PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 – Theories of Conditioning.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Classical Conditioning II
Advertisements

Theories of Learning Chapter 4 – Theories of Conditioning
Facebook Group: The group is called: Psych281 Spring08 Available only to University of Alberta network Sorry to be rude but… Please don’t add me as a friend.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 – Theories of Conditioning.
Siegel, 1976 Demonstration of addiction, tolerance and withdrawal or Cues are EVERYWHERE!
PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 9, Theories and Applications of Aversive Conditioning.
Spontaneous Recovery of Responding following Forward and Backward Blocking Oskar Pineño, Kouji Urushihara and Ralph R. Miller State University of New York.
Theories of Classical Conditioning
Factors Influencing Respondent & Operant Learning: Part 2 Lesson 10.
Psychology 485 September 28,  Introduction & History  Three major questions: What is learned? Why learn through classical conditioning? How does.
Inhibitory Pavlovian Conditioning Stimuli can become conditioned to signal the absence of a US— such learning is called Inhibitory Conditioning CS+ = excitatory.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 9 – Motivation.
Lectures 7&8: Pavlovian Conditioning (Determining Conditions) Learning, Psychology 5310 Spring, 2015 Professor Delamater.
PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 8 – Aversive Conditioning.
Avoidance Conditioning Combining Classical and Operant Conditioning Classical and operant conditioning often take place in the same situation. We saw this.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 5 – The Role of Conditioning in Behavior.
Rescorla-Wagner (1972) Theory of Classical Conditioning.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning
Rescorla's Correlation *Experiments * Note that Rescorla referred to his experiments as contingency experiments, however since a true contingency (cause-effect.
Contingency Theory of Classical Conditioning
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 – Theories of Conditioning.
Learning What is Learning? –Relatively permanent change in behavior that results from experience (behaviorist tradition) –Can there be learning that does.
PSY402 Theories of Learning Wednesday, November 19, 2003 Chapter 6 -- Traditional Theories (Cont.)
PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 (Cont.) Indirect Conditioning Applications of Conditioning.
WHS AP Psychology Unit 5: Learning (Behaviorism) Essential Task 5-2: Describe basic classical conditioning phenomena with specific attention to unconditioned.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 9 – Motivation.
PSY402 Theories of Learning Wednesday January 15, 2003.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 3 – Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)
Innate Knowledge (what an organism is born with) Experience leads to changes in knowledge and behavior Learning refers to the process of adaptation Of.
Principles of Behavior Change Classical Conditioning.
Theoretical Analysis of Classical Conditioning Thomas G. Bowers, Ph.D. Penn State Harrisburg.
CHAPTER 4 Pavlovian Conditioning: Causal Factors.
Psychology 2250 Last Class Characteristics of Habituation and Sensitization -time course -stimulus-specificity -effects of strong extraneous stimuli (dishabituation)
Current Theoretical Approaches and Issues in Classical Conditioning Psychology 3306.
Innate Behavior Patterns Reflex Tropism –kinesis (undirected) –taxis (directed) Fixed Action Pattern –species-specific; unlearned; goes to completion Reaction.
Classical Conditioning: Mechanisms The general outline for this section: I. What makes for an effective CS and/or US? II. What is learned in classical.
Classical Conditioning Underlying Processes and Practical Application.
Learning Experiments and Concepts.  What is learning?
Experimental Evidence  Rats drink little saccharin water at first but increase over time.  Loud tones (110 db) produce different responses depending.
Innate Knowledge (what an organism is born with) Experience leads to changes in knowledge and behavior Learning refers to the process of adaptation Of.
Factors Influencing Conditioning  CS and US Intensity, and Attention to the CS  Temporal relationship  Predictiveness  Preparedness  Redundancy 1.
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 – Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)
Lectures 9&10: Pavlovian Conditioning (Major Theories)
Blocking The phenomenon of blocking tells us that what happens to one CS depends not only on its relationship to the US but also on the strength of other.
Current Theoretical Approaches and Issues in Classical Conditioning Psychology 3306.
PSY402 Theories of Learning Friday January 17, 2003.
Copyright © Allyn and Bacon Chapter 6 Learning This multimedia product and its contents are protected under copyright law. The following are prohibited.
An Introduction to THEORIES of LEARNING CHAPTER An Introduction to Theories of Learning, Ninth Edition Matthew H. Olson | B. R. Hergenhahn Copyright ©
PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 3 – Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)
Rescorla-Wagner Model  US-processing model  Can account for some Pavlovian Conditioning phenomena: acquisition blocking unblocking with an upshift conditioned.
The Rescorla-Wagner Model
PSY402 Theories of Learning
PSY402 Theories of Learning
Classical Conditioning
Unit 6: Learning (Behaviorism)
PSY 402 Theories of Learning
PSY402 Theories of Learning
Classical Conditioning and prediction
Factors Influencing Respondent & Operant Learning
PSY402 Theories of Learning
Unit 5: Learning (Behaviorism)
Learning (Behaviorism)
Contingency Theory of Classical Conditioning
Pavlovian Conditioning: Mechanisms and Theories
PSY402 Theories of Learning
PSY402 Theories of Learning
PSY 402 Theories of Learning
PSY402 Theories of Learning
PSY402 Theories of Learning
Presentation transcript:

PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 – Theories of Conditioning

Rescorla-Wagner Model  Classical conditioning occurs only if the US (UCS) is surprising to the organism. If the UCS is already predicted by a CS, then it is not surprising – it is expected. When the CS predicts the UCS perfectly, no further learning occurs. The asymptote (lambda, ) is the point where the learning levels off (no increase in learning occurs).

Parts of the Model   V =  ( – V)  V is the Associative Strength (amount of learning).   V is the change in learning (increase in Associative Strength.   and  are the salience of the CS and UCS  – V is the surprisingness of the US (the distance away from the asymptote).

Multiple Conditioned Stimuli (CS’s)  The basic model explains changes in learning with one UCS and one CS. This doesn’t explain what happens during blocking and unblocking, with multiple CS’s.   V =  ( – ΣV)  When multiple CS’s are present,  V is the sum of the associative strengths of all of the CS’s (such as V N + V L ).

Blocking  First a noise is conditioned so that V N = 1.0  Next a light is added. The formula predicts its associative strength:  V L =  ( – ΣV) ΣV = V N + V L  If we assume that  and V N is 0 because no learning has occurred yet, then:  V L =.2[1.0 – ( )] = 0

Unblocking  As before, a noise is conditioned so that V N = 1.0  A stronger US is presented with the new CS (V L ).  As before, the formula predicts its associative strength:  V L =  ( – ΣV) ΣV = V N + V L  Again, we assume that  and V N is 0 but now the stronger US is 2.0 instead of 1.0:  V L =.2[2.0 – ( )] =.2[1.0] =.2

Extinction  During extinction, the CS is presented without the UCS. This is the same as presenting a UCS with intensity = 0.  The formula predicts the associative strength during extinction:  V N =  ( – V) but is now 0 (due to extinction)  V N =.2[0 – 1] = -.2  The associative strength is decreasing. Use the decreased value for V N (1-.2) for the next trial.

Inhibition  During inhibition, a second CS L is presented that has never been associated with the UCS (V = 0). The formula predicts the associative strength for both CS’s:  V N =  ( –  V) and  V L =  ( –  V)  V N =.2[0 – ( )] = -.2  V L =.2[0 – ( )] = -.2  V = V N + V L.

Protection from Extinction  When extinction of an excitor takes place together with extinction of an inhibitor, the excitor is never fully extinguished. This is called protection from extinction.  To fully extinguish an excitor, and to extinguish it faster, pair it with another excitor (another CS associated with the US).  The model predicts both of the these results.

Overexpectation Effect  The value of a model is that it predicts new findings.  If you pair two previously conditioned CS’s (excitors) on the same trial, V for each will decrease until  V equals. This is because  V “overexpects” the UCS.  Similarly, if a new CS (X) is added to the pair, it will become an inhibitor.

Contextual Cues  Contextual cues consist of everything in the environment in addition the CS and UCS. They cannot be ignored simply because the experimenter is not manipulating them.  Whenever a CS or a UCS appears “alone,” it is still being paired with the context.  When the context is considered another CS, then ideas about blocking explain learning. Zero contingency occurs because context is blocked.

Comparator Theories  An alternative theory to Rescorla-Wagner proposes that the CS and UCS are associated and the UCS and context are associated.  The two sets of associations are compared to determine the amount of responding to the CS. The comparison determines the responding, not the learning. Strengthening or weakening the context, after learning, affects the amount of responding, supporting the theory.

Problems with Rescorla-Wagner  It predicts that presenting an inhibitory CS without the UCS should lead to extinction, but it doesn’t.  The model cannot account for latent inhibition (preexposure to the CS).  Mackintosh demonstrated that animals learn to ignore redundant stimuli – the model doesn’t predict this learning.

The Mackintosh Model  Mackintosh proposed that the amount of learning depends on how much attention the animal pays to the CS.  The attention to the CS is the  term in the Rescorla-Wagner model.  Alpha increases when the CS is the best predictor and conditioning occurs to the best predictor of the UCS.

Criticisms of the Mackintosh Model  The model does a good job of explaining latent inhibition and its own criticisms of Rescorla-Wagner, but other problems arose.  While attention is important, it doesn’t necessarily increase when a CS becomes the best predictor. Hall & Pearce showed that preexposure to a tone that was a good predictor of weak shock didn’t help learning when a stronger shock was used.

Conditioned Inhibition  A CS can signal the presence of a UCS. This is called excitation, CS +  A CS that never appears with the UCS signals the absence of the UCS. It becomes an “all clear” signal. This is called inhibition, CS -  In fear conditioning an excitor produces anxiety, an inhibitor produces relief or safety.

Pearce Hall Model  Animals don’t waste attention on stimuli whose meaning is already well understood. Instead, they devote attention to understanding new stimuli.  For their model, the value of alpha depends on how surprising the UCS was on the previous trial. If the UCS is surprising, the CS is not well understood. Alpha is high when this occurs.