Discussion Future Research Although there was no statistical significance between prelinguistic and linguistic levels of communicative acts, canonical.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Optimizing Outcomes for Early Identified Children Through Inclusive Service Provision Sarah Wainscott & Marion Helfrich The River School Washington, D.C.
Advertisements

Colorado School for the Deaf and the Blind November 13, 2013
Maternal Psychological Control: Links to Close Friendship and Depression in Early Adolescence Heather L. Tencer Jessica R. Meyer Felicia D. Hall University.
Pediatric Chronic Pain and Differences in Parental Health-Related Quality of Life Gustavo R. Medrano¹, Susan T. Heinze¹, Keri R. Hainsworth 2,3, Steven.
Social inclusion of young children with Autistic Spectrum Disorder in Australian early childhood programs Sue Walker and Donna Berthelsen Queensland University.
Assessment Considerations for Young Children with Cleft Palate Introduction CLEFT LIP AND/OR PALATE Cleft lip and/or palate (CLP) is the fourth most common.
Future Research A prospective study with a larger number of participants is necessary to further validate the relationship between LENA and language samples.
Acknowledgments This study was funded in part by a grant from the National Institutes of Health-Institute on Deafness and other Communicative Disorders.
Marital Satisfaction and Family Functioning in Families with Toddlers: Evidence For a Single Construct? Phillip R. Sevigny, M. A. & Lynn Loutzenhiser,
Putting Together the Pieces: Meaning Matters in Children’s Plural Comprehension Craig Van Pay, Areanna Lakowske & Jennifer Zapf.
Automated Language Environment Analysis (LENA) in Understanding Language Profiles in Young Children with Down Syndrome, Autism, and Typical Development.
The Effects of Increased Cognitive Demands on the Written Discourse Ability of Young Adolescents Ashleigh Elaine Zumwalt Eastern Illinois University.
The Effects of Enhanced Milieu Teaching on Children with Down Syndrome
Nancy J. Scherer A. Lynn Williams East Tennessee State University Ann Kaiser, Megan Roberts, Jennifer Frey, Kristin Mullins Vanderbilt University Carol.
The Discrepancy-Depression Association: Gender and Grade Differences Erin N. Stevens, M. C. Lovejoy, & Laura D. Pittman Northern Illinois University Introduction:
But What Does It All Mean? Key Concepts for Getting the Most Out of Your Assessments Emily Moiduddin.
Developing Communication Skills in Children with ASD Kerry Shelton CEP 843.
By: Mary Hardin-Jones, Kathy Chapman, and Nancy Scherer.
Jared A. Rowland, M.S., Michael M. Knepp, M.S., Sheri L. Towe, M.S., Chris S. Immel, M.S., Ryoichi J.P. Noguchi, M.S., Chad L. Stephens, M.S. & David W.
Preschool-Age Sound- Shape Correspondences to the Bouba-Kiki Effect Karlee Jones, B.S. Ed. & Matthew Carter, Ph.D. Valdosta State University.
Background Purposes of the Study Methods Amanda Rumpca and Dr. Marie Stadler, Ph.D. CCC-SLP  Communication Sciences and Disorders  University of Wisconsin-Eau.
NHS nd International Conference on Newborn Hearing Screening, Diagnosis, and Intervention Como, Italy May 31, 2002 Progress in Specific Language.
Chapter Ten Individuals With Speech and Language Impairments.
◄ ▼ ► ▲ Figure 2: Percent change ([(P 1 – P 2 )/P 1 ]*100) in pre- to post-step test heart rates in oxygen saturation observed at different elevations.
Rhythmic Performances of Young Children Chet-Yeng Loong, Ph.D.
Evidence-based practice in stuttering: The Lidcombe Program
Participants: Participants consisted of 26 (n = 26), healthy, college participants (5 males and 21 females) aged years. See Table 1. Protocol:
Many children with speech-language impairment will have difficulty with reading. Even those children who begin kindergarten with adequate early literacy.
Single Subject Research (Richards et al.) Chapter 8.
RESULTS INTRODUCTION Laurentian_University.svgLaurentian_University.svg‎ (SVG file, nominally 500 × 87 pixels, file size: 57 KB) Comparison of the ASQ.
Quality Of Life, Health And Well Being Of Highly Active Individuals Louisa Raisbeck, Jeanne Johnston, Joel Stager, Francoise Benay Human Performance Laboratory,
Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt.
Tracking Treatment Progress of Families with Oppositional Preschoolers Jaimee C. Perez, M.S., Stephen Bell, Ph.D., Robert W. Adams Linda Garzarella, B.A.,
INTRODUCTION Early after injury, persons with mild traumatic brain injury (TBI) have been shown to experience physical, cognitive, and emotional difficulties.
V v A Comparison of the Effectiveness between Traditional and Video Modeling Strategies on Motor Skill Assessments Emmalee Cron, Layne Case & Joonkoo Yun.
Phonological development in lexically precocious 2-year-olds by Smith, McGregor & Demille Presented by: Marrian B. Bufete.
Objective The current study examined whether the timing of recovery from late onset of productive vocabulary (e.g., either earlier or later blooming) was.
Results Effects of a Naturalistic Sign Intervention on Expressive Language of Toddlers with Down Syndrome Introduction Children with DS have specific phenotypic.
Electrophysiological Processing of Single Words in Toddlers and School-Age Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Sharon Coffey-Corina 1, Denise Padden.
Introduction The authors of this research would like to thank the University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire’s Office of Research and Sponsored Programs for financial.
Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.
Business and Management Research
Comparative efficacy of the Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS) versus a Speech- Generating Device: Effects on Social- communicative Skills and.
Insight and Locus of Control as Related to Aggression in Individuals with Severe Mental Illness (SMI) Bethany L. Ridling Faculty Mentor: Dr. William Spaulding.
MethodIntroductionResults Discussion Factors Affecting Psychosocial Functioning in Serious Mental Illness and Implications for Treatment Jason E. Vogler,
Parent Education, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch, Ph.D. Baylor University Stephen Camarata, Ph.D. Edward G.
Personally Important Posttraumatic Growth as a Predictor of Self-Esteem in Adolescents Leah McDiarmid, Kanako Taku Ph.D., & Aundreah Walenski Presented.
Gender Differences in Buffering Stress Responses in Same-Sex Friend Dyads Sydney N. Pauling, Jenalee R. Doom, & Megan R. Gunnar Institute of Child Development,
The Effect of Facial Paralysis on Career Success: A Preliminary Investigation Cramer Kallem and Kathleen Bogart PhD Oregon State University Introduction.
ABSTRACT The purpose of the present study was to investigate the test-retest reliability of force-time derived parameters of an explosive push up. Seven.
Dyadic Patterns of Parental Perceptions of Health- Related Quality of Life Gustavo R. Medrano & W. Hobart Davies University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Pediatric.
Prenatal Smoke Exposure and Language Outcomes at 15 Months: Social Aspects of Communication vs Expressive and Receptive Language Departments of Family.
Denise Romero Corral Angela Walton Maricela Martinez.
Development of Rhyming Tasks in Preschool Aged Children with Hearing Loss Mary Katherine Connelly, Kaylie McNally Joan A. Luckhurst, Ph.D., CCC/SLP, faculty.
Background Objectives Methods Study Design A program evaluation of WIHD AfterCare families utilizing data collected from self-report measures and demographic.
Project VIABLE - Direct Behavior Rating: Evaluating Behaviors with Positive and Negative Definitions Rose Jaffery 1, Albee T. Ongusco 3, Amy M. Briesch.
Unit 7 Research Designs. What is a Research Design?? Researcher’s strategy: Describes how the researcher(s) will answer their questions/test hypotheses.
Association of Cognitive Outcomes and Response Status in Late Life Depression: A 12 Month Longitudinal Study David Bickford B.A., Alana Kivowitz B.A.,
Discussion Results Introduction From Lateral to Leader: A Study of Preschoolers’ Relationships with Peers Erin Podgorski & Dr. Carin L. Neitzel, The University.
1 A Comparison of Motor Delays in Young Children: Autism Spectrum Disorder, Developmental Delay, and Developmental Concerns Beth Provost, Brian R. Lopez,
USE OF THE COMMUNICATION FUNCTION CLASSIFICATION SYSTEM WITH TODDLERS Ashley Hopkin & Dr. Mary Jo Cooley Hidecker Division of Communication Disorders The.
Speech Ratings of Patients with Clefts: Comparing Caregivers’ Ratings to Speech- Language Pathologist’s Emily Swain Department of Speech Pathology and.
Association of Body Mass Index (BMI) and Depression Severity
Ali Fay, B.S. Ed. Valdosta State University
Does training in number knowledge improve arithmetic scores?
Copyright © American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
TREATMENT SENSITIVITY OF THE DYADIC PARENT-CHILD INTERACTION CODING SYSTEM-II Jenny Klein, B.S., Branlyn Werba, M.S., and Sheila Eyberg, Ph.D. University.
Relationships among Adolescents’ Negative Interaction Styles with Friends and Romantic Partners and Depressive Symptoms Joanna M. Chango, Erin M. Miga,
Introduction Results Hypotheses Discussion Method
Laura M. Sylke & David E. Szwedo James Madison University Introduction
Presentation transcript:

Discussion Future Research Although there was no statistical significance between prelinguistic and linguistic levels of communicative acts, canonical vocalizations, and words, several comparisons approached significance. A prospective study with a larger number of participants is necessary to identify potential predictive relationships. Clinical Implications Assessment of communicative acts at the prelinguistic level may assist clinicians by assessing early communicative milestones that are less dependent on vocal production. Introduction Studies of early communicative development have shown a relationship between rates of communicative acts and canonical vocalizations with the acquisition of words for typically developing children (Yoder & Warren, 1999; Calandrella & Wilcox, 2000). Typically developing children are using communicative acts at a rate of 1 act per minute at the prelinguistic level (Wetherby, Cain, Yonclas, & Walker, 1988). The rate increases to approximately 2 acts per minute when children are using more than 10 words and 5 acts per minute when children reach the multi-word stage (Wetherby, Cain, Yonclas, & Walker, 1988). Delays in early speech and language have been reported for children with cleft lip and/or palate (Scherer & D’Antonio, 1997; Chapman, Hardin-Jones, Schulte, & Halter, 2001). It has been suggested that these delays are a direct relationship to the presence of an open palate until after repair at approximately 12 months of age. However, there has been little examination of nonverbal milestones of early communicative development in children with cleft lip and/or palate. If speech and language delays are related to the early unrepaired cleft, one would expect that rates of communicative acts would be similar to rates reported for typically developing peers, while rates of canonical vocalizations and word use may be delayed. The purpose of this study was to examine children's rates of communicative acts, canonical vocalizations, and words during the transition from prelinguistic to linguistic level. Abstract This retrospective study examined the rates of nonverbal communicative acts, canonical vocalizations, and word use in children with cleft palate as they transitioned from prelinguistic to linguistic levels. Fifteen children with cleft lip and/or palate were selected from a prior longitudinal study and their scores from the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (CDI) were collected from their evaluations at 18, 24, and 30 months of age. Ten minutes of the language samples were scored for the number of communicative acts (protodeclaratives and protoimperatives) based on Bates, Camaioni, & Volterra (1975), canonical vocalizations, and words. Results showed that the children with cleft lip and/or palate went through the transition from prelinguistic to linguistic level at an older age than reported in typically developing peers. However, their use of nonverbal communicative act and canonical vocalization rates were similar to typically developing children when they transitioned from prelinguistic to linguistic levels. Rate of word use was delayed when compared to reports of typically developing children. Statistical comparisons revealed no significant differences between rates of communicative acts, canonical vocalizations and words at prelinguistic and linguistic levels, although communicative acts and words approached significance. This preliminary study suggests that children with cleft lip and/or palate are developing nonverbal as well as vocal and verbal skills at a slower pace than their typically developing peers. Therefore, a prospective replication with larger numbers of children is warranted. Method Participants: Retrospective study (previous longitudinal study of non-syndromic children with cleft lip and/or palate). 15 participants (nine females and six males) from northeast Tennessee. Nine of the participants had cleft lip and palate, while the remaining six participants had cleft palate only. Procedures: The children were evaluated at 18, 24, and 30 months of age, during which time they transitioned from the prelinguistic level (10 or less words on MCDI) to the linguistic level (more than10 words on MCDI). At 18 months of age, 100% of the participants were in the prelinguistic level. At 24 months of age, 50% of the participants were in the prelinguistic level and the remaining 50% were in the linguistic level. At 30 months of age, 100% of the participants were in the linguistic level. The researchers independently viewed ten-minute videotaped samples of each child while he/she was actively engaged with the clinician. Data were recorded on the number of communicative acts, canonical vocalizations, and words the child produced at each age. A communicative act consisted of the following: two out of three communicative means (eye gaze, gesture, or vocalization), directed towards another person, and displayed a communicative purpose (protoimperative and protodeclarative) (Calandrella and Wilcox, 2000; Bates, Camaioni, Volterra, 1975). The data were then converted to a rate per minute ratio. Reliability: Reliability for coding was calculated with a second reviewer watching 30 percent of each ten-minute sample. The mean inter- coder reliability was 98.3 percent (87-100%). Statistical Analysis: Statistical analysis was performed using paired t-test and SigmaStat Normality Test. Correlation and effect size were calculated using the Spearman Rank Order Correlation and the Corrected D test respectively. Results Figure 1. Production of Communicative Acts, Canonical Vocalizations and Words from Prelinguistic to Linguistic Levels in Children with Cleft Lip and/or Palate The average rate per minute of communicative acts and canonical vocalizations increased substantially. The pattern of acquisition was normal compared to typically developing children; however, the acquisition was at a slower pace. Word use per minute increased less dramatically. The children in this study were delayed chronologically in comparison to typically developing children. Statistical comparisons revealed no statistically significant differences between rates of communicative acts, canonical vocalizations and words at prelinguistic and linguistic levels, although communicative acts (p=0.09)approached significance. Discussion cont. Proctor-Williams, Dixon, Brown, Ringley, Barber, & Light-Newell (2007) conducted a study using similar methodology with 29 typically developing children at 7, 10, 13, and 20 months of age. Children in the current study (shown in Figures 2-4) demonstrated higher rates of communicative acts and canonical vocalizations at the prelinguistic and linguistic levels when compared to the children in the Proctor-Williams et al. (2007) study. However, the children with cleft lip and/or palate exhibited lower rates per minute of word use. When age was compared, the children with cleft lip and/or palate were approximately 10 months older than the typically developing children. Other studies of typically developing children have reported use of communicative acts at a rate of 1 act per minute at the prelinguistic level and 2 acts per minute at the linguistic level (Wetherby, Cain, Yonclas, & Walker, 1988). The children in this study used 1.76 acts per minute at the prelinguistic level and 3.34 acts per minute at the linguistic level, well above the rate reported for prelinguistic and linguistic levels. However, the children with cleft lip and/or palate were older than the typically developing children. COMMUNICATIVE ACTS AND WORD ACQUISITION IN TODDLERS WITH CLEFT PALATE Sarah Boyce, B.A., Gerri Martin, B.S., Chariti Skinner, B.B.A., Kaley Wetherholt, B.S. & Nancy Scherer, Ph. D. Department of Audiology and Speech-Language Pathology, East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN Table 1. Number of vocabulary words reported on the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (MCDI) at 18, 24, and 30 months. Subject18 mths24 mths30 mths References Bates, E., Camaioni, L., Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, Calandrella, A.M., & Wilcox, M.J. (2000). Predicting language outcomes for young prelinguistic children with developmental delay. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, Chapman, K. L., Hardin-Jones, M., Schulte, J., & Halter, K. (2001). Vocal development of 9-month-old babies with cleft palate. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, Scherer, N. J., & D'Antonio, L. (1997). Language and play development in toddlers with cleft lip and/or palate. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 6(4), Wetherby, A. M., Cain, D. H., Yonclas, D. G., & Walker, V. G. (1988). Analysis of intentional communication of normal children from the prelinguistic to the multiword stage. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, Yoder, P.J., & Warren, S.F. (1999). Facilitating proto-declaratives and proto- imperatives in prelinguistic children with developmental delays. Journal of Early Intervention, 22(4), Comparison of Communicative Acts (Figure 2), Canonical Vocalizations (Figure 3), and Words (Figure 4) between typically developing children and children with cleft lip and/or palate at the prelinguistic and linguistic levels.