Knower-Levels and the Acuity of the Approximate Number System James Negen and Barbara W. Sarnecka University of California, Irvine

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Method Participants 184 five-year-old (M age=5.63, SD=0.22) kindergarten students from 30 classrooms in central Illinois Teacher ratings The second edition.
Advertisements

Comparing Two Proportions (p1 vs. p2)
KRUSKAL-WALIS ANOVA BY RANK (Nonparametric test)
The Psychological Well-Being of “Seeking” and “Achieving” Purpose in Adulthood Patrick L. Hill, Jessica Collado, Anthony Burrow, Jay Brandenberger, & Daniel.
February 9 th, 2010 Psychology 485.  Introduction Different levels of numerical competence, Why learn?  How are numbers learned and processed?  What.
INFERENTIAL STATISTICS. Descriptive statistics is used simply to describe what's going on in the data. Inferential statistics helps us reach conclusions.
Fractions, Decimals and Percentages Part 1
Procedure Baseline participants completed the category fluency task without seeing the video clip. Results Visual Acuity Young adults had better visual.
Number (Notes and Talking Points). Two Different Views The knowledge of mathematical things is almost innate in us … for layman and people who are utterly.
Everything you ever wanted to know about Intelligence, but were afraid to ask! Carolyn R. Fallahi, Ph. D.
Audiovisual Emotional Speech of Game Playing Children: Effects of Age and Culture By Shahid, Krahmer, & Swerts Presented by Alex Park
Chapter 10: Hypothesis Testing
Analysis of frequency counts with Chi square
Chapter 3 Producing Data 1. During most of this semester we go about statistics as if we already have data to work with. This is okay, but a little misleading.
Brooke McDowell and Yeshim Iqbal.  Two Core Systems of Numbers Parallel individuation Precisely keeping track of discrete objects “Cracker Study” (Feigenson,
The Problem of Transposition Behavioral Versus Cognitive Views of Learning Key Issue in Learning Theory: What is Learned? Behavioral View: An association.
BPS - 3rd Ed. Chapter 131 Confidence intervals: the basics.
SW388R7 Data Analysis & Computers II Slide 1 Multiple Regression – Basic Relationships Purpose of multiple regression Different types of multiple regression.
Relationships Among Variables
Does learning to count involve a semantic induction? Kathryn Davidson, Kortney Eng, David Barner.
1 CSI5388 Data Sets: Running Proper Comparative Studies with Large Data Repositories [Based on Salzberg, S.L., 1997 “On Comparing Classifiers: Pitfalls.
Helping Your Child with Their Maths at Home Infant Maths Evening.
Brooke McDowell and Yeshim Iqbal.  Two Systems of Knowing Numbers Parallel individuation Precisely keeping track of discrete objects “Cracker Study”
Categorical Data Prof. Andy Field.
Statistical Techniques I
Chapter 5 Sampling Distributions
Helping Your Child with Their Maths at Home
Participants and Procedure  Twenty-five older adults aged 62 to 83 (M = 70.86, SD = 5.89).  Recruited from St. John’s and surrounding areas  56% female.
The Psychology of the Person Chapter 2 Research Naomi Wagner, Ph.D Lecture Outlines Based on Burger, 8 th edition.
 Closing the loop: Providing test developers with performance level descriptors so standard setters can do their job Amanda A. Wolkowitz Alpine Testing.
Stats 95.
Statistical analysis Prepared and gathered by Alireza Yousefy(Ph.D)
Essential Statistics Chapter 131 Introduction to Inference.
INTRODUCTION TO INFERENCE BPS - 5th Ed. Chapter 14 1.
CHAPTER 14 Introduction to Inference BPS - 5TH ED.CHAPTER 14 1.
Cognitive Information Processing Dr. K. A. Korb University of Jos.
BDI Training. Agenda General Information about BDI Administering the BDI Scoring the BDI Watch BDI on 2 year old – score protocol.
Introductory Topics PSY Scientific Method.
ANS Acuity and Learning Number Words from Number Books and Games James Negen, Meghan C. Goldman, Tanya D. Anaya and Barbara W. Sarnecka University of California,
Distributions of the Sample Mean
Introduction The authors of this research would like to thank the University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire’s Office of Research and Sponsored Programs for financial.
From Bad to Worse: Variations in Judgments of Associative Memory Erin Buchanan, Ph.D., Missouri State University Abstract Four groups were tested in variations.
DIRECTIONAL HYPOTHESIS The 1-tailed test: –Instead of dividing alpha by 2, you are looking for unlikely outcomes on only 1 side of the distribution –No.
CHAPTER 4 – RESEARCH METHODS Psychology 110. How Do We Know What We Know? You can know something because a friend told you You can know something because.
© 2008 McGraw-Hill Higher Education The Statistical Imagination Chapter 11: Bivariate Relationships: t-test for Comparing the Means of Two Groups.
Research Methods Chapter 2.
Major Science Project Process A blueprint for experiment success.
Theory of Mind and Executive Functioning: Dual Task Studies Claire Conway, Rebecca Bull & Louise Phillips School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen,
Ring ON Pole Cup ON Saucer Telephone ON Wall Moustache ON Face Lady ON TV IM? UM? AUF? AN?
Processing Faces with Emotional Expressions: Negative Faces Cause Greater Stroop Interference for Young and Older Adults Gabrielle Osborne 1, Deborah Burke.
Understanding Numbers
Tests of Significance We use test to determine whether a “prediction” is “true” or “false”. More precisely, a test of significance gets at the question.
Chapter 7: Hypothesis Testing. Learning Objectives Describe the process of hypothesis testing Correctly state hypotheses Distinguish between one-tailed.
Mathematics Section Numbers and Operations Measurement Data Interpretation Algebra Calculators are not allowed on the test!
Does the brain compute confidence estimates about decisions?
Day care refers to care for children under school age, generally at a day nursery or by a childminder It does NOT refer to full time (i.e. 24-hour-a-
Copyright © 2010, 2007, 2004 Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter 21 More About Tests and Intervals.
Training Class Inclusion in Individuals with Autism
Logistic Regression APKC – STATS AFAC (2016).
Bowden, Shores, & Mathias (2006): Failure to Replicate or Just Failure to Notice. Does Effort Still Account for More Variance in Neuropsychological Test.
Measures of Association
Categorical Data Aims Loglinear models Categorical data
Chapter 5 Sampling Distributions
PERFORMANCE AND TALENT MANAGEMENT
Essential Statistics Introduction to Inference
Erin M. Buchanan, Katherine D. Miller, Emily R. Klug, and the DOOM Lab
Chapter 10: Intelligence & Testing
Multiplication and Division
MGS 3100 Business Analysis Regression Feb 18, 2016
Presentation transcript:

Knower-Levels and the Acuity of the Approximate Number System James Negen and Barbara W. Sarnecka University of California, Irvine Abstract The approximate number system (ANS) – also known as the analogue magnitude system, the ‘number sense’, and the ‘number module’ – is a cognitive system that provides nonverbal estimates of the number of items in a stimulus. Adults can map these estimates to number words (e.g., Dehaene, 1997). Some theories say that the ANS provides numeric meaning for the number words from the beginning (e.g., Gelman & Gallistel, 1992); others say that cardinal number-word meanings are not initially connected to the ANS (Carey, 2009; Le Corre & Carey, 2007). Knower-levels are a way of classifying children based on what number words they know. 1-knowers knows ‘one’, 2-knowers know ‘one’ and ‘two’, etc., and CP-knowers know how to assign meanings to number words through counting. Wagner and Johnson (2011) argue that the child’s progress at learning the first few number-word meanings is linked to her ANS acuity. Our findings here do not support this conclusion. Methods Results “Can you give Mr. Bunny TWO lemons?” Give-N Task Diagnoses a child’s Knower-Level (i.e., what number words the child knows) Experimenter asks the child to give a certain number of items to a stuffed animal Requested number starts small, increases with correct answers and decreases with incorrect ones Example to left: a child, asked to give two lemons, puts three on a plate (left frame) and slides them to the animal (right frame) “Which side has MORE dots?” Set-Comparison Task Measures a child’s ANS Acuity Experimenter asks the child “Which side has MORE dots?” First ratios are very easy (3:1). The child may not go on until 8 very easy trials in a row are correct. Ratios get progressively harder (until 10:9) Half are area-correlated, half are area-anticorrelated (example left) so that non-numeric cues are not useful Feedback given after every trial (e.g., “Yes, that’s right.”) Background Discussion The set-comparison task measures ANS acuity. Age is a significant predictor of set-comparison performance (p <.001), as is knower-level (p =.018). However, knower-level is not correlated with set- comparison performance when partialled for age, r(55) =.091, p =.251. We have not found a statistically significant relationship between ANS acuity and knower- level when age is controlled. There are at least three possible interpretations of these results: First, these findings could point away from the conclusions of Wagner & Johnson (2011), who suggest that ANS acuity predicts number-word knowledge. Our method of measuring ANS acuity may explain the difference in results: We used dots in each set (as apposed to ¼ of trials having only 1-4 items), excluding the use of parallel individuation. We had area-correlated and -anticorrelated trials (not just area-equated trials), so overall above-chance performance must rely on numeric perceptions. Studies that just use area-equated trials can’t distinguish a child with high ANS acuity from a child that just usually picks the smaller dots. Our participants had to demonstrate understanding of the instructions before training ended, and continued to receive feedback. This insures that any observed effects are not driven by differences in understanding the instructions. Second, since most participants have been 3-knowers or higher, it is possible that more 1-knowers and 2-knowers are needed to see the effect. Finally, since 46/58 children performed better on area-correlated trials than area- anticorrelated trials, it is possible that a proper analysis needs to separate the child’s ANS acuity from her ability to ignore irrelevant information. Special thanks to Jessica Sullivan and David Barner for providing an alternative version of the Set-Comparison Task for us to adapt. 46 children performed better on area-correlated trials while 10 performed better on area-anticorrelated trials (p <.001, binomial). The average absolute difference was 27%. This may indicate a need for an analysis method that estimates acuity separately from bias towards the larger (or smaller) set. Note that this occurred despite the fact that all children got 8 cards correct in a row at the 3:1 ratio, which included 4 area- anticorrleated trials. Also, children under 5 years old performed above chance on area-anticorrelated trials (1616/2880, p <.001), contrasting with Soltész, Szűcs & Szűcs (2011). Here we present a study of 58 children (2;4 to 7;6) that examines the relationship between number-word knowledge and the acuity of the approximate number system (ANS). In contrast to other recent findings (Wagner & Johnson, 2011), we find no significant relationship between ANS acuity and number- word knowledge when age is controlled. This suggests that the number-word learning process is not driven by improvements in ANS acuity. Future work on this study will (1) continue to collect more data and (2) develop methods of analysis that accounts for the child’s impulse to simply pick the set with greater area.