Modelling and measuring maps of nerve connections Part A David Willshaw Institute for Adaptive & Neural Computation School of Informatics University of.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Chapter 4: The Visual Cortex and Beyond
Advertisements

EXAM 3 REVIEW Katherine & Tina. Developments of Neural Circuits Lecture 19.
Attention and neglect.
LGN, superior colliculus, and optic tectum development and innervation Chris Strang Ph.D. Vision Sciences WORB
November 12, 2013Computer Vision Lecture 12: Texture 1Signature Another popular method of representing shape is called the signature. In order to compute.
Chapter 23: Electrostatic Energy and Capacitance
Wigner-Seitz Cell The Wigner–Seitz cell around a lattice point is defined as the locus of points in space that are closer to that lattice point than to.
Neuronal Coding in the Retina and Fixational Eye Movements Christian Mendl, Tim Gollisch Max Planck Institute of Neurobiology, Junior Research Group Visual.
A saliency map model explains the effects of random variations along irrelevant dimensions in texture segmentation and visual search Li Zhaoping, University.
Perception of Stimuli Stephen Taylor.
December 5, 2013Computer Vision Lecture 20: Hidden Markov Models/Depth 1 Stereo Vision Due to the limited resolution of images, increasing the baseline.
How Patterned Connections Can Be Set Up by Self-Organization D.J. Willshaw C. Von Der Malsburg.
Module : Development of the Nervous System
1 Activity-Dependent Development Plasticity 1.Development of OD columns 2.Effects of visual deprivation 3. The critical period 4. Hebb’s hypothesis 5.
Color vision Different cone photo- receptors have opsin molecules which are differentially sensitive to certain wavelengths of light – these are the physical.
Human Sensing: The eye and visual processing Physiology and Function Martin Jagersand.
Axon Guidance How does an axon find the right target?
Modelling and measuring maps of nerve connections PART B David Willshaw Institute for Adaptive & Neural Computation School of Informatics University of.
Module : Development of the Nervous System Lecture 6 Synapse formation & refinement.
Activity-Dependent Development II April 25, 2007 Mu-ming Poo 1.The neurotrophin hypothesis 2.Maps in somatic sensory and motor cortices 3.Development of.
MEASURES OF CENTRAL TENDENCY & DISPERSION Research Methods.
Unsupervised learning
Studying Visual Attention with the Visual Search Paradigm Marc Pomplun Department of Computer Science University of Massachusetts at Boston
Active Vision Key points: Acting to obtain information Eye movements Depth from motion parallax Extracting motion information from a spatio-temporal pattern.
ONLY CONNECT David Willshaw Institute for Adaptive & Neural Computation School of Informatics University of Edinburgh
Chapter 4 : Similar Triangles Informally, similar triangles can be thought of as having the same shape but different sizes. If you had a picture of a triangle.
5. Exotic modes of nuclear rotation Tilted Axis Cranking -TAC.
1 Computational Vision CSCI 363, Fall 2012 Lecture 3 Neurons Central Visual Pathways See Reading Assignment on "Assignments page"
Nature of Science Science Nature of Science Scientific methods Formulation of a hypothesis Formulation of a hypothesis Survey literature/Archives.
3.2 VISION 70% of your receptor cells are in your eyes taste and touch need direct contact where as sight and smell don’t Sight can be experienced from.
Chapter 3 Sensation and Perception McGraw-Hill ©2010 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All rights reserved.
5th September 2013Bio Neurobiology I - Synapse and map formation1 Synapse formation Raghav Rajan Bio 334 – Neurobiology I September 5th 2013.
Self Organizing Feature Map CS570 인공지능 이대성 Computer Science KAIST.
Covalent Bonding Orbitals Adapted from bobcatchemistry.
Networks Igor Segota Statistical physics presentation.
DO LOCAL MODIFICATION RULES ALLOW EFFICIENT LEARNING ABOUT DISTRIBUTED REPRESENTATIONS ? A. R. Gardner-Medwin THE PRINCIPLE OF LOCAL COMPUTABILITY Neural.
Dr.Abeer Mahmoud ARTIFICIAL INTELLIGENCE (CS 461D) Dr. Abeer Mahmoud Computer science Department Princess Nora University Faculty of Computer & Information.
VSEPR model for geometry of a molecule or an ion
Innervation of the Eye and Orbit Part 1: The Optic Nerve and
October 1, 2013Computer Vision Lecture 9: From Edges to Contours 1 Canny Edge Detector However, usually there will still be noise in the array E[i, j],
Ch 5. The Patterning of Neural Connections 5.5 ~ 5.6 Adaptive Cooperative Systems, Martin Beckerman, Summarized by Kwonill, Kim Biointelligence Laboratory,
Genetic Analysis of Ephrin-A2 and Ephrin-A5 Show Their Requirement in Multiple Aspects of Retinocollicular Mapping Interdisciplinary Program in Brain Science.
Chapter 22 Fundamentals of Sensory Systems
Activity-dependent Development
Lecture 4 Chapter 3. Bivariate Associations. Objectives (PSLS Chapter 3) Relationships: Scatterplots and correlation  Bivariate data  Scatterplots (2.
Chapter 9: Perceiving Color. Figure 9-1 p200 Figure 9-2 p201.
Grades for Exam I BCS 249 Range: Average: 71 Before curve:
Axon Guidance How does an axon find the right target?
Artificial Intelligence (CS 370D)
Common Classification Tasks
Volume 25, Issue 3, Pages (March 2000)
Data types and representation
VEGF Gene Delivery to Muscle
Volume 48, Issue 5, Pages (December 2005)
Volume 87, Issue 6, Pages (September 2015)
Binocular Disparity and the Perception of Depth
Retinal Axon Response to Ephrin-As Shows a Graded, Concentration-Dependent Transition from Growth Promotion to Inhibition  Michael J Hansen, Gerard E.
Changing Light Waves to Neural Impulses
Retinal Input Instructs Alignment of Visual Topographic Maps
Development and Plasticity of the Primary Visual Cortex
Benjamin Scholl, Daniel E. Wilson, David Fitzpatrick  Neuron 
Ch. 5 The Patterning of Neural Connections 5. 1 ~ 5
Benjamin Scholl, Daniel E. Wilson, David Fitzpatrick  Neuron 
Lowry A. Kirkby, Georgeann S. Sack, Alana Firl, Marla B. Feller  Neuron 
Volume 102, Issue 1, Pages (July 2000)
Volume 87, Issue 6, Pages (September 2015)
Fertilization The process of haploid gametes joining together to form a diploid cell with 2n chromosomes. n chromosomes come from the male parent and n.
Grid Cells and Neural Coding in High-End Cortices
Eye: Retina and Neural Mechanisms.
Chapter 13: Item nonresponse
Presentation transcript:

Modelling and measuring maps of nerve connections Part A David Willshaw Institute for Adaptive & Neural Computation School of Informatics University of Edinburgh

2 The talk has three parts Review of my modelling work on the development of retinocollicular maps Spin off: How to measure the precision of a somatotopic map (Work in progress)‏ Spin off: Examination of the development of the olfactory nervous system

3 PART 1: Retinotectal Mapping Revisited: New data and old theories

Tadpoles 4

5

6 Frog visual system

FROG BRAIN CAT BRAIN

8 What is the mechanism for the formation of ordered maps of nerve connections? Probably more than one: 1. involving electrical activity (neighbour matching by correlated activity?) 2. involving molecular recognition (chemoaffinity?)‏ From Jacobson (1967)‏

Assorted evidence to be accounted for that indicates: 9 Flexibility of connection pattern – usually associated with neural activity based mechanisms Rigidity of connection pattern – usually associated with molecular based mechanisms

Regeneration studies in adult goldfish - molecular recognition involving distinct immutable labels? 10

Visuotectal map in stage Xenopus 11

Map in stage 55 Xenopus 12

Visuotectal map in postmetamorphic Xenopus – spreading of connections to fill available space? 13

Evidence from surgically constructed compound eyes Gaze, Straznicky, 1970s- 1980s - JEEM-> Development) Continuous map from mirror symmetric compound-eye

Discontinuous maps from point-symmetric compound-eyes 15

16 Mechanism 1: correlated neural activity According to the correlated neural activity model, spontaneous electrical activity drives the process. By a Hebbian-type mechanism, connections between neighbouring retinal cells and neighbouring tectal cells are strengthened; those between non-neighbours are weakened (Willshaw and von der Malsburg 1976).

17 Mechanism 2, molecular recognition: Sperry’s chemoaffinity hypothesis (1943)

18 Sperry’s chemoaffinity hypothesis (1943)

19 Sperry’s chemoaffinity hypothesis (1943)

20 Modern evidence for molecular labels from McLaughlin, Hindges and O’Leary (2003)‏

21 But are these molecules used in map-making? If Ephs and ephrins are the labels of chemoaffinity, then changing them should result in abnormal patterns of connectivity I have analysed the abnormal retinocollicular maps in mouse after knockin and knockout of specific EphA receptors in the retina

Mouse – retinocollicular projection The EphA3 receptor is knocked in to 50% of the retinal ganglion cells, dispersed over the entire retina (Brown et al., Cell (2000), 102, 77-88) In the map, the retina is projected twice over the colliculus, once over the front and once over the back The EphA3+ cells form one projection, the normal cells the other 22

23 Distribution of EphA receptors across the normal retina (Reber, Burrola & Lemke, Nature, October 2004)‏

24 Reber et al. (2004) made 6 combinations of EphA3 knockin with EphA4 knockout The distributions of total EphA density over the nasotemporal axis of the retina for each of the 6 cases: Black: for EphA3- cells Green: for EphA3+ cells

Reber et al., Nature 2004 combined EphA3 knockin with EphA4 knockout, in each case obtaining a similar pattern of maps I showed that in all these maps, the distribution of EphA receptor making up the gradient of EphA across the retina and present in the axonal terminals is distributed across the colliculus as a gradient from rostral to caudal Regardless of the absolute values forming the gradient 25

26 The retinal induction model (Willshaw, 2006; after Willshaw and von der Malsburg, 1979) provides a mechanism for this A Law of Retinocollicular Map-Making: For the map of nasotemporal retina on rostrocaudal colliculus, axons arrange themselves on the colliculus according to the relative amount of EphA they carry: Temporal axons (highest EphA density) go to rostral colliculus Nasal axons (lowest EphA density) go to caudal colliculus Axons with intermediate amounts of EphA go to intermediate positions

27 Rolling back the clock 30 years: we knew then that if map-making employs label matching, then label flexibility is required

28 Our solution back then: the Marker Induction model (Willshaw and von der Malsburg, 1979)‏ The retina carries fixed labelsThe retina carries fixed labels A very rough map is formed initiallyA very rough map is formed initially Molecules are induced into the tectum through each synapse formed, in amounts characteristic of the axon’s cell of originMolecules are induced into the tectum through each synapse formed, in amounts characteristic of the axon’s cell of origin Exchange of molecules between tectal cellsExchange of molecules between tectal cells Modification of synapsesModification of synapses Simultaneous refinement of tectal molecular distributionSimultaneous refinement of tectal molecular distribution

29 The Marker Induction model Initially there are labels in the retina and very weak labels, or none, in the target. During development, simultaneously the set of fixed retinal labels is copied onto the target AND an ordered projection develops.

30 According to the marker induction idea, chemical labels are induced onto the target structure from the retina through the synaptic contacts made The pattern of labels across the retina is duplicated in the target

31 Reber et al. (2004) made 6 combinations of EphA3 knockin with EphA4 knockout The distributions of total EphA density over the nasotemporal axis of the retina for each of the 6 cases: Black: for EphA3- cells Green: for EphA3+ cells

32 How marker induction produces the double maps seen by Brown et al, 2000

33 In conclusion of part 1 The prevailing view of axons are guided initially by molecular signals to form a very rough map and then the map is formed by correlated neural activity. However, according to a 30 year old model, a mechanism of correlated neural activity is not required. Axons can sort themselves out according to the amount of label they carry. Many predictions: eg, the distribution of labels in colliculus will be different in each of the six knockins/outs discussed.

34 PART 2 Spin off: measuring maps

35 Read all about this in: Willshaw (2006). Development, 133, Willshaw & von der Malsburg (1979). Phil Trans Roy Soc B, 287,

36 Since more than one mechanism is involved in map making, testing out the effect of one mechanism (eg, by genetic or surgical interference) will produce disordered maps A method is needed to measure the order in such maps and hence assess the role played by the mechanism disrupted The basic idea is to treat local and global order separately

37 Since more than one mechanism is involved in map making, testing out the effect of one mechanism (eg, by genetic or surgical interference) will produce disordered maps A method is needed to measure the order in such maps and hence assess the role played by the mechanism disrupted The basic idea is to treat local and global order separately

38 My goal is to provide quantitative measures of the local and global order in any map I deal here with maps defined by the projection of a given number of points from one space onto the other

39 My method is based on the fact that in an ordered mapping, neighbourhood relations are preserved Establish which of the members of the set of points in one of the spaces are neighbours – say, the points in the tectum/colliculus Find the precision in which these neighbours map onto points in the other space (here, visual field). Find how individual parts of the map are oriented and in what overall orientation

40 1) Definition of neighbours in tectum Given a set of points distributed over the tectum: Use a Delaunay triangulation to define nearest neighbours. 23 points spread over the tectumLines join nearest neighbours

41 Given the set of points in the visual field corresponding to the given set of tectal points: Join up each pair of field points for which the related tectal points are nearest neighbours For each corresponding pair of field and tectal links, calculate the ratio of their lengths (after scaling map areas)‏

42 2) Quantification of Map Precision In an ideal map, the lattice formed in the tectum will be reproduced exactly in the visual field, and so each ratio of link lengths will all be 1 A single peak in the distribution of length ratios indicates homogeneity The standard deviation measures the orderliness of the map

43 Counting the number of links that cross In a completely ordered map, the tectal lattice is reproduced perfectly in the visual field, with no links crossing In a disordered map, the number of links in the projected graph that cross over each other is a direct measure of (dis)order

44 A partially disordered map – adult Xenopus 23 points, 53 crossings

45 Distribution of NN link lengths: 23 points (disordered)‏ Mean: 1.56 Std Dev: 0.96

46 3) Quantification of Map Orientation Compare the orientations of each of the sides of the matching Delaunay triangles

47 Quantification of Map Orientation Look at the distribution of all such angles