The Prefrontal Cortex: Brain Waves and Cognition Earl K. Miller The Picower Institute for Learning and Memory and Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences,

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Read this article for Friday next week [1]Chelazzi L, Miller EK, Duncan J, Desimone R. A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex. Nature.
Advertisements

Are We Paying Attention Yet? A review of the relation between attention and saccades By Travis McKinney.
Gene-gene interaction of COMT and DRD2 modulates context updating and novelty processing Manuel Garcia-Garcia, Francisco Barceló, Iria SanMiguel, Immaculada.
Part 1: Definitions, brain basis Isabelle Rapin
The Physiology of Attention. Physiology of Attention Neural systems involved in orienting Neural correlates of selection.
Visual Attention Attention is the ability to select objects of interest from the surrounding environment A reliable measure of attention is eye movement.
Chapter 4: Local integration 2: Neural correlates of the BOLD signal
Human (ERP and imaging) and monkey (cell recording) data together 1. Modality specific extrastriate cortex is modulated by attention (V4, IT, MT). 2. V1.
Pre-frontal cortex and Executive Function Squire et al Ch 52.
Read this article for Friday next week [1]Chelazzi L, Miller EK, Duncan J, Desimone R. A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex. Nature.
Cross-cortical Coherence during Effector Decision Making Chess Stetson Andersen Laboratory Caltech Sloan-Swartz Meeting 2009/07/28.
1.Exams due 9am 16 th. (grades due 10am 19 th ) 2.Describe the organization of visual signals in extra-striate visual cortex and the specialization of.
NEUR 3680 Midterm II Review Megan Metzler
Psych 216: Movement Attention. What is attention? There is too much information available in the world to process it all. Demonstration: change-detection.
Neuronal Coding in the Retina and Fixational Eye Movements Christian Mendl, Tim Gollisch Max Planck Institute of Neurobiology, Junior Research Group Visual.
Mirror Neurons.
Brain Rhythms and Short-Term Memory Earl K. Miller The Picower Institute for Learning and Memory and Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts.
From Perception to Action And what’s in between?.
Measures electrical activity of neurons near electrode tip Single-electrode Recording The primary tool for investigation of brain- behavior relationships.
Final Review Session Neural Correlates of Visual Awareness Mirror Neurons
Memory III Working Memory & Brain. Atkinson & Shiffrin (1968) Model of Memory.
Business Minor grade adjustments on Midterm 2 Opportunity to participate in Cognitive Neuroscience and Perception experiment - sign up for Tuesday, Wednesday.
Disorders of Orienting Lesions to parietal cortex can produce some strange behavioural consequences –patients fail to notice events on the contralesional.
Alan L. Yuille. UCLA. Dept. Statistics and Psychology. Neural Prosthetic: Mind Reading. STATS 19 SEM Talk 6. Neural.
Memory III Working Memory. Atkinson & Shiffrin (1968) Model of Memory.
Sensory Memory and Working Memory. Sensory Memory Brief Iconic/echoic High capacity Pre-attentive Is there a Neural Correlate of Sensory Memory?
Consequences of Attentional Selection Single unit recordings.
Zicong Zhang Authors Wendy A. Suzuki Professor of Neural Science and Psychology, New York University Research interest: Organization of memory.
Studying Visual Attention with the Visual Search Paradigm Marc Pomplun Department of Computer Science University of Massachusetts at Boston
Change blindness and time to consciousness Professor: Liu Student: Ruby.
Attention Modulates Responses in the Human Lateral Geniculate Nucleus Nature Neuroscience, 2002, 5(11): Presented by Juan Mo.
Adaptive, behaviorally gated, persistent encoding of task-relevant auditory information in ferret frontal cortex.
Psych 216: Movement Attention. What is attention? Covert and overt selection appear to recruit the same areas of the brain.
Attention Part 2. Early Selection Model (Broadbent, 1958) inputdetectionrecognition FI L T E R Only information that passed the filter received further.
Changju Lee Visual System Neural Network Lab. Department of Bio and Brain Engineering.
Cognitive Information Processing Dr. K. A. Korb University of Jos.
Studying Memory Encoding with fMRI Event-related vs. Blocked Designs Aneta Kielar.
Subject wearing a VR helmet immersed in the virtual environment on the right, with obstacles and targets. Subjects follow the path, avoid the obstacels,
How well do we understand the neural origins of the fMRI BOLD signal? Owen J Arthurs and Simon Boniface Trends in Neuroscience, 2002 Gillian Elizabeth.
Visual Perception, Attention & Action. Anthony J Greene2.
Control of Attention in schizophrenia 1.Advance understanding of schizophrenia. Move from description of deficits to explanation of specific mechanisms.
Chapter 50 The Prefrontal Cortex and Executive Brain Functions Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Cognition, Brain and Consciousness: An Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience Edited by Bernard J. Baars and Nicole M. Gage 2007 Academic Press Chapter.
Biological Modeling of Neural Networks: Week 12 – Decision models: Competitive dynamics Wulfram Gerstner EPFL, Lausanne, Switzerland 12.1 Review: Population.
The Function of Synchrony Marieke Rohde Reading Group DyStURB (Dynamical Structures to Understand Real Brains)
Pattern Classification of Attentional Control States S. G. Robison, D. N. Osherson, K. A. Norman, & J. D. Cohen Dept. of Psychology, Princeton University,
Synchronous activity within and between areas V4 and FEF in attention Steve Gotts Laboratory of Brain and Cognition NIMH, NIH with: Georgia Gregoriou,
The brain at rest. Spontaneous rhythms in a dish Connected neural populations tend to synchronize and oscillate together.
Copyright © 2007 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Neuroscience: Exploring the Brain, 3e
Reappraising the relationship between working memory and conscious awareness A Trends in Cognitive Science publication by David Soto and Juha Silvanto.
Corresponding author: Ruth Raymaekers, Ghent University, Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health Psychology, Research Group Developmental Disorders;
D AVIDSSON ET AL L ONG - TERM MEDITATORS SELF - INDUCE HIGH - AMPLITUDE GAMMA SYNCHRONY DURING MENTAL PRACTICE Background: Practitioners understand.
The role of synchronous gamma-band activity in schizophrenia Jakramate 2009 / 01 / 14.
An ERP investigation of response inhibition in adults with DCD Elisabeth Hill Duncan Brown José van Velzen.
Date of download: 6/28/2016 Copyright © 2016 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. From: Teamwork Matters: Coordinated Neuronal Activity in.
Ch. 13 A face in the crowd: which groups of neurons process face stimuli, and how do they interact? KARI L. HOFFMANN 2009/1/13 BI, Population Coding Seminar.
Visual Attention (neural mechanisms) Arash Afraz.
Neurofeedback of beta frequencies:
NATURE NEUROSCIENCE 2007 Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep Daoyun Ji & Matthew A Wilson Department of Brain.
Volume 60, Issue 4, Pages (November 2008)
Heather L. Dean, Maureen A. Hagan, Bijan Pesaran  Neuron 
M. Victoria Puig, Earl K. Miller  Neuron 
Heather L. Dean, Maureen A. Hagan, Bijan Pesaran  Neuron 
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Adaptive Training Diminishes Distractibility in Aging across Species
Progress Seminar 권순빈.
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Georgia G. Gregoriou, Stephen J. Gotts, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Volume 76, Issue 4, Pages (November 2012)
Volume 65, Issue 4, Pages (February 2010)
Presentation transcript:

The Prefrontal Cortex: Brain Waves and Cognition Earl K. Miller The Picower Institute for Learning and Memory and Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology

Our Goal: To understand the neural basis of higher cognition. Our Approach: Multiple-electrode recording in trained monkeys. The prefrontal cortex (PFC)

Measures electrical activity of neurons near electrode tip Single-electrode Recording The primary tool for investigation of brain- behavior relationships for over 60 years A useful tool for studying the details of properties of individual neurons. Ideal for an understanding at the level of individual neurons. Less appropriate for studying networks and systems of neurons. Does not allow measurements of the precise timing of activity between neurons that give insight into how they communicate and interact. The classic single-electrode approach only allows indirect inferences about neural networks. The result: a piecemeal understanding of brain function

A More Global View of Brain Function: FMRI. However…. FMRI measures patterns of blood flow to brain areas (the BOLD signal). Result of neurons needing energy (oxygen) when they fire electrical impulses (“action potentials”). The Good: Provides a global view of which brain areas are engaged by a cognitive function. The Bad: It takes five-six seconds for the BOLD signal to build. A lot can happen in the brain in 5-6 seconds.

Our approach: Multiple-electrode Recording in Monkeys Performing Cognitive-demanding Tasks Electrode arrays with 500 um spacing to investigate microcircuitry Electrode arrays in different brain areas to investigate large-scale networks. Allows direct measurements of the networks that underlie cognition.

Working memory is the ability to hold and manipulate information in mind. It is central to normal cognition and closely linked to a wide range of cognitive abilities such as attention, planning, reasoning, etc. Working Memory – The “Sketchpad” of Conscious Thought

Fixate until fixation cross disappears. Then look at the cued position A Classic Test of Working Memory: Oculomotor Spatial Delayed Response Task (Goldman-Rakic and colleagues)

Fixate until fixation cross disappears. Then look at the cued position A Classic Test of Working Memory: Oculomotor Spatial Delayed Response Task (Goldman-Rakic and colleagues)

The Classic Approach to Studying Neurons: Measure Average Level of Neural Activity of Individual Neurons From Funahashi and Goldman-Rakic (1989) This neuron “remembers” the upper left location. It is more active when the remembered cue was in the upper left. Holding a single thought or memory in mind is a fundamental, but relatively simple, cognitive function.

Siegel, Warden, and Miller (2009) Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. How Do You Hold and Order Multiple Items in Working Memory? Just examining the activity of individual neurons does not clearly distinguish object order The classic approach: Information about each object from the average activity of individual PFC neurons Task: Remember two objects and their order of appearance

Brain waves are rhythmic, coordinated oscillations between neurons (1 – 100 Hz). They reflect how and when networks of neurons communicate. They allow local networks of neurons to synchronize with one another and with distant networks. This allows the brain to orchestrate billions of neurons to produce elaborate behaviors. The idea is that when neurons fire in synchrony with one another, they are better able to communicate than when they fire out of sync. Mounting evidence that brain waves play a critical role in attention, working memory, memory storage, recall, learning, sequencing, planning and more. Abnormal brain waves are associated with neuropsychiatric disorders. So, How Do You Hold and Order Multiple Items in Working Memory? A solution: Brain waves Parkinson’s patients show increased beta band brain waves (which can be decreased by DA therapy) Schizophrenia patients show decreased gamma band brain waves. Guanfacine (ADHD treatment) increases brain wave (EEG) synchrony in rats. Methylphenidate (ADHD) increases theta brain waves in the hippocampus.

Siegel, Warden, and Miller (2009) Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. How Do You Hold and Order Multiple Items in Working Memory? Task: Remember two objects and their order of appearance Hypothesis: Brain waves act as a “carrier signal” that helps order multiple thoughts held in mind.

32 Hz Brain Waves During Memory Delays Siegel, Warden, and Miller (2009) Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci.

Information about which object is held in memory from activity in each brain wave phase bin. Object Information in Activity of Individual Neurons by Brain Wave Phase Siegel, Warden, and Miller (2009) Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci.

P = Objects were balanced by order Siegel, Warden, and Miller (2009) Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. Object Information in Activity of Individual Neurons by Brain Wave Phase

P = P < Objects were balanced by order Difference = 62 deg P = Siegel, Warden, and Miller (2009) Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. Object Information in Activity of Individual Neurons by Brain Wave Phase

Conclusions During working memory, prefrontal activity shows 32 Hz brain waves. Information about the different objects line up on different parts (phases) of the brain waves according to their memorized order. This may help order thought and keep multiple thoughts from interfering with one another. A reduction in gamma band brain waves was recently seen in schizophrenics. This may also explain why short-term memory has a capacity limitation. Siegel, Warden, and Miller (2009) Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 32 Hz brain waves = spike- timing dependent plasticity?

Cognitive capacity: How many things can you hold in mind simultaneously? Individual differences in capacity limits can explain about 25-50% of the individual differences in tests of intelligence It is linked to normal cognition and intelligence: Capacity is highest in younger adults and reduced in many neuropsychiatric disorders Schizophrenia Parkinson’s Disease Vogel et al (2001); Gold et al (2003); Cowan et al (2006); Hackley et al (2009) Cognitive capacity is the bandwidth of cognition. It may be directly related to brain waves.

A Potential Application for Brain Waves: A Cognitive Enhancer? If we could (slightly) slow down the frequency, or increase the amplitude, of the gamma band oscillations, we could, in theory, add an additional memory slot and increase cognitive capacity. This could increase the bandwidth of cognition and effectively increase general intelligence. Cognitive capacity (the width of one wave)

Bottom-up vs top-down attention Top-down (search): Goal-directed, knowledge-based, volitional Bottom-up (pop-out): Stimuli-driven, reflexive Other examples: fire alarms, looming objects

Bottom-up (Reflexive) vs Top-down (Volitional) Attention Buschman and Miller (2007) Science Buschman and Miller (2009) Neuron indicates monkeys’ eye position Bottom-up (pop-out) Top-down (search)

60 electrodes simultaneously implanted in the frontal cortex (PFC and FEF) and parietal cortex (LIP) An area with a shorter neural latency for a given signal is more likely to be its source. Where Do Top-down and Bottom-up Attention Signals Originate?

Bottom-up (reflexive) Top-down (volitional) When Does Each Brain Area First Find the Target? Bottom-up Top-down LIP > PFC > FEFPFC = FEF > LIP All P < 0.01 Visual array onset Buschman and Miller (2007) Science

How Do We Control Voluntary Shifts of Attention? Buschman and Miller (2007) Science Buschman and Miller (2009) Neuron indicates monkeys’ eye position

How Do We Search a Crowded Visual Scene? Serial search: A moving “spotlight” of attention It is well known that neurons in many brain areas reflect the ultimate focusing of attention on a target (e.g., Waldo). However, neural correlates of shifting attention to search for the target have not been observed with the classic single- electrode approach.

Behavioral Reaction Times Suggest That Monkeys Use a Clockwise Covert Serial Search Strategy Example of behavioral reaction time from one monkey during one testing session. This monkey tended to start covert search at the lower right location (4 o’clock) and then searched clockwise. Each monkey chose a different starting point; both showed evidence for clockwise covert search. Buschman and Miller (2009) Neuron

FEF Population Activity during Search Preferred location Neuron’s preferred location Eye movement to target Clockwise covert search Neural Evidence for a Moving “Spotlight” of Attention Buschman and Miller (2009) Neuron

FEF Population Activity during Search Preferred location One position clockwise from the preferred direction Neuron’s preferred location Eye movement to target Clockwise covert search Buschman and Miller (2009) Neuron Neural Evidence for a Moving “Spotlight” of Attention

FEF Population Activity during Search Preferred location One position clockwise from the preferred direction Neuron’s preferred location Eye movement to target Clockwise covert search Buschman and Miller (2009) Neuron Neural Evidence for a Moving “Spotlight” of Attention

FEF Population Activity during Search Preferred location One position clockwise from the preferred direction Neuron’s preferred location Eye movement to target Clockwise covert search Two positions clockwise from the preferred direction Neural Evidence for a Moving “Spotlight” of Attention

FEF Population Activity during Search Preferred location One position clockwise from the preferred direction Neuron’s preferred location Eye movement to target Clockwise covert search Two positions clockwise from the preferred direction Neural Evidence for a Moving “Spotlight” of Attention

How Long Does It Take to Shift Attention? ~40 ms Behavioral testing: Adding more distractors increased behavioral reaction time by 40 ms per distractor Buschman and Miller (2009) Neuron

Baseline Shift of attention Buschman and Miller (2007) Science Visual Popout Stronger for top-down attention Stronger for bottom-up attention Hypothesis: Middle frequency (beta) band brain waves control when attention is shifted. 40 ms Visual search Normalized difference in coherence between frontal and parietal LFPs during search minus pop-out Middle Frequency (Beta) Band Brain Waves Appear During Visual Search

Serial Shifts of Covert Attention Were Synchronized to 25 Hz Brain Waves in the Prefrontal Cortex Neuron’s receptive field location Upper right Lower right Lower left Upper right (target) Shifts of attention every 40 ms Target AttentionFound target

Brain Wave Frequency Was Correlated with Search Time Target Slower oscillations = slower shifts of attention = more time required to search = longer reaction time Buschman and Miller (2009) Neuron Correlation between brain wave frequency and time to find the target

Top-down (volitional) attention: Signals originate from prefrontal cortex Serial shifts of attention (every ~40 ms) 25 Hz brain waves may act as a ‘clock’ that controls the shifts in attention. Top-down Bottom-up Buschman and Miller (2007) Science Buschman and Miller (2009) Neuron Hypothesis: A reduction in beta-band oscillations might explain why some people have trouble shifting attention away from distracting things.

Novel images Familiar images Fixation Cue DelayTarget onset 800 ms 500 ms1000 msResponse 40 % 10 % The Role of Dopamine (D1R) Receptors in the Prefrontal Cortex During Learning Monkeys learned by trial and error to associate two novel visual cues with either an eye movement to the right or left Puig, M.V. and Miller, E.K. (in preparation)

… Injection schedules Block number Location of the injections and grid configuration Saline 3 µl SCH (D1 antagonist) 30 µg in 3 µl Infusion rate: 0.3 µl/min (3 µl in 10 minutes) BaselineDrugWashout // … BaselineDrugWashout // … BaselineDrugWashout // Session type #1 Session type #2 Session type #3 Recording with Multiple Electrodes while Injecting a D1R Blocker Puig, M.V. and Miller, E.K. (in preparation)

Blocking D1R Receptors Impairs New Learning But Not Long-Term Memory Performance novel associations Performance familiar associations Baseline Saline Washout Percent correct Baseline SCH Washout Percent correct ns Saline SCH23390 BaselineWashout Baseline Criterion Chance

Blocking D1Rs Decreases Attention and Increases Impulsivity Fixation breaks per blockEarly trials per block Baseline Treatment Washout Saline SCH Effect on attentionEffect on impulsivity *** Puig, M.V. and Miller, E.K. (in preparation)

Blocking D1R Receptors Causes Neuronal Avalanches: Super-synchronous activity Avalanches appeared in 47 of 68 electrodes (~70% of 9 sessions) Duration 18 ± 5min (~10-30 min) Frequency of deflections 0.44  0.03 Hz ( Hz) Amplitude of deflections is huge: in most cases over 500  V Performance 7 sessions with impairment: drops to 56 ± 15 % Amplitude (  V) Puig, M.V. and Miller, E.K. (in preparation)

Blocking D1R Receptors Causes a Broad-Band Increase in PFC Brain Waves Cue Delay Response Normalized spectrum dB Baseline SCH Task Interval: Brain wave frequency Normalized spectrum dB Abnormal brain waves are a bad thing Puig, M.V. and Miller, E.K. (in preparation)

Brain waves are central to brain function. They regulate communication between neurons and there is mounting evidence that they play specific and important roles in higher cognition. Abnormal brain waves are apparent in neuropsychiatric disorders. Multiple-electrodes offer a new tool for directly measuring the effects of potential drug therapies on cognition. They allow direct examination of the functioning of microcircuits and large-scale networks of neurons. This gets directly at the network mechanisms underlying cognition. The combination of cutting-edge multiple-electrode technology and sophisticated behavioral paradigms in monkeys can provide a powerful diagnostic of the cellular mechanisms that underlie cognitive enhancements by potential drug therapies. CONCLUSIONS

Miller Lab Oct