John Atkins (1979) in Aranda and Alyawarra Kinship: A Quantitative Argument for a Double Helix Model. American Ethnologist 6(1):1-24.

The original and new results: an overview Denham, McDaniel, and Atkins (1979) found evidence for a Cultural Model of the Alyawarra (and Aranda) kinship system as one where –generational times were 50% longer for males than for females, –but the discrepancies of actual kin term usage and this model were about 23%, –some of which was due to use of Omaha terms that merged generations, and some to nonreciprocal use of terms. We reevaluate these discrepancies by –a network analysis of equivalence classes in marriage behavior, – locating the discrepancies in kin-term usage within the kinship network, and –deriving an analysis of the different logics-in-play from this evidence

Four Logics Operative in Alyawarra Kinship: Summarized * Denham et al. 1979: p. 12 for row 2, overall fit; p. 10 for row 4, where we find a potential W is often equated with M, and a WB with MB (the Omaha pattern of terminology). A sided marriage network is one in which marriages can be classified into two supersets such that parents of bride and groom tend to come from opposite sides. A viri- or uxori-sided network is one that is sided, with sidedness respectively assigned to males by patrilineal inheritance, or to females by matrilineal inheritance. The Alyawarra are a case in which both tendencies exist, i.e., the marriage network is both viri- and uxori-sided (Houseman 1997).

Logic0: Tetradic rules for (four-) named marriage sections are constituted by three local principles of equivalence classes amongst relatives, plus a proviso that the rules apply uniformly: 1. Equivalence of siblings but nonequivalence of cross-cousins. 2. Equivalence of alternating generations in male and female lines. 3. Marriage cannot take place between those of the same equivalence class. 4. All rules apply uniformly, and uniformly to equivalence classes (if the brothers in an equivalence class marry into another such class, then so do the sisters, and vice versa). Note that these might well be rules that hold for all four-section systems. Triangles -male sibling equivalence classes Circles - female sibling equivalence classes Marriage = =

Tetradic Logic0 (e.g., Section Memberships): Egocentric Solid lines assign relative equivalence classes both by descent rules (patrilines, matrilines) and by affinity (marriage). The equivalence classes would hold from the perspectives of sibling sets 1/2, 9/10, and 17/18,. Dotted lines show the variety of equivalence classes to which children might be assigned for women in equivalence class 4. Alyawarra section names are Kamara, Pityara, Burla and Ngwariya Kamara = Pityara Kamara Burla Ngwariya = Burla Kamara Pityara Kamara = Pityara

Section Rules (Logic0): Neither Genealogical nor Static More general than kinship: section rules are also used in ceremonials, beyond kinship proper Based on an equivalence-class logic that applies to but is not subsumed by genealogical relationships –Anthropological models of sections often infer marriage patterns from equivalence-class logics as if the genealogical marriage rules were implied by these rules when in fact they are not. –Kinship terminologies often fit the section logic and again are often used by anthropologists to imply marriage rules that do not in fact match actual behavior. –Classificatory kinship terminologies consistent with sections require careful consideration of equivalence-classes among complex bundles of genealogical relationships.

Ceremonial activities.

Kinship Dynamics of Sections: Historically, 0 to 4 to 8, in 30 years The four-section system of Logic0 was probably adopted by the Alyawarra in the 1850s from a society just to the west, at the same time as is documented for their Aranda neighbors. Ditto for an eight-subsection variant of that system, which was adopted by the Northern Aranda in the 1880s. What we call Logic1 — Models of the Kin Term System— includes Radcliffe-Brown’s model of this system. The Northern Aranda have eight named subsections, while only four sections are named among the Alyawarra, with eight unnamed subsections implicit in the kinship terminology

Octadic Logic0: Tetradic Logic0 extended to 8 subsections Historically, ca. 1880: “each of [the] sections was divided into two, so that, to a [Ngwariya] man, for example, only half of the [Burla] women were eligible as wives, the other half being Unkulla or forbidden to them” (Spencer and Gillen 1927: ). = 2Burla Original Modified

Octadic Logic0 provides a basis for a complete inventory of fit not only to the implicit eight-subsection groupings implied by informants’ designations of their first choice in appropriate usage of kinship terms applied to specific alters, but also:  Where and to what extent do violations of subsection logic correspond to irregularities in the network patterns of Logic2  To what extent usages are systematically non-reciprocal (i.e., not merely as a result of the elicitation methods) given both the network and subsection framing of Logic2  Where precisely and exhaustively Omaha terms are employed in Logic3  These questions will be the subject of a separate article. Meanwhile, partial answers are provided by our network analysis.

Artificial closure of equivalence class marriage rules in Radcliffe-Brown’s ‘Kariera’ model: one of “repeated sister exchange” Artificial closure of equivalence class marriage rules in R-B’s ‘Aranda’ model: “sister exchange in alternate generations” From Logic1  Logic2, ‘open’ format: one of many possibilities, (Denham et al., 1979 and Tjon Sie Fat 1981, 1983) Anthropological Fictions and Generalizations (Logic1)

Logic1, but a false image: Kin terms ‘fit’ R-B’s 8-subsection ‘Cultural Model’ of ‘normative’ Alyawarra and Northern Aranda kinship (Aranda-type). A good ‘fit’ of model and terminology but not to behavior: the Alyawarra have no sister exchanges, etc.

Other Generalizations of Logic1  Logic2: Network Models of Kinship Organization, attuned to age differences and strategies ‘open’ format: 0 age difference between spouses ‘open’ format: 14 year age difference (as per Denham et al. 1979) ‘open’ format: 28 year age difference Figure same as above, but steeper diagonal Logic2 is a Logic1 kin-term pattern, but now analyzed in relation to social (network) organization, not just the kin-term pattern alone

A 1 D 1 K1 N2 B 2 B1 P2 D 1 C 1 N1 K2 A 2 D 2 B2 P1 C 1 B 1 K2 N1 C 2 D 2 B1 P2 D 1 A 1 N2 C 2 K1 B1 P2 B 2 D 1 C 1 N1 K2 A 2 D 2 B2 P1 C 1 K2 N1 B 1 D 2 C 2 P2 K1 A 1 B1 N2 Key: Assuming that ego is in section K, then K,P,B,N are section designations and K1,K2, through N1,N2 are implicit 8-subsection designations. A 1, A 2, B 1, B 2, C 1, C 2, D 1 and D 2 are distinct kin terms consistent with 8 implicit subsections. Logic2?: The ‘Open Format’ proposed by Denham et al. (1979) Red= matri lines

To see if Denham et al.’s Logic2 is a network pattern We place Alyawarra lineages into regular equivalence classes in a network diagram according to whether they have equivalent relations to equivalent sets of other lineages (White and Reitz 1983), using the genealogical and marriage data on all 377 individuals. Actual patterns of marriage choice, social organization, and patterns of alliance then emerge as an element of the logic, i.e., a logic realized in the context of the social network. This is the real-time decision-making ‘realization’ of cultural models, complete with a time dimension. In the following results, we find the ‘open’ format model of Denham et al. (1979) does fit the equivalence-class patterns of actual behavior ‘roles’ in the social network.

Logic2: The actual kinship network with social organization given by regular equivalence analysis of inter-lineage marriages (key: red =mothers, black =fathers, lite marriages ) Node Colors: blue-Kamara, green-Burla, yellow-Pityara, orange-Ngwariya Node Size: proportional to age (smallest black nodes are unknown/deceased) key N=all 377 individuals

Logic2: The actual kinship network and social organization; this time the colors are those of the languages spoken by individuals Key to Languages Spoken: blue-Aranda, yellow-Alyawarra, green-in between

Logic2: The actual kinship network and social organization, with a close-up of the network of Alyawarra speakers (red and black arrows=mothers, fathers; green lines=marriages) 6 (early) red lines correspond to “open format” 28 year difference 87 red lines correspond to “open format” 14 year difference 7 red lines correspond to “open format” 0 year difference

Logic3: The actual kinship network, social organization, showing Omaha usage within the network of Alyawarre speakers blue lines=marriage, green lines=Omaha terms such as W=M, WB=MB, found to be non-reciprocal and to exclude marriage Use of Omaha terms excludes possibility of marriage

Difference between the Logics The standard interpretation of kin-term Logic1 models of Aranda-type (including Alyawarra) kinship (e.g., by R-B and Lévi-Strauss) is that of symmetric or direct exchange. The actual kinship network, where Logic1  Logic2, confirms Denham et al.’s (1979) ‘open format’ model, one of asymmetric or generalized exchange. It has: –No generational closure (no cycles of siblings-in-law) –No algebraic closure (rather there are open-ended alliances) –dynamically ‘open’ and subject to adjustments that follow from marriage strategies (not static or locked into prescriptive rules) Analysis of signaling in Logic3 is –Consistent with this dynamic, signaling non-marriageable –Non-reciprocal in use of terms at behavioral level

Further, we see these results… Kinship terminologies are egocentric But local rules have implications for the larger network structure, a point evident to participants: e.g., choices as to age of spouse co-vary with implications for reciprocity (same age) versus asymmetry (large age differences) The global network structure is the social organizational context in which to evaluate the terminologies, cultural models and decision models … as consistent with Read’s theory of kinship terminologies as applied to classificatory kinship

And that’s a good place to stop

Actor and Group Positions in Social Network Our additions to Read’s model of behavioral realization of last year Use of signaling Responses of others and behavioral effects on context

Concluding note on methods It is easy to misunderstand the logics of equivalence classes in Australian systems. Denham’s experimental field methods provided a different set of understandings of these logics. Anthropologists concerned with ‘modeling’ kinship, from R-B to Lévi-Strauss, have inferred prescriptive marriage rules from kin terms in interpreting their models. Denham’s methods also permit a network analysis that gives a better understanding. The Alyawarra case, we think, exemplify the underlying dynamics of the adaptive social organization that accompanies section systems generally. Denham’s methods might have benefited from interviews on how and when sections and subsections were introduced, how the unnamed subsections are reckoned, if at all, and informants’ reflections on how they determine the ‘Alyawarra way’ of reckoning the correct kinship term for alters.

Four Logics Operative in Alyawarra Kinship, recalled * Denham et al. 1979: p. 12 for row 2, overall fit; p. 10 for row 4, where we find a potential W is often equated with M, and a WB with MB (the Omaha pattern of terminology).

A more detailed summary In Logic1 the Alyawarra strictly adhere to marriage rules and ‘normative’ kinship terms that conform to marriage sections (cross-cutting exogamous sides) and unnamed endogamous matrimoieties that unify interleaved alternating generations. In Logic 2, the axiom of generational closure – that successive sibling-in-law links close into cycles in an endogamous group – does not apply because female age at marriage is significantly lower than that of males. The resulting extra-normative age bias yields recurrent patterns of marriage between patrilineages that are much more likely to be asymmetric than symmetric (as with sister exchange, for example). Patterns of marriage among the deceased are quickly forgotten, and no longer cast their shadow as a constraint on future behaviors. Thus wife-givers and wife-takers may engage in exceptional marriages that inflect Logic2 behaviors into new systemic patterns leading to lineage remapping of generations. This allows the “kinship system” to evolve dynamically across a class of network models influenced stochastically by age distributions at marriage. In Logic3, the unintended effect of demography (H>W age differences) is supplemented by widespread and intended use of extra-normative Omaha terminology as an exclusionary device that says, “don’t marry here”, but does so nonreciprocally.

The field data collected by Denham in are available in the Group Compositions in Band Societies archived on-line at ~drwhite/