Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Applying Regression.

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Applying Regression."— Presentation transcript:

1 Applying Regression

2 The Course 14 (or so) lessons Some flexibility Depends how we feel
What we get through

3 Part I: Theory of Regression
Models in statistics Models with more than one parameter: regression Samples to populations Introducing multiple regression More on multiple regression

4 Part 2: Application of regression
Categorical predictor variables Assumptions in regression analysis Issues in regression analysis Non-linear regression Categorical and count variables Moderators (interactions) in regression Mediation and path analysis Part 3:Taking Regression Further (Kind of brief) Introducing longitudinal multilevel models

5 Bonuses Bonus lesson1: Why is it called regression? Bonus lesson 2: Other types of regression.

6 House Rules Jeremy must remember If you don’t understand
Not to talk too fast If you don’t understand Ask Any time If you think I’m wrong Ask. (I’m not always right)

7 The Assistants Carla Xena - cxenag@essex.ac.uk
Eugenia Suarez Arian

8 Learning New Techniques
Best kind of data to learn a new technique Data that you know well, and understand Your own data In computer labs (esp later on) Use your own data if you like My data I’ll provide you with Simple examples, small sample sizes Conceptually simple (even silly)

9 Computer Programs Stata Excel GPower Mostly For calculations
I’ll explain SPSS options You’ll like Stata more Excel For calculations Semi-optional GPower

10 Lesson 1: Models in statistics
Models, parsimony, error, mean, OLS estimators

11 What is a Model?

12 What is a model? Representation
Of reality Not reality Model aeroplane represents a real aeroplane If model aeroplane = real aeroplane, it isn’t a model

13 Statistics is about modelling Sifting
Representing and simplifying Sifting What is important from what is not important Parsimony In statistical models we seek parsimony Parsimony  simplicity

14 Parsimony in Science A model should be: More it explains
1: able to explain a lot 2: use as few concepts as possible More it explains The more you get Fewer concepts The lower the price Is it worth paying a higher price for a better model?

15 The Mean as a Model

16 The (Arithmetic) Mean We all know the mean The mean is: The ‘average’
Learned about it at school Forget (didn’t know) about how clever the mean is The mean is: An Ordinary Least Squares (OLS) estimator Best Linear Unbiased Estimator (BLUE)

17 Mean as OLS Estimator Going back a step or two
MODEL was a representation of DATA We said we want a model that explains a lot How much does a model explain? DATA = MODEL + ERROR ERROR = DATA - MODEL We want a model with as little ERROR as possible

18 What is error? Data (Y) Model (b0) mean Error (e) 1.40 1.60 -0.20 1.55
-0.05 1.80 0.20 1.62 0.02 1.63 0.03

19 How can we calculate the ‘amount’ of error?
Sum of errors? Sum of absolute errors?

20 Are small and large errors equivalent?
One error of 4 Four errors of 1 The same? What happens with different data? Y = (2, 2, 5) b0 = 2 Not very representative Y = (2, 2, 4, 4) b0 = any value from 2 - 4 Indeterminate There are an infinite number of solutions which would satisfy our criteria for minimum error

21 Sum of squared errors (SSE)

22 Determinate If we minimise SSE Shown in graph Always gives one answer
Get the mean Shown in graph SSE plotted against b0 Min value of SSE occurs when b0 = mean

23

24 The Mean as an OLS Estimate

25 Mean as OLS Estimate The mean is an Ordinary Least Squares (OLS) estimate As are lots of other things This is exciting because OLS estimators are BLUE Best Linear Unbiased Estimators Proven with Gauss-Markov Theorem Which we won’t worry about

26 BLUE Estimators Best Minimum variance (of all possible unbiased estimators) Narrower distribution than other estimators e.g. median, mode

27 SSE and the Standard Deviation
Tying up a loose end

28 SSE closely related to SD Sample standard deviation – s
Biased estimator of population SD Population standard deviation - s Need to know the mean to calculate SD Reduces N by 1 Hence divide by N-1, not N Like losing one df

29 Proof That the mean minimises SSE Available in Not that difficult
As statistical proofs go Available in Maxwell and Delaney – Designing experiments and analysing data Judd and McClelland – Data Analysis: a model comparison approach (out of print?)

30 What’s a df? The number of parameters free to vary
When one is fixed Term comes from engineering Movement available to structures

31 Back to the Data Mean has 5 (N) df s has N –1 df 1st moment
Mean has been fixed 2nd moment Can think of it as amount of cases vary away from the mean

32 While we are at it … Skewness has N – 2 df Kurtosis has N – 3 df
3rd moment Kurtosis has N – 3 df 4rd moment Amount cases vary from s

33 Parsimony and df Number of df remaining
Measure of parsimony Model which contained all the data Has 0 df Not a parsimonious model Normal distribution Can be described in terms of mean and s 2 parameters (z with 0 parameters)

34 Summary of Lesson 1 Statistics is about modelling DATA
Models have parameters Fewer parameters, more parsimony, better Models need to minimise ERROR Best model, least ERROR Depends on how we define ERROR If we define error as sum of squared deviations from predicted value Mean is best MODEL

35 Lesson 1a A really brief introduction to Stata

36 Commands Command review Output Variable list Commands

37 Stata Commands Can use menus All have similar format:
But commands are easy All have similar format: command variables , options Stata is case sensitive BEDS, beds, Beds Stata lets you shorten summarize sqft su sq

38 More Stata Commands Open exercise 1.4.dta Run Or summarize sqm
table beds mean price histogram price Or su be tab be mean pr hist pr

39 Lesson 2: Models with one more parameter - regression

40 In Lesson 1 we said … Use a model to predict and describe data
Mean is a simple, one parameter model

41 More Models Slopes and Intercepts

42 More Models The mean is OK We often have more information than that
As far as it goes It just doesn’t go very far Very simple prediction, uses very little information We often have more information than that We want to use more information than that

43 House Prices Look at house prices in one area of Los Angeles
Predictors of house prices Using: Sale price, size, number of bedrooms, size of lot, year built …

44

45

46 House Prices 3628 OLYMPIAD Dr 649500 4 3 2575 3673 OLYMPIAD Dr 450000
address listprice beds baths sqft 3628 OLYMPIAD Dr 649500 4 3 2575 3673 OLYMPIAD Dr 450000 2 1910 3838 CHANSON Dr 489900 2856 3838 West 58TH Pl 330000 1651 3919 West 58TH Pl 349000 1466 3954 FAIRWAY Blvd 514900 2.25 2018 4044 OLYMPIAD Dr 649000 2.5 3019 4336 DON LUIS Dr 474000 2188 4421 West 59TH St 460000 1519 4518 WHELAN Pl 388000 1.5 1403 4670 West 63RD St 259500 1491 5000 ANGELES VISTA Blvd 678800 5 3808

47 One Parameter Model The mean “How much is that house worth?” $415,689
Use 1 df to say that

48 Adding More Parameters
We have more information than this We might as well use it Add a linear function of number of size (square feet) (x1)

49 Alternative Expression
Estimate of Y (expected value of Y) Value of Y

50 Estimating the Model We can estimate this model in four different, equivalent ways Provides more than one way of thinking about it 1. Estimating the slope which minimises SSE 2. Examining the proportional reduction in SSE 3. Calculating the covariance 4. Looking at the efficiency of the predictions

51 Estimate the Slope to Minimise SSE

52 Estimate the Slope Stage 1 Mark errors on it Draw a scatterplot
x-axis at mean Not at zero Mark errors on it Called ‘residuals’ Sum and square these to find SSE

53

54

55 Add another slope to the chart
Redraw residuals Recalculate SSE Move the line around to find slope which minimises SSE Find the slope

56 First attempt:

57 Any straight line can be defined with two parameters
The location (height) of the slope b0 Sometimes called a The gradient of the slope b1

58 Gradient b1 units 1 unit

59 Height b0 units

60 Height is defined as the point that the slope hits the y-axis
If we fix slope to zero Height becomes mean Hence mean is b0 Height is defined as the point that the slope hits the y-axis The constant The y-intercept

61 Why the constant? Implicit in Stata b0x0
Where x0 is 1.00 for every case i.e. x0 is constant Implicit in Stata (And SPSS, SAS, R) Some packages force you to make it explicit (Later on we’ll need to make it explicit)

62 Why the intercept? Where the regression line intercepts the y-axis
Sometimes called y-intercept

63 Finding the Slope How do we find the values of b0 and b1?
Start with we jiggle the values, to find the best estimates which minimise SSE Iterative approach Computer intensive – used to matter, doesn’t really any more (With fast computers and sensible search algorithms – more on that later)

64 Start with b0=416 (mean) b1=0.5 (nice round number)
SSE = 365,774 b0=300, b1=0.5, SSE=341,683 b0=300, b1=0.6, SSE=310,240 b0=300, b1=0.8, SSE=264,573 b0=300, b1=1, SSE=301, 797 b0=250, b1=1, SSE=255,366 …..

65 Gives the position of the
Quite a long time later b0 = b1 = 1.084 SSE = 145,636.78 Gives the position of the Regression line (or) Line of best fit Better than guessing Not necessarily the only method But it is OLS, so it is the best (it is BLUE)

66

67 We now know Told us two things
A zero square metre house is worth  $216,000 Adding a square meter adds $1,080 Told us two things Don’t extrapolate to meaningless values of x-axis Constant is not necessarily useful It is necessary to estimate the equation

68 Exercise 2a, 2b

69 Standardised Regression Line
One big but: Scale dependent Values change £ to €, inflation Scales change £, £000, £00? Need to deal with this

70 Don’t express in ‘raw’ units
Express in SD units sx1=183.82 sy= b1 = 1.103 We increase x1 by 1, and Ŷ increases by 1.084 So we increase x1 by 1 and Ŷ increases by SDs

71 Similarly, 1 unit of x1 = 1/69.017 SDs
Increase x1 by 1 SD Ŷ increases by  (69.017/1) = Put them both together

72 The standardised regression line
Change (in SDs) in Ŷ associated with a change of 1 SD in x1 A different route to the same answer Standardise both variables (divide by SD) Find line of best fit

73 The Correlation Coefficient
The standardised regression line has a special name The Correlation Coefficient (r) (r stands for ‘regression’, but more on that later) Correlation coefficient is a standardised regression slope Relative change, in terms of SDs

74 Exercise 2c

75 Proportional Reduction in Error

76 Proportional Reduction in Error
We might be interested in the level of improvement of the model How much less error (as proportion) do we have Proportional Reduction in Error (PRE) Mean only Error(model 0) = 341,683 Mean + slope Error(model 1) = 196,046

77

78 But we squared all the errors in the first place
So we could take the square root This is the correlation coefficient Correlation coefficient is the square root of the proportion of variance explained

79 Standardised Covariance

80 Standardised Covariance
We are still iterating Need a ‘closed-form’ equation Equation to solve to get the parameter estimates Answer is a standardised covariance A variable has variance Amount of ‘differentness’ We have used SSE so far

81 SSE varies with N Divide by N Gives us the variance Same as SD2
Higher N, higher SSE Divide by N Gives SSE per person (or house) (Actually N – 1, we have lost a df to the mean) Gives us the variance Same as SD2 We thought of SSE as a scattergram Y plotted against X (repeated image follows)

82

83 Or we could plot Y against Y
Axes meet at the mean (415) Draw a square for each point Calculate an area for each square Sum the areas Sum of areas SSE Sum of areas divided by N Variance

84 Plot of Y against Y 20 40 60 80 100 120 140 160 180

85 Draw Squares 138 – 88.9 = 40.1 Area = 40.1 x 40.1 = 1608.1 138 – 88.9
20 40 60 80 100 120 140 160 180 138 – 88.9 = 40.1 Area = 40.1 x 40.1 = 138 – 88.9 = 40.1 35 – 88.9 = -53.9 Area = -53.9 x -53.9 = 35 – 88.9 = -53.9

86 What if we do the same procedure
Instead of Y against Y Y against X Draw rectangles (not squares) Sum the area Divide by N - 1 This gives us the variance of x with y The Covariance Shortened to Cov(x, y)

87

88 Area = (-33.9) x (-2) = 67.8 = 49.1 55 – 88.9 = -33.9 4 - 3 = 1 1 - 3 = -2 Area = 49.1 x 1 = 49.1

89 More formally (and easily)
We can state what we are doing as an equation Where Cov(x, y) is the covariance Cov(x,y)=5165 What do points in different sectors do to the covariance?

90 Problem with the covariance
Tells us about two things The variance of X and Y The covariance Need to standardise it Like the slope Two ways to standardise the covariance Standardise the variables first Subtract from mean and divide by SD Standardise the covariance afterwards

91 Need the combined variance
First approach Much more computationally expensive Too much like hard work to do by hand Need to standardise every value Second approach Much easier Standardise the final value only Need the combined variance Multiply two variances Find square root (were multiplied in first place)

92 Standardised covariance

93 The correlation coefficient
A standardised covariance is a correlation coefficient

94 Expanded …

95 This means … We now have a closed form equation to calculate the correlation Which is the standardised slope Which we can use to calculate the unstandardised slope

96 We know that: We know that:

97 So value of b1 is the same as the iterative approach

98 The variables are centred at zero
The intercept Just while we are at it The variables are centred at zero We subtracted the mean from both variables Intercept is zero, because the axes cross at the mean

99 Add mean of y to the constant Subtract mean of x
Adjusts for centring y Subtract mean of x But not the whole mean of x Need to correct it for the slope

100 Accuracy of Prediction

101 One More (Last One) We have one more way to calculate the correlation
Looking at the accuracy of the prediction Use the parameters b0 and b1 To calculate a predicted value for each case

102 Plot actual price against predicted price
From the model

103

104 Seems a futile thing to do
r = 0.653 The correlation between actual and predicted value Seems a futile thing to do And at this stage, it is But later on, we will see why

105 Some More Formulae For hand calculation Point biserial

106 Phi (f) Used for 2 dichotomous variables Vote P Vote Q Homeowner A: 19
Not homeowner C: 60 D:53

107 Problem with the phi correlation
Unless Px= Py (or Px = 1 – Py) Maximum (absolute) value is < 1.00 Tetrachoric correlation can be used to correct this Rank (Spearman) correlation Used where data are ranked

108 Summary Mean is an OLS estimate Regression line
OLS estimates are BLUE Regression line Best prediction of outcome from predictor OLS estimate (like mean) Standardised regression line A correlation

109 Four ways to think about a correlation
1. Standardised regression line 2. Proportional Reduction in Error (PRE) 3. Standardised covariance 4. Accuracy of prediction

110 Regression and Correlation in Stata
correlate x y correlate x y , cov regress y x Or regress price sqm

111 Post-Estimation Stata commands ‘leave behind’ something
You can run post-estimation commands They mean ‘from the last regression’ Get predicted values: predict my_preds Get residuals: predict my_res, residuals This comes after the comma, so it’s an option

112 Graphs Scatterplot Regression line Both graphs lfit price beds
scatter price beds Regression line lfit price beds Both graphs twoway (scatter price beds) (lfit price beds)

113 What happens if you run reg without a predictor?
regress price

114 Exercises

115 Lesson 3: Samples to Populations – Standard Errors and Statistical Significance

116 The Problem In Social Sciences Theoretically
We investigate samples Theoretically Randomly taken from a specified population Every member has an equal chance of being sampled Sampling one member does not alter the chances of sampling another Not the case in (say) physics, biology, etc.

117 Population But it’s the population that we are interested in
Not the sample Population statistic represented with Greek letter Hat means ‘estimate’

118 Sample statistics (e.g. mean) estimate population parameters
Want to know Likely size of the parameter If it is > 0

119 Sampling Distribution
We need to know the sampling distribution of a parameter estimate How much does it vary from sample to sample If we make some assumptions We can know the sampling distribution of many statistics Start with the mean

120 Sampling Distribution of the Mean
Given Normal distribution Random sample Continuous data Mean has a known sampling distribution Repeatedly sampling will give a known distribution of means Centred around the true (population) mean (m)

121 Analysis Example: Memory
Difference in memory for different words 10 participants given a list of 30 words to learn, and then tested Two types of word Abstract: e.g. love, justice Concrete: e.g. carrot, table

122

123 Confidence Intervals This means Using
If we know the mean in our sample We can estimate where the mean in the population (m) is likely to be Using The standard error (se) of the mean Represents the standard deviation of the sampling distribution of the mean

124 1 SD contains 68% Almost 2 SDs contain 95%

125 We know the sampling distribution of the mean
t distributed if N < 30 Normal with large N (>30) Asymptotically normal Know the range within means from other samples will fall Therefore the likely range of m

126 Two implications of equation
Increasing N decreases SE But only a bit (SE halfs if N is 400 times bigger) Decreasing SD decreases SE Calculate Confidence Intervals From standard errors 95% is a standard level of CI 95% of samples the true mean will lie within the 95% CIs In large samples: 95% CI = 1.96  SE In smaller samples: depends on t distribution (df=N-1=9)

127

128

129 What is a CI? (For 95% CI): 95% chance that the true (population) value lies within the confidence interval? No; 95% of samples, true mean will land within the confidence interval?

130 Significance Test Probability that m is a certain value
Almost always 0 Doesn’t have to be though We want to test the hypothesis that the difference is equal to 0 i.e. find the probability of this difference occurring in our sample IF m=0 (Not the same as the probability that m=0)

131 Calculate SE, and then t t has a known sampling distribution
Can test probability that a certain value is included

132 Other Parameter Estimates
Same approach Prediction, slope, intercept, predicted values At this point, prediction and slope are the same Won’t be later on One predictor only More complicated with > 1

133 Testing the Degree of Prediction
Prediction is correlation of Y with Ŷ The correlation – when we have one IV Use F, rather than t Started with SSE for the mean only This is SStotal Divide this into SSresidual SSregression SStot = SSreg + SSres

134

135 Back to the house prices
Original SSE (SStotal) = SSresidual = What is left after our model SSregression = – = What our model explains

136

137 F = 18.6, df = 1, 25, p = 0.0002 Can reject H0 A significant effect
H0: Prediction is not better than chance A significant effect

138 Statistical Significance: What does a p-value (really) mean?

139 A Quiz Six questions, each true or false
Write down your answers (if you like) An experiment has been done. Carried out perfectly. All assumptions perfectly satisfied. Absolutely no problems. P = 0.01 Which of the following can we say?

140 1. You have absolutely disproved the null hypothesis (that is, there is no difference between the population means).

141 2. You have found the probability of the null hypothesis being true.

142 3. You have absolutely proved your experimental hypothesis (that there is a difference between the population means).

143 4. You can deduce the probability of the experimental hypothesis being true.

144 5. You know, if you decide to reject the null hypothesis, the probability that you are making the wrong decision.

145 6. You have a reliable experimental finding in the sense that if, hypothetically, the experiment were repeated a great number of times, you would obtain a significant result on 99% of occasions.

146 OK, What is a p-value Cohen (1994)
“[a p-value] does not tell us what we want to know, and we so much want to know what we want to know that, out of desperation, we nevertheless believe it does” (p 997).

147 OK, What is a p-value Sorry, didn’t answer the question
It’s “The probability of obtaining a result as or more extreme than the result we have in the study, given that the null hypothesis is true” Not probability the null hypothesis is true

148 A Bit of Notation Not because we like notation Probability – P
But we have to say a lot less Probability – P Null hypothesis is true – H Result (data) – D Given - |

149 What’s a P Value P(D|H) Not P(H|D) (what we want to know)
Probability of the data occurring if the null hypothesis is true Not P(H|D) (what we want to know) Probability that the null hypothesis is true, given that we have the data = p(H) P(H|D) ≠ P(D|H)

150 What is probability you are prime minister
Given that you are British P(M|B) Very low What is probability you are British Given you are prime minister P(B|M) Very high P(M|B) ≠ P(B|M)

151 The police have your DNA DNA matches 1 in 1,000,000 people
There’s been a murder Someone murdered an instructor (perhaps they talked too much) The police have DNA The police have your DNA They match(!) DNA matches 1 in 1,000,000 people What’s the probability you didn’t do the murder, given the DNA match (H|D)

152 Luckily, you have Jeremy on your defence team We say:
Police say: P(D|H) = 1/1,000,000 Luckily, you have Jeremy on your defence team We say: P(D|H) ≠ P(H|D) Probability that someone matches the DNA, who didn’t do the murder Incredibly high

153 Back to the Questions Haller and Kraus (2002)
Asked those questions of groups in Germany Psychology Students Psychology lecturers and professors (who didn’t teach stats) Psychology lecturers and professors (who did teach stats)

154 We have found evidence against the null hypothesis
You have absolutely disproved the null hypothesis (that is, there is no difference between the population means). True 34% of students 15% of professors/lecturers, 10% of professors/lecturers teaching statistics False We have found evidence against the null hypothesis

155 You have found the probability of the null hypothesis being true.
32% of students 26% of professors/lecturers 17% of professors/lecturers teaching statistics False We don’t know

156 3. You have absolutely proved your experimental hypothesis (that there is a difference between the population means). 20% of students 13% of professors/lecturers 10% of professors/lecturers teaching statistics False

157 You can deduce the probability of the experimental hypothesis being true.
59% of students 33% of professors/lecturers 33% of professors/lecturers teaching statistics False

158 You know, if you decide to reject the null hypothesis, the probability that you are making the wrong decision. 68% of students 67% of professors/lecturers 73% of professors professors/lecturers teaching statistics False Can be worked out P(replication)

159 You have a reliable experimental finding in the sense that if, hypothetically, the experiment were repeated a great number of times, you would obtain a significant result on 99% of occasions. 41% of students 49% of professors/lecturers 37% of professors professors/lecturers teaching statistics False Another tricky one It can be worked out

160 One Last Quiz I carry out a study You replicate the study exactly
All assumptions perfectly satisfied Random sample from population I find p = 0.05 You replicate the study exactly What is probability you find p < 0.05?

161 You replicate the study exactly
I carry out a study All assumptions perfectly satisfied Random sample from population I find p = 0.01 You replicate the study exactly What is probability you find p < 0.05?

162 Significance testing creates boundaries and gaps where none exist.
Significance testing means that we find it hard to build upon knowledge we don’t get an accumulation of knowledge

163 Yates (1951) "the emphasis given to formal tests of significance ... has resulted in ... an undue concentration of effort by mathematical statisticians on investigations of tests of significance applicable to problems which are of little or no practical importance ... and ... it has caused scientific research workers to pay undue attention to the results of the tests of significance ... and too little to the estimates of the magnitude of the effects they are investigating

164 Testing the Slope Same idea as with the mean
Estimate 95% CI of slope Estimate significance of difference from a value (usually 0) Need to know the SD of the slope Similar to SD of the mean

165

166 Similar to equation for SD of mean Then we need standard error
Similar (ish) When we have standard error Can go on to 95% CI Significance of difference

167

168 Confidence Limits 95% CI 95% confidence limits
t dist with N - k - 1 df is 2.31 CI = 5.24  2.31 = 12.06 95% confidence limits

169 Significance of difference from zero
i.e. probability of getting result if b=0 Not probability that b = 0 This probability is (of course) the same as the value for the prediction

170 Testing the Standardised Slope (Correlation)
Correlation is bounded between –1 and +1 Does not have symmetrical distribution, except around 0 Need to transform it Fisher z’ transformation – approximately normal

171 95% CIs 0.879 – 1.96 * 0.38 = 0.13 * 0.38 = 1.62

172 Transform back to correlation
95% CIs = 0.13 to 0.92 Very wide Because of small sample size Maybe that’s why CIs are not reported?

173 Using Excel Functions in excel
Fisher() – to carry out Fisher transformation Fisherinv() – to transform back to correlation

174 The Others Same ideas for calculation of CIs and SEs for
Predicted score Gives expected range of values given X Same for intercept But we have probably had enough

175 One more tricky thing (Don’t worry if you don’t understand)
For means, regression estimates, etc Estimate 1.0000 95% confidence intervals 0.0000, P = They match

176 For correlations, odds ratios, etc 95% CIs P-value
No longer match 95% CIs 0.0000, P-value Because of the sampling distribution of the mean Does not depend on the value The sampling distribution of a proportion Does depend on the value More certainty around 0.9 than around 0.00.

177 Lesson 4: Introducing Multiple Regression

178 Residuals We said We could have said We ignored the i on the Y
Y = b0 + b1x1 We could have said Yi = b0 + b1xi1 + ei We ignored the i on the Y And we ignored the ei It’s called error, after all But it isn’t just error Trying to tell us something

179 What Error Tells Us Error tells us that a case has a different score for Y than we predict There is something about that case Called the residual What is left over, after the model Contains information Something is making the residual  0 But what?

180

181

182 If all cases were equal on X
The residual (+ the mean) is the expected value of Y If all cases were equal on X It is the value of Y, controlling for X Other words: Holding constant Partialling Residualising (residualised scores) Conditioned on

183 Sometimes adjustment is enough on its own Teenage pregnancy rate
Measure performance against criteria Teenage pregnancy rate Measure pregnancy and abortion rate in areas Control for socio-economic deprivation, religion, rural/urban and anything else important See which areas have lower teenage pregnancy and abortion rate, given same level of deprivation Value added education tables Measure school performance Control for initial intake

184 Sqm Price Predicted Residual Adj Value (mean + resid) 239.2 605.0 475.77 129.23 544.8 177.4 400.0 408.78 -8.78 406.8 265.3 529.5 504.08 25.42 441.0 153.4 315.0 382.69 -67.69 347.9 136.2 341.0 364.05 -23.05 392.6 187.5 525.0 419.66 105.34 520.9 280.5 585.0 520.51 64.49 480.1 203.3 430.0 436.79 -6.79 408.8 141.1 436.0 369.39 66.61 482.2 130.3 390.0 357.70 32.30 447.9

185 Control? In experimental research In non-experimental research
Use experimental control e.g. same conditions, materials, time of day, accurate measures, random assignment to conditions In non-experimental research Can’t use experimental control Use statistical control instead

186 Analysis of Residuals What predicts differences in crime rate
After controlling for socio-economic deprivation Number of police? Crime prevention schemes? Rural/Urban proportions? Something else This is (mostly) what multiple regression is about

187 Books and attend as IV, grade as outcome
Exam performance Consider number of books a student read (books) Number of lectures (max 20) a student attended (attend) Books and attend as IV, grade as outcome

188 First 10 cases

189 Use books as IV Use attend as IV R=0.492, F=12.1, df=1, 28, p=0.001
b0=52.1, b1=5.7 (Intercept makes sense) Use attend as IV R=0.482, F=11.5, df=1, 38, p=0.002 b0=37.0, b1=1.9 (Intercept makes less sense)

190 100 90 80 70 60 50 Grade (100) 40 30 -1 1 2 3 4 5 Books

191

192 Problem Use R2 to give proportion of shared variance
Books = 24% Attend = 23% So we have explained 24% + 23% = 47% of the variance NO!!!!!

193 Look at the correlation matrix
BOOKS ATTEND GRADE 1 0.44 0.49 0.48 Correlation of books and attend is (unsurprisingly) not zero Some of the variance that books shares with grade, is also shared by attend

194 My wife has access to 2 cars
I have access to 2 cars My wife has access to 2 cars We have access to four cars? No. We need to know how many of my 2 cars are shared Similarly with regression But we can do this with the residuals Residuals are what is left after (say) books See if residual variance is explained by attend Can use this new residual variance to calculate SSres, SStotal and SSreg

195 Because assumes that the variables have a causal priority
Well. Almost. This would give us correct values for SS Would not be correct for slopes, etc Because assumes that the variables have a causal priority Why should attend have to take what is left from books? Why should books have to take what is left by attend? Use OLS again; take variance they share

196 Simultaneously estimate 2 parameters
b1 and b2 Y = b0 + b1x1 + b2x2 x1 and x2 are IVs Shared variance Not trying to fit a line any more Trying to fit a plane Can solve iteratively Closed form equations better But they are unwieldy

197 3D scatterplot (2points only) y x2 x1

198 b2 y b1 b0 x2 x1

199

200 Increasing Power What if the predictors don’t correlate?
Regression is still good It increases the power to detect effects (More on power later) Less variance left over When do we know the two predictors don’t correlate?

201 (Really) Ridiculous Equations

202 The good news The bad news There is an easier way
It involves matrix algebra We don’t really need to know how to do it

203 We’re not programming computers
So we usually don’t care Very, very occasionally it helps to know what the computer is doing

204 Back to the Good News We can calculate the standardised parameters as
B=Rxx-1 x Rxy Where B is the vector of regression weights Rxx-1 is the inverse of the correlation matrix of the independent (x) variables Rxy is the vector of correlations of the correlations of the x and y variables

205 Exercise 4.2

206 Exercises Exercise 4.1 Exercise 4.2 Exercise 4.3 Exercise 4.4
Grades data in Excel Exercise 4.2 Repeat in Stata Exercise 4.3 Zero correlation Exercise 4.4 Repeat therapy data Exercise 4.5 PTSD in families.

207 Lesson 5: More on Multiple Regression

208 Contents More on parameter estimates R, R2, adjusted R2 Extra bits
Standard errors of coefficients R, R2, adjusted R2 Extra bits Suppressors Decisions about control variables Standardized estimates > 1 Variable entry techniques

209 More on Parameter Estimates

210 Parameter Estimates Parameter estimates (b1, b2 … bk) were standardised Because we analysed a correlation matrix Represent the correlation of each IV with the outcome When all other IVs are held constant

211 Can also be unstandardised
Unstandardised represent the unit (rather than SD’s) change in the outcome associated with a 1 unit change in the IV When all the other variables are held constant Parameters have standard errors associated with them As with one IV Hence t-test, and associated probability can be calculated Trickier than with one IV

212 Standard Error of Regression Coefficient
Standardised is easier R2i is the value of R2 when all other predictors are used as predictors of that variable Note that if R2i = 0, the equation is the same as for previous

213 Multiple R

214 Multiple R The degree of prediction
R (or Multiple R) No longer equal to b R2 Might be equal to the sum of squares of B Only if all x’s are uncorrelated

215 In Terms of Variance Can also think of R2 in terms of variance explained. Each IV explains some variance in the outcome The IVs share some of their variance Can’t share the same variance twice

216 Variance in Y accounted for by x1 rx1y2 = 0.36
The total variance of Y = 1

217 In this model But R2 = ryx12 + ryx22 R2 = 0.36 + 0.36 = 0.72
If x1 and x2 are correlated No longer the case

218 Variance in Y accounted for by x1 rx1y2 = 0.36
Variance shared between x1 and x2 (not equal to rx1x2) The total variance of Y = 1 Variance in Y accounted for by x2 rx2y2 = 0.36

219 So But Two different ways We can no longer sum the r2
Need to sum them, and subtract the shared variance – i.e. the correlation But It’s not the correlation between them It’s the correlation between them as a proportion of the variance of Y Two different ways

220 Based on estimates If rx1x2 = 0 rxy = bx1 Equivalent to ryx12 + ryx22

221 Based on correlations rx1x2 = 0 Equivalent to ryx12 + ryx22

222 Can also be calculated using methods we have seen
Based on PRE (predicted value) Based on correlation with prediction Same procedure with >2 IVs

223 Adjusted R2 R2 is on average an overestimate of population value of R2
Any x will not correlate 0 with Y Any variation away from 0 increases R Variation from 0 more pronounced with lower N Need to correct R2 Adjusted R2

224 Calculation of Adj. R2 1 – R2 Proportion of unexplained variance
We multiple this by an adjustment More variables – greater adjustment More people – less adjustment

225

226 Some stranger things that can happen Counter-intuitive
Extra Bits Some stranger things that can happen Counter-intuitive

227 Suppressor variables Can be hard to understand Definition
Very counter-intuitive Definition A predictor which increases the size of the parameters associated with other predictors above the size of their correlations

228 An example (based on Horst, 1941)
Success of trainee pilots Mechanical ability (x1), verbal ability (x2), success (y) Correlation matrix

229 Mechanical ability correlates 0.3 with success
Verbal ability correlates 0.0 with success What will the parameter estimates be? (Don’t look ahead until you have had a guess)

230 Mechanical ability Verbal ability So what is happening? b = 0.4
Larger than r! Verbal ability b = -0.2 Smaller than r!! So what is happening? You need verbal ability to do the mechanical ability test Not actually related to mechanical ability Measure of mechanical ability is contaminated by verbal ability

231 High mech, low verbal High mech, high verbal High mech Low verbal
This is positive (.4) Low verbal Negative, because we are talking about standardised scores (-(-.2)  (.2) Your mech is really high – you did well on the mechanical test, without being good at the words High mech, high verbal Well, you had a head start on mech, because of verbal, and need to be brought down a bit

232 Another suppressor? b1 = b2 =

233 Another suppressor? b1 =0.26 b2 = -0.06

234 And another? b1 = b2 =

235 And another? b1 = 0.53 b2 = -0.47

236 One more? b1 = b2 =

237 One more? b1 = 0.53 b2 = 0.47

238 Suppression happens when two opposing forces are happening together
And have opposite effects Don’t throw away your IVs, Just because they are uncorrelated with the outcome Be careful in interpretation of regression estimates Really need the correlations too, to interpret what is going on Cannot compare between studies with different predictors Think about what you want to know Before throwing variables into the analysis

239 What to Control For? What is the added value of a ‘better’ college
In terms of salary More academic people go to ‘better’ colleges Control for: Ability? Social class? Mother’s education? Parent’s income? Course? Ethnic group? …

240 Decisions about control variables Effect of gender
Guided from theory Effect of gender Controlling for hair length and skirt wearing?

241

242 Do dogs make kids healthier?
What to control for? Parent’s weight? Yes: Obese parents are more likely to have obese kids, kids who are thinner, relative to the parents are thinner. No: Dog might make parent thinner. By controlling for parental weight, you’re controlling for the effect of dog

243

244 Bad control vars Bad control vars Dog Kid’s health Good control vars

245 Parent Weight Child Asthma Dog Kid’s health Rural/Urban? House/apartment? Income

246 Standardised Estimates > 1
Correlations are bounded -1.00 ≤ r ≤ +1.00 We think of standardised regression estimates as being similarly bounded But they are not Can go >1.00, <-1.00 R cannot, because that is a proportion of variance

247 Three measures of ability
Mechanical ability, verbal ability 1, verbal ability 2 Score on science exam Before reading on, what are the parameter estimates?

248 Mechanical About where we expect Verbal 1 Very high Verbal 2 Very low

249 Verbal 1 and verbal 2 are correlated so highly
What is going on It’s a suppressor again a predictor which increases the size of the parameters associated with other predictors above the size of their correlations Verbal 1 and verbal 2 are correlated so highly They need to cancel each other out

250 Variable Selection What are the appropriate predictors to use in a model? Depends what you are trying to do Multiple regression has two separate uses Prediction Explanation

251 Prediction Explanation What will happen in the future?
Emphasis on practical application Variables selected (more) empirically Value free Explanation Why did something happen? Emphasis on understanding phenomena Variables selected theoretically Not value free

252 More on causality later on … Which are appropriate variables
Visiting the doctor Precedes suicide attempts Predicts suicide Does not explain suicide More on causality later on … Which are appropriate variables To collect data on? To include in analysis? Decision needs to be based on theoretical knowledge of the behaviour of those variables Statistical analysis of those variables (later) Unless you didn’t collect the data Common sense (not a useful thing to say)

253 Variable Entry Techniques
Entry-wise All variables entered simultaneously Hierarchical Variables entered in a predetermined order Stepwise Variables entered according to change in R2 Actually a family of techniques

254 Hierarchical regression
Entrywise regression All variables entered simultaneously All treated equally Hierarchical regression Entered in a theoretically determined order Change in R2 is assessed, and tested for significance e.g. sex and age Should not be treated equally with other variables Sex and age MUST be first (unchangeable) Confused with hierarchical linear modelling (MLM)

255 R-Squared Change SSE0, df0 SSE and df for first (smaller) model
SSE and df for second (larger) model

256 Stepwise Example Variables entered empirically
Variable which increases R2 the most goes first Then the next … Variables which have no effect can be removed from the equation Example House prices – what’s important? Size, lot size, list price,

257 Stepwise Analysis Data determines the order
Model 1: listing price, R2 = 0.87 Model 2: listing price + lot size, R2 = 0.89

258 Hierarchical analysis
Theory determines the order Model 1: Lot size+ House size, R2 = 0.499 Model 2: + List price, R2 = 0.905 Change in R2 = 0.41, p < 0.001

259 Other problems with stepwise
Which is the best model? Entrywise – OK Stepwise – excluded age Excluded size MOST IMPORTANT PREDICTOR Hierarchical Listing price accounted for additional variance Whoever decides the price has information that we don’t Other problems with stepwise F and df are wrong (cheats with df) Unstable results Small changes (sampling variance) – large differences in models

260 Uses a lot of paper Don’t use a stepwise procedure to pack your suitcase

261 Is Stepwise Always Evil?
Yes All right, no Research goal is entirely predictive (technological) Not explanatory (scientific) What happens, not why N is large 40 people per predictor, Cohen, Cohen, Aiken, West (2003) Cross validation takes place

262 Alternatives to stepwise regression
More recently developed Used for genetic studies 1000s of predictors, one outcome, small samples Least Angle Regression LARS (least angle regression) Lasso (Least absolute shrinkage and selection operator)

263 Entry Methods in Stata Entrywise Hierarchical What regress does
Two ways Use hireg Add on module net search hireg Then install

264 Hierarchical Regression
Use (on one line) hireg outcome (block1var1 block1var2) (block2var1 block2var2) Hireg reports Parameter estimates for the two regressions R2 for each model, change in R2

265 Model R F(df) 1: (1,98) 2: (2,97) p R2 change F(df)change p 0.136 (1,97) P value for the R2 P value for the change in R2

266 Hierarchical Regression (Cont…)
I don’t like hireg, for two reasons It’s different to regression It only works for OLS regression, not logistic, multinomial, Poisson, etc Alternative 2: Use test The p-value associated with the change in R2 for a variable Equal to the p-value for that variable.

267 Hierarchical Regression (Cont…)
Example (using cars) Parameters from final model: hireg price () (extro) car | Coef. Std. Err. t P>|t| [95% Conf. Interval] extro | R2 change statistics R2 change F(df) change p (1,36) (What is relationship between t and F?) We know the p-value of the R2 change When there is one predictor in the block What about when there’s more than one?

268 Hierarchical Regression (Cont)
test isn’t exactly what we want But it is the same as what we want Advantage of test You can always use it (I can always remember how it works)

269 (For SPSS) SPSS calls them ‘blocks’
Enter some variables, click ‘next block’ Enter more variables Click on ‘Statistics’ Click on R-squared change

270 Stepwise Regression Add stepwise: prefix With
Pr() – probability value to be removed from equation Pe() – probability value to be entered into equation stepwise, pe(0.05) pr(0.2): reg price sqm lotsize originallis

271 A quick note on R2 R2 is sometimes regarded as the ‘fit’ of a regression model Bad idea If good fit is required – maximise R2 Leads to entering variables which do not make theoretical sense

272 Propensity Scores Another method of controlling for variables
Ensure that predictors are uncorrelated with one predictor Don’t need to control for them

273 x’s Uncorrelated? Two cases when x’s are uncorrelated
Experimental design Predictors are uncorrelated We randomly assigned people to conditions to ensure that was the case Sample weights We can deliberately sample Ensure that they are uncorrelated

274 Or use post hoc sample weights
20 women with college degree 20 women without college degree 20 men with college degree 20 men without college degree Or use post hoc sample weights Propensity weighting Weight to ensure that variables are uncorrelated Usually done to avoid having to control E.g. ethnic differences in PTSD symptoms Can incorporate many more control variables 100+

275 Propensity Scores Race profiling of police stops
Same time, place, area, etc

276 Critique of Multiple Regression
Goertzel (2002) “Myths of murder and multiple regression” Skeptical Inquirer (Paper B1) Econometrics and regression are ‘junk science’ Multiple regression models (in US) Used to guide social policy

277 More Guns, Less Crime Lott and Mustard: A 1% increase in gun ownership
(controlling for other factors) Lott and Mustard: A 1% increase in gun ownership 3.3% decrease in murder rates But: More guns in rural Southern US More crime in urban North (crack cocaine epidemic at time of data)

278 Executions Cut Crime No difference between crimes in states in US with or without death penalty Ehrlich (1975) controlled all variables that affect crime rates Death penalty had effect in reducing crime rate No statistical way to decide who’s right

279 Legalised Abortion Donohue and Levitt (1999) Lott and Whitley (2001)
Legalised abortion in 1970’s cut crime in 1990’s Lott and Whitley (2001) “Legalising abortion decreased murder rates by … 0.5 to 7 per cent.” It’s impossible to model these data Controlling for other historical events Crack cocaine (again)

280 Crime is still dropping in the US
Despite the recession Levitt says it’s mysterious, because the abortion effect should be over Some suggest Xboxes, Playstations, etc Netflix, DVRs (Violent movies reduce crime).

281 Another Critique Berk (2003) Three cheers for regression
Regression analysis: a constructive critique (Sage) Three cheers for regression As a descriptive technique Two cheers for regression As an inferential technique One cheer for regression As a causal analysis

282 Is Regression Useless? Do regression carefully Validate models
Don’t go beyond data which you have a strong theoretical understanding of Validate models Where possible, validate predictive power of models in other areas, times, groups Particularly important with stepwise

283 Lesson 6: Categorical Predictors

284 Introduction

285 Introduction So far, just looked at continuous predictors
Also possible to use categorical (nominal, qualitative) predictors e.g. Sex; Job; Religion; Region; Type (of anything) Usually analysed with t-test/ANOVA

286 Historical Note But these (t-test/ANOVA) are special cases of regression analysis Aspects of General Linear Models (GLMs) So why treat them differently? Fisher’s fault Computers’ fault Regression, as we have seen, is computationally difficult Matrix inversion and multiplication Can’t do it, without a computer

287 In the special cases where:
You have one categorical predictor Your IVs are uncorrelated It is much easier to do it by partitioning of sums of squares These cases Very rare in ‘applied’ research Very common in ‘experimental’ research Fisher worked at Rothamsted agricultural research station Never have problems manipulating wheat, pigs, cabbages, etc

288 Still (too) common to dichotomise a variable
In psychology Led to a split between ‘experimental’ psychologists and ‘correlational’ psychologists Experimental psychologists (until recently) would not think in terms of continuous variables Still (too) common to dichotomise a variable Too difficult to analyse it properly Equivalent to discarding 1/3 of your data

289 The Approach

290 The Approach Recode the nominal variable Names are slightly confusing
Into one, or more, variables to represent that variable Names are slightly confusing Some texts talk of ‘dummy coding’ to refer to all of these techniques Some (most) refer to ‘dummy coding’ to refer to one of them Most have more than one name

291 If a variable has g possible categories it is represented by g-1 variables
Simplest case: Smokes: Yes or No Variable 1 represents ‘Yes’ Variable 2 is redundant If it isn’t yes, it’s no

292 The Techniques

293 We will examine two coding schemes
Dummy coding For two groups For >2 groups Effect coding Look at analysis of change Equivalent to ANCOVA Pretest-posttest designs

294 Dummy Coding – 2 Groups Sometimes called ‘simple coding’
A categorical variable with two groups One group chosen as a reference group The other group is represented in a variable e.g. 2 groups: Experimental (Group 1) and Control (Group 0) Control is the reference group Dummy variable represents experimental group Call this variable ‘group1’

295 For variable ‘group1’ 1 = ‘Yes’, 0=‘No’

296 Some data Group is x, score is y

297 Control Group = 0 Experimental Group = 1
Intercept = Score on Y when x = 0 Intercept = mean of control group Experimental Group = 1 b = change in Y when x increases 1 unit b = difference between experimental group and control group

298 Gradient of slope represents difference between means

299 Dummy Coding – 3+ Groups With three groups the approach is the similar
g = 3, therefore g-1 = 2 variables needed 3 Groups Control Experimental Group 1 Experimental Group 2

300 Recoded into two variables
Note – do not need a 3rd variable If we are not in group 1 or group 2 MUST be in control group 3rd variable would add no information (What would happen to determinant?)

301 b1 and b2 and associated p-values
F and associated p Tests H0 that b1 and b2 and associated p-values Test difference between each experimental group and the reference group To test difference between experimental groups Need to rerun analysis (or just do ANOVA with post-hoc tests)

302 Need to correct for this
One more complication Have now run multiple comparisons Increases a – i.e. probability of type I error Need to correct for this Bonferroni correction Multiply given p-values by two/three (depending how many comparisons were made)

303 Effect Coding Usually used for 3+ groups
Compares each group (except the reference group) to the mean of all groups Dummy coding compares each group to the reference group. Example with 5 groups 1 group selected as reference group Group 5

304 Each group (except reference) has a variable
1 if the individual is in that group 0 if not -1 if in reference group

305 Examples Dummy coding and Effect Coding
Group 1 chosen as reference group each time Data

306 Dummy Group dummy2 dummy3 1 2 3 Effect Group Effect2 effect3 1 -1 2 3

307 Dummy R=0.543, F=5.7, df=2, 27, p=0.009 b0 = 52.4, b1 = 3.9, p=0.100 b2 = 7.7, p=0.002 Effect R=0.543, F=5.7, df=2, 27, p=0.009 b0 = 56.27, b1 = 0.03, p=0.980 b2 = 3.8, p=0.007

308 In Stata Use xi: prefix for dummy coding
Use xi3: module for more codings But I don’t like it, I do it by hand I don’t understand what it’s doing It makes very long variables And then I can’t use test BUT: If doing stepwise, you need to keep the variables together Example: xi: reg outcome contpred i.catpred Put i. in front of categorical predictors This has changed in Stata 11. xi: no longer needed

309 xi: reg salary i.job_description
salary | Coef. Std. Err t P>|t| _Ijob_desc~2 | _Ijob_desc~3 | _cons |

310 Exercise 6.1 5 golf balls Which is best?

311 In SPSS SPSS provides two equivalent procedures for regression
GLM GLM will: Automatically code categorical variables Automatically calculate interaction terms Allow you to not understand GLM won’t: Give standardised effects Give hierarchical R2 p-values

312 ANCOVA and Regression

313 Test (Which is a trick; but it’s designed to make you think about it) Use bank data (Ex 5.3) Compare the pay rise (difference between salbegin and salary) For ethnic minority and non-minority staff What do you find?

314 ANCOVA and Regression Dummy coding approach has one special use
In ANCOVA, for the analysis of change Pre-test post-test experimental design Control group and (one or more) experimental groups Tempting to use difference score + t-test / mixed design ANOVA Inappropriate

315 Salivary cortisol levels
Used as a measure of stress Not absolute level, but change in level over day may be interesting Test at: 9.00am, 9.00pm Two groups High stress group (cancer biopsy) Group 1 Low stress group (no biopsy) Group 0

316 Correlation of AM and PM = 0.493 (p=0.008)
Has there been a significant difference in the rate of change of salivary cortisol? 3 different approaches

317 Approach 1 – find the differences, do a t-test
t = 1.31, df=26, p=0.203 Approach 2 – mixed ANOVA, look for interaction effect F = 1.71, df = 1, 26, p = 0.203 F = t2 Approach 3 – regression (ANCOVA) based approach

318 Why is the regression approach better?
IVs: AM and group outcome: PM b1 (group) = 3.59, standardised b1=0.432, p = 0.01 Why is the regression approach better? The other two approaches took the difference Assumes that r = 1.00 Any difference from r = 1.00 and you add error variance Subtracting error is the same as adding error

319 Using regression Two effects Data is am-pm cortisol
Ensures that all the variance that is subtracted is true Reduces the error variance Two effects Adjusts the means Compensates for differences between groups Removes error variance Data is am-pm cortisol

320 More on Change If difference score is correlated with either pre-test or post-test Subtraction fails to remove the difference between the scores If two scores are uncorrelated Difference will be correlated with both Failure to control Equal SDs, r = 0 Correlation of change and pre-score =0.707

321 Even More on Change A topic of surprising complexity
What I said about difference scores isn’t always true Lord’s paradox – it depends on the precise question you want to answer Collins and Horn (1993). Best methods for the analysis of change Collins and Sayer (2001). New methods for the analysis of change More later

322 Lesson 7: Assumptions in Regression Analysis

323 The Assumptions The distribution of residuals is normal (at each value of the outcome). The variance of the residuals for every set of values for the predictor is equal. violation is called heteroscedasticity. The error term is additive no interactions. At every value of the outcome the expected (mean) value of the residuals is zero No non-linear relationships

324 The expected correlation between residuals, for any two cases, is 0.
The independence assumption (lack of autocorrelation) All predictors are uncorrelated with the error term. No predictors are a perfect linear function of other predictors (no perfect multicollinearity) The mean of the error term is zero.

325 What are we going to do … Deal with some of these assumptions in some detail Deal with others in passing only look at them again later on

326 Assumption 1: The Distribution of Residuals is Normal at Every Value of the outcome

327 Look at Normal Distributions
A normal distribution symmetrical, bell-shaped (so they say)

328 What can go wrong? Skew Kurtosis Outliers non-symmetricality
one tail longer than the other Kurtosis too flat or too peaked kurtosed Outliers Individual cases which are far from the distribution

329 Effects on the Mean Skew Kurtosis
biases the mean, in direction of skew Kurtosis mean not biased standard deviation is and hence standard errors, and significance tests

330 Examining Univariate Distributions
Graphs Histograms Boxplots P-P plots Calculation based methods

331 Histograms A and B

332 C and D

333 E & F

334 Histograms can be tricky ….

335 Boxplots

336 P-P Plots A & B

337 C & D

338 E & F

339 Calculation Based Skew and Kurtosis statistics
Outlier detection statistics

340 Skew and Kurtosis Statistics
Normal distribution skew = 0 kurtosis = 0 Two methods for calculation Fisher’s and Pearson’s Very similar answers Associated standard error can be used for significance (t-test) of departure from normality not actually very useful Never normal above N = 400

341

342 Outlier Detection Calculate distance from mean
z-score (number of standard deviations) deleted z-score that case biased the mean, so remove it Look up expected distance from mean 1% 3+ SDs

343 Non-Normality in Regression

344 Effects on OLS Estimates
The mean is an OLS estimate The regression line is an OLS estimate Lack of normality biases the position of the regression slope makes the standard errors wrong probability values attached to statistical significance wrong

345 Checks on Normality Check residuals are normally distributed
Draw histogram residuals Use regression diagnostics Lots of them Most aren’t very interesting

346 Regression Diagnostics
Residuals Standardised, studentised-deleted look for cases > |3| (?) Influence statistics Look for the effect a case has If we remove that case, do we get a different answer? DFBeta, Standardised DFBeta changes in b

347 Distances DfFit, Standardised DfFit
change in predicted value Distances measures of ‘distance’ from the centroid some include IV, some don’t

348 More on Residuals Residuals are trickier than you might have imagined
Raw residuals OK Standardised residuals Residuals divided by SD

349 Standardised / Studentised
Now we can calculate the standardised residuals SPSS calls them studentised residuals Also called internally studentised residuals

350 Deleted Studentised Residuals
Studentised residuals do not have a known distribution Cannot use them for inference Deleted studentised residuals Externally studentised residuals Studentized (jackknifed) residuals Distributed as t With df = N – k – 1

351 Testing Significance We can calculate the probability of a residual
Is it sampled from the same population BUT Massive type I error rate Bonferroni correct it Multiply p value by N

352 Bivariate Normality We didn’t just say “residuals normally distributed” We said “at every value of the outcomes” Two variables can be normally distributed – univariate, but not bivariate

353 Couple’s IQs male and female Seem reasonably normal

354 But wait!!

355 When we look at bivariate normality So plot X against Y
not normal – there is an outlier So plot X against Y OK for bivariate but – may be a multivariate outlier Need to draw graph in 3+ dimensions can’t draw a graph in 3 dimensions But we can look at the residuals instead …

356 IQ histogram of residuals

357 Multivariate Outliers …
Will be explored later in the exercises So we move on …

358 What to do about Non-Normality
Skew and Kurtosis Skew – much easier to deal with Kurtosis – less serious anyway Transform data removes skew positive skew – log transform negative skew - square

359 Transformation May need to transform IV and/or outcome
More often outcome time, income, symptoms (e.g. depression) all positively skewed can cause non-linear effects (more later) if only one is transformed alters interpretation of unstandardised parameter May alter meaning of variable Some people say that this is such a big problem Never transform May add / remove non-linear and moderator effects

360 Change measures Outliers increase sensitivity at ranges Can be tricky
avoiding floor and ceiling effects Outliers Can be tricky Why did the outlier occur? Error? Delete them. Weird person? Probably delete them Normal person? Tricky.

361 You are trying to model a process
is the data point ‘outside’ the process e.g. lottery winners, when looking at salary yawn, when looking at reaction time Which is better? A good model, which explains 99% of your data? (because we threw outliers out) A poor model, which explains all of it (because we keep outliers in) I prefer a good model

362 More on House Prices Zillow.com tracks and predicts house prices
In the USA Sometimes detects outliers We don’t trust this selling price We haven’t used it

363 Example in Stata reg salary educ predict res, res hist res
gen logsalary= log(salary) reg logsalary educ predict logres, res hist logres

364

365

366 But … Parameter estimates change
Interpretation of parameter estimate is different Exercise 7.0, 7.1

367 Bootstrapping Bootstrapping is very, very cool And very, very clever
But very, very simple

368 Bootstrapping When we estimate a test statistic (F or r or t or c2)
We rely on knowing the sampling distribution Which we know If the distributional assumptions are satisfied

369 Estimate the Distribution
Bootstrapping lets you: Skip the bit about distribution Estimate the sampling distribution from the data This shouldn’t be allowed Hence bootstrapping But it is

370 How to Bootstrap We resample, with replacement Take our sample
Sample 1 individual Put that individual back, so that they can be sampled again Sample another individual Keep going until we’ve sampled as many people as were in the sample Analyze the data Repeat the process B times Where B is a big number

371 Example Original B1 B2 B3 1 1 1 2 2 1 2 2 3 3 3 3 4 3 4 2 5 3 4 4 6 3 4 4 7 7 8 6 8 7 8 7 9 9 9 9 10 9 10 9

372 Analyze each dataset 2 approaches to CI or P Semi-parametric
Sampling distribution of statistic Gives sampling distribution 2 approaches to CI or P Semi-parametric Calculate standard error of statistic Call that the standard deviation Does not make assumption about distribution of data Makes assumption about sampling distribution

373 Non-parametric needs more samples
Stata calls this percentile Count. If you have 1000 samples 25th is lower CI 975th is upper CI P-value is proportion that cross zero Non-parametric needs more samples

374 Bootstrapping in Stata
Very easy: Use bootstrap: (or bs: or bstrap: ) prefix or (Better) use vce(bootstrap) option By default does 50 samples Not enough Use reps() At least 1000

375 Example Again reg salary salbegin educ, vce(bootstrap, reps(50))
| Observed Bootstrap | Coef Std. Err z salbegin | Again salbegin |

376 More Reps 1,000 reps Z = 17.31 Again Z = 17.59 10,000 reps 17.23 17.02

377 Exercise 7.2, 7.3

378 Assumption 2: The variance of the residuals for every set of values for the predictor is equal.

379 Heteroscedasticity This assumption is a about heteroscedasticity of the residuals Hetero=different Scedastic = scattered We don’t want heteroscedasticity we want our data to be homoscedastic Draw a scatterplot to investigate

380

381 Easy to get – use predicted values
Only works with one IV need every combination of IVs Easy to get – use predicted values use residuals there Plot predicted values against residuals A bit like turning the scatterplot to make the line of best fit flat

382 Good – no heteroscedasticity

383 Bad – heteroscedasticity

384 Testing Heteroscedasticity
White’s test Do regression, save residuals. Square residuals Square IVs Calculate interactions of IVs e.g. x1•x2, x1•x3, x2 • x3

385 Use education and salbegin to predict salary (employee data.sav)
Run regression using squared residuals as outcome IVs, squared IVs, and interactions as IVs Test statistic = N x R2 Distributed as c2 Df = k (for second regression) Use education and salbegin to predict salary (employee data.sav) R2 = 0.113, N=474, c2 = 53.5, df=5, p < Automatic in Stata estat imtest, white

386 Plot of Predicted and Residual

387 White’s Test as Test of Interest
Possible to have a theory that predicts heteroscedasticity Lupien, et al, 2006 Heteroscedasticity in relationship of hippocampal volume and age

388 Magnitude of Heteroscedasticity
Chop data into 5 “slices” Calculate variance of each slice Check ratio of smallest to largest Less than 5 OK

389 gen slice = 1 replace slice = 2 if pred > 30000 replace slice = 3 if pred > 60000 replace slice = 4 if pred > 90000 replace slice = 5 if pred > bysort slice: su pred 1: 3954 5: 17116 (Doesn’t look too bad, thanks to skew in predictors)

390 Dealing with Heteroscedasticity
Use Huber-White (robust) estimates Also called sandwich estimates Also called empirical estimates Use survey techniques Relatively straightforward in SAS and Stata, fiddly in SPSS Google: SPSS Huber-White

391 Why’s it a Sandwich? SE can be calculated with: Sandwich estimator:

392 Example reg salary educ reg salary educ , robust
Standard errors: 204, 2821 reg salary educ , robust 267 3347 SEs usually go up, can go down

393 Heteroscedasticity – Implications and Meanings
What happens as a result of heteroscedasticity? Parameter estimates are correct not biased Standard errors (hence p-values) are incorrect

394 However … If there is no skew in predicted scores If skewed,
P-values a tiny bit wrong If skewed, P-values can be very wrong Exercise 7.4

395 Robust SE Haiku T-stat looks too good. Use robust standard errors significance gone

396 What is heteroscedasticity trying to tell us?
Meaning What is heteroscedasticity trying to tell us? Our model is wrong – it is misspecified Something important is happening that we have not accounted for e.g. amount of money given to charity (given) depends on: earnings degree of importance person assigns to the charity (import)

397 Do the regression analysis
R2 = 0.60,, p < 0.001 seems quite good b0 = 0.24, p=0.97 b1 = 0.71, p < 0.001 b2 = 0.23, p = 0.031 White’s test c2 = 18.6, df=5, p=0.002 The plot of predicted values against residuals …

398 Plot shows heteroscedastic relationship

399 Which means … the effects of the variables are not additive
If you think that what a charity does is important you might give more money how much more depends on how much money you have

400

401 One more thing about heteroscedasticity
it is the equivalent of homogeneity of variance in ANOVA/t-tests

402 Exercise 7.4, 7.5, 7.6

403 Assumption 3: The Error Term is Additive

404 Additivity What heteroscedasticity shows you
effects of variables need to be additive (assume no interaction between the variables) Heteroscedasticity doesn’t always show it to you can test for it, but hard work (same as homogeneity of covariance assumption in ANCOVA) Have to know it from your theory A specification error

405 Additivity and Theory Two IVs Alcohol has sedative effect
A bit makes you a bit tired A lot makes you very tired Some painkillers have sedative effect A bit of alcohol and a bit of painkiller doesn’t make you very tired Effects multiply together, don’t add together

406 So many possible non-additive effects
If you don’t test for it It’s very hard to know that it will happen So many possible non-additive effects Cannot test for all of them Can test for obvious In medicine Choose to test for salient non-additive effects e.g. sex, race More on this, when we look at moderators

407 Exercise 7.6 Exercise 7.7

408 Assumption 4: At every value of the outcome the expected (mean) value of the residuals is zero

409 Linearity Relationships between variables should be linear
best represented by a straight line Not a very common problem in social sciences measures are not sufficiently accurate (much measurement error) to make a difference R2 too low unlike, say, physics

410 Relationship between speed of travel and fuel used

411 R2 = 0.938 BUT looks pretty good
know speed, make a good prediction of fuel BUT look at the chart if we know speed we can make a perfect prediction of fuel used R2 should be 1.00

412 Detecting Non-Linearity
Residual plot just like heteroscedasticity Using this example very, very obvious usually pretty obvious

413 Residual plot

414 Linearity: A Case of Additivity
Linearity = additivity along the range of the IV Jeremy rides his bicycle harder Increase in speed depends on current speed Not additive, multiplicative MacCallum and Mar (1995). Distinguishing between moderator and quadratic effects in multiple regression. Psychological Bulletin.

415 The independence assumption (lack of autocorrelation)
Assumption 5: The expected correlation between residuals, for any two cases, is 0. The independence assumption (lack of autocorrelation)

416 Independence Assumption
Also: lack of autocorrelation Tricky one often ignored exists for almost all tests All cases should be independent of one another knowing the value of one case should not tell you anything about the value of other cases

417 How is it Detected? Can be difficult
need some clever statistics (multilevel models) Better off avoiding situations where it arises Or handling it when it does arise Residual Plots

418 Residual Plots Were data collected in time order?
If so plot ID number against the residuals Look for any pattern Test for linear relationship Non-linear relationship Heteroscedasticity

419

420 How does it arise? Two main ways time-series analyses
When cases are time periods weather on Tuesday and weather on Wednesday correlated inflation 1972, inflation 1973 are correlated clusters of cases patients treated by three doctors children from different classes people assessed in groups

421 Why does it matter? Standard errors can be wrong
therefore significance tests can be wrong Parameter estimates can be wrong really, really wrong from positive to negative An example students do an exam (on statistics) choose one of three questions IV: time outcome: grade

422 Result, with line of best fit

423 Result shows that BUT … Look again
people who spent longer in the exam, achieve better grades BUT … we haven’t considered which question people answered we might have violated the independence assumption outcome will be autocorrelated Look again with questions marked

424 Now somewhat different

425 Now, people that spent longer got lower grades
questions differed in difficulty do a hard one, get better grade if you can do it, you can do it quickly

426 Dealing with Non-Independence
For time series data Time series analysis (another course) Multilevel models (hard, some another course) For clustered data Robust standard errors Generalized estimating equations Multilevel models

427 Cluster Robust Standard Errors
Predictor: School size Outcome: Grades Sample: 20 schools 20 children per school What is the N?

428 Robust Standard Errors
Sample is: 400 children – is it 400? Not really Each child adds information First child in a school adds lots of information about that school 100th child in a school adds less information’ How much less depends on how similar the children in the school are 20 schools It’s more than 20

429 Robust SE in Stata Very easy
reg predictor outcome , robust cluster(clusterid) BUT Only to be used where clustering is a nuisance only Only adjusts standard errors, not parameter estimates Only to be used where parameter estimates shouldn’t be affected by clustering

430 Example of Robust SE Effects of incentives for attendance at adult literacy class Some students rewarded for attendance Others not rewarded 152 classes randomly assigned to each condition Scores measured at mid term and final

431 Example of Robust SE Naïve Clustered reg postscore tx midscore
Est: SE: Clustered reg postscore tx midscore, robust cluster(classid) Est: SE

432 Problem with Robust Estimates
Only corrects standard error Does not correct estimate Other predictors must be uncorrelated with predictors of group membership Or estimates wrong Two alternatives: Generalized estimating equations (gee) Multilevel models

433 Independence + Heteroscedasticity
Assumption is that residuals are: Independently and identically distributed i.i.d. Same procedure used for both problems Really, same problem

434 Exercise 7.9, exercise 7.10

435 Assumption 6: All predictor variables are uncorrelated with the error term.

436 Uncorrelated with the Error Term
A curious assumption by definition, the residuals are uncorrelated with the predictors (try it and see, if you like) There are no other predictors that are important That correlate with the error i.e. Have an effect

437 OLS estimates will be (badly) biased in this case
Problem in economics Demand increases supply Supply increases wages Higher wages increase demand OLS estimates will be (badly) biased in this case need a different estimation procedure two-stage least squares simultaneous equation modelling Instrumental variables

438 Another Haiku Supply and demand: without a good instrument, not identified.

439 no perfect multicollinearity
Assumption 7: No predictors are a perfect linear function of other predictors no perfect multicollinearity

440 No Perfect Multicollinearity
IVs must not be linear functions of one another matrix of correlations of IVs is not positive definite cannot be inverted analysis cannot proceed Have seen this with age, age start, time working (can’t have all three in the model) also occurs with subscale and total in model at the same time

441 Large amounts of collinearity
a problem (as we shall see) sometimes not an assumption Exercise 7.11

442 Assumption 8: The mean of the error term is zero.
You will like this one.

443 Mean of the Error Term = 0 Mean of the residuals = 0
That is what the constant is for if the mean of the error term deviates from zero, the constant soaks it up - note, Greek letters because we are talking about population values

444 Can do regression without the constant
Usually a bad idea E.g R2 = 0.995, p < 0.001 Looks good

445

446 Lesson 8: Issues in Regression Analysis
Things that alter the interpretation of the regression equation

447 The Four Issues Causality Sample sizes Collinearity Measurement error

448 Causality

449 What is a Cause? Debate about definition of cause
some statistics (and philosophy) books try to avoid it completely We are not going into depth just going to show why it is hard Two dimensions of cause Ultimate versus proximal cause Determinate versus probabilistic

450 Proximal versus Ultimate Why am I here?
I walked here because This is the location of the class because Eric Tanenbaum asked me because (I don’t know) because I was in my office when he rang because I was a lecturer at Derby University because I saw an advert in the paper because

451 Proximal cause Ultimate cause I exist because My parents met because
My father had a job … Proximal cause the direct and immediate cause of something Ultimate cause the thing that started the process off I fell off my bicycle because of the bump I fell off because I was going too fast

452 Determinate versus Probabilistic Cause Why did I fall off my bicycle?
I was going too fast But every time I ride too fast, I don’t fall off Probabilistic cause Why did my tyre go flat? A nail was stuck in my tyre Every time a nail sticks in my tyre, the tyre goes flat Deterministic cause

453 Can get into trouble by mixing them together
Eating deep fried Mars Bars and doing no exercise are causes of heart disease “My Grandad ate three deep fried Mars Bars every day, and the most exercise he ever got was when he walked to the shop next door to buy one” (Deliberately?) confusing deterministic and probabilistic causes

454 Criteria for Causation
Association (correlation) Direction of Influence (a  b) Isolation (not c  a and c  b)

455 Association Correlation does not mean causation But
we all know But Causation does mean correlation Need to show that two things are related may be correlation may be regression when controlling for third (or more) factor

456 Relationship between price and sales
suppliers may be cunning when people want it more stick the price up So – no relationship between price and sales

457 But which variables do we enter?
Until (or course) we control for demand b1 (Price) = b2 (Demand) = 0.94 But which variables do we enter?

458 Direction of Influence
Relationship between A and B three possible processes A B A causes B A B B causes A A B C C causes A & B

459 How do we establish the direction of influence?
Longitudinally? Barometer Drops Storm Now if we could just get that barometer needle to stay where it is … Where the role of theory comes in (more on this later)

460 Isolation Isolate the outcome from all other influences Cannot do this
as experimenters try to do Cannot do this can statistically isolate the effect using multiple regression

461 Role of Theory Strong theory is crucial to making causal statements
Fisher said: to make causal statements “make your theories elaborate.” don’t rely purely on statistical analysis Need strong theory to guide analyses what critics of non-experimental research don’t understand

462 S.J. Gould – a critic says correlate price of petrol and his age, for the last 10 years find a correlation Ha! (He says) that doesn’t mean there is a causal link Of course not! (We say). No social scientist would do that analysis without first thinking (very hard) about the possible causal relations between the variables of interest Would control for time, prices, etc …

463 Gould says “Most correlations are non-causal” (1982, p243)
Atkinson, et al. (1996) relationship between college grades and number of hours worked negative correlation Need to control for other variables – ability, intelligence Gould says “Most correlations are non-causal” (1982, p243) Of course!!!!

464 120 non-causal correlations
karaoke jokes (about statistics) children wake early bathroom headache sleeping equations (beermat) laugh thirsty fried breakfast no beer curry chips falling over lose keys curtains closed I drink a lot of beer 16 causal relations 120 non-causal correlations

465 Abelson (1995) elaborates on this
‘method of signatures’ A collection of correlations relating to the process the ‘signature’ of the process e.g. tobacco smoking and lung cancer can we account for all of these findings with any other theory?

466 The longer a person has smoked cigarettes, the greater the risk of cancer.
The more cigarettes a person smokes over a given time period, the greater the risk of cancer. People who stop smoking have lower cancer rates than do those who keep smoking. Smoker’s cancers tend to occur in the lungs, and be of a particular type. Smokers have elevated rates of other diseases. People who smoke cigars or pipes, and do not usually inhale, have abnormally high rates of lip cancer. Smokers of filter-tipped cigarettes have lower cancer rates than other cigarette smokers. Non-smokers who live with smokers have elevated cancer rates. (Abelson, 1995: )

467 Failure to use theory to select appropriate variables
In addition, should be no anomalous correlations If smokers had more fallen arches than non-smokers, not consistent with theory Failure to use theory to select appropriate variables specification error e.g. in previous example Predict wealth from price and sales increase price, price increases Increase sales, price increases

468 Sometimes these are indicators of the process, not the process itself
e.g. barometer – stopping the needle won’t help e.g. inflation? Indicator or cause of economic health?

469 No Causation without Experimentation
Blatantly untrue I don’t doubt that the sun shining makes us warm Why the aversion? Pearl (2000) says problem is that there is no mathematical operator (e.g. “=“) No one realised that you needed one Until you build a robot

470 AI and Causality A robot needs to make judgements about causality
Needs to have a mathematical representation of causality Suddenly, a problem! Doesn’t exist Most operators are non-directional Causality is directional

471 “How many subjects does it take to run a regression analysis?”
Sample Sizes “How many subjects does it take to run a regression analysis?”

472 Introduction Social scientists don’t worry enough about the sample size required “Why didn’t you get a significant result?” “I didn’t have a large enough sample” Not a common answer, but very common reason More recently awareness of sample size is increasing use too few – no point doing the research use too many – waste their time

473 Research funding bodies Ethical review panels
both become more interested in sample size calculations We will look at two approaches Rules of thumb (quite quickly) Power Analysis (more slowly)

474 Rules of Thumb Lots of simple rules of thumb exist
10 cases per IV and at least 100 cases Green (1991) more sophisticated To test significance of R2 – N = k To test significance of slopes, N = k Rules of thumb don’t take into account all the information that we have Power analysis does

475 Power Analysis Introducing Power Analysis Hypothesis test
tells us the probability of a result of that magnitude occurring, if the null hypothesis is correct (i.e. there is no effect in the population) Doesn’t tell us the probability of that result, if the null hypothesis is false (i.e., there actually is an effect in the population)

476 According to Cohen (1982) all null hypotheses are false
everything that might have an effect, does have an effect it is just that the effect is often very tiny

477 Type I error is false rejection of H0
Type I Errors Type I error is false rejection of H0 Probability of making a type I error a – the significance value cut-off usually 0.05 (by convention) Always this value Not affected by sample size type of test

478 Type II error is false acceptance of the null hypothesis
Type II errors Type II error is false acceptance of the null hypothesis Much, much trickier We think we have some idea we almost certainly don’t Example I do an experiment (random sampling, all assumptions perfectly satisfied) I find p = 0.05

479 Very hard to work out You repeat the experiment exactly
different random sample from same population What is probability you will find p < 0.05? Answer: 0.5 Another experiment, I find p = 0.01 Probability you find p < 0.05? Answer: 0.79 Very hard to work out not intuitive need to understand non-central sampling distributions (more in a minute)

480 Probability of type II error = beta (b)
same as population regression parameter (to be confusing) Power = 1 – Beta Probability of getting a significant result (given that there is a significant result to be found)

481   H0 false (effect to be found) H0 True (no effect to be found)
State of the World H0 true (we find no effect – p > 0.05) H0 false (we find an effect – p < 0.05) Research Findings Type II error p = b power = 1 - b Type I error p = a

482 Four parameters in power analysis
a – prob. of Type I error b – prob. of Type II error (power = 1 – b) Effect size – size of effect in population N Know any three, can calculate the fourth Look at them one at a time

483 a Probability of Type I error
Usually set to 0.05 Somewhat arbitrary sometimes adjusted because of circumstances rarely because of power analysis May want to adjust it, based on power analysis

484 b – Probability of type II error
Power (probability of finding a result) = 1 – b Standard is 80% Some argue for 90% Implication that Type I error is 4 times more serious than type II error adjust ratio with compromise power analysis

485 Effect size in the population
Most problematic to determine Three ways What effect size would be useful to find? R2 = no use (probably) Base it on previous research what have other people found? Use Cohen’s conventions small R2 = 0.02 medium R2 = 0.13 large R2 = 0.26

486 Effect size usually measured as f2
For R2

487 For (standardised) slopes
Where sr2 is the contribution to the variance accounted for by the variable of interest i.e. sr2 = R2 (with variable) – R2 (without) change in R2 in hierarchical regression

488 N – the sample size usually use other three parameters to determine this sometimes adjust other parameters (a) based on this e.g. You can have 50 participants. No more.

489 With power analysis program
Doing power analysis With power analysis program SamplePower, Gpower (free), Nquery With Stata command sampsi Which I find very confusing But we’ll use it anyway

490 sampsi Limited in usefulness
A categorical, two group predictor sampsi 0 0.5, pre(1) r01(0.5) n1(50) sd(1) Find power for detecting an effect of 0.5 When there’s one other variable at baseline Which correlates 0.5 50 people in each group When sd is 1.0

491 sampsi … Method: ANCOVA relative efficiency = 1.143
adjustment to sd = adjusted sd1 = Estimated power: power =

492 GPower Better for regression designs

493

494

495 Underpowered Studies Research in the social sciences is often underpowered Why? See Paper B11 – “the persistence of underpowered studies”

496 Extra Reading Power traditionally focuses on p values What about CIs?
Paper B8 – “Obtaining regression coefficients that are accurate, not simply significant”

497 Exercise 8.1

498 Collinearity

499 Collinearity as Issue and Assumption
Collinearity (multicollinearity) the extent to which the predictors are (multiply) correlated If R2 for any IV, using other IVs = 1.00 perfect collinearity variable is linear sum of other variables regression will not proceed (SPSS will arbitrarily throw out a variable)

500 Four things to look at in collinearity
R2 < 1.00, but high other problems may arise Four things to look at in collinearity meaning implications detection actions

501 Meaning of Collinearity
Literally ‘co-linearity’ lying along the same line Perfect collinearity when some IVs predict another Total = S1 + S2 + S3 + S4 S1 = Total – (S2 + S3 + S4) rare

502 Less than perfect when some IVs are close to predicting other IVs
correlations between IVs are high (usually, but not always)  high multiple correlations

503 Implications Effects the stability of the parameter estimates Because
and so the standard errors of the parameter estimates and so the significance and CIs Because shared variance, which the regression procedure doesn’t know where to put

504 Sex differences due to genetics? due to upbringing?
(almost) perfect collinearity statistically impossible to tell

505 When collinearity is less than perfect
increases variability of estimates between samples estimates are unstable reflected in the variances, and hence standard errors

506 Detecting Collinearity
Look at the parameter estimates large standardised parameter estimates (>0.3?), which are not significant be suspicious Run a series of regressions each IV as outcome all other IVs as IVs for each IV

507 Ask for collinearity diagnostics
Sounds like hard work? SPSS does it for us! Ask for collinearity diagnostics Tolerance – calculated for every IV Variance Inflation Factor sq. root of amount s.e. has been increased

508 Actions What you can do about collinearity Get new data
“no quick fix” (Fox, 1991) Get new data avoids the problem address the question in a different way e.g. find people who have been raised as the ‘wrong’ gender exist, but rare Not a very useful suggestion

509 Remove / Combine variables
Collect more data not different data, more data collinearity increases standard error (se) se decreases as N increases get a bigger N Remove / Combine variables If an IV correlates highly with other IVs Not telling us much new If you have two (or more) IVs which are very similar e.g. 2 measures of depression, socio-economic status, achievement, etc

510 Use stepwise regression (or some flavour of)
sum them, average them, remove one Many measures use principal components analysis to reduce them Use stepwise regression (or some flavour of) See previous comments Can be useful in theoretical vacuum Ridge regression not very useful behaves weirdly

511 Exercise 8.2, 8.3, 8.4

512 Measurement Error

513 What is Measurement Error
In social science, it is unlikely that we measure any variable perfectly measurement error represents this imperfection We assume that we have a true score T A measure of that score x

514 just like a regression equation
standardise the parameters T is the reliability the amount of variance in x which comes from T but, like a regression equation assume that e is random and has mean of zero more on that later

515 Simple Effects of Measurement Error
Lowers the measured correlation between two variables Real correlation true scores (x* and y*) Measured correlation measured scores (x and y)

516 Measured correlation of x and y rxy True correlation of x and y rx*y*
Reliability of x rxx Reliability of y ryy e e x y Measured correlation of x and y rxy

517 Attenuation of correlation
Attenuation corrected correlation

518 Example

519 Complex Effects of Measurement Error
Really horribly complex Measurement error reduces correlations reduces estimate of b reducing one estimate increases others because of effects of control combined with effects of suppressor variables exercise to examine this

520 Dealing with Measurement Error
Attenuation correction very dangerous not recommended Avoid in the first place use reliable measures don’t discard information don’t categorise Age: 10-20, 21-30, …

521 Complications Assume measurement error is Additive Linear additive
e.g. weight – people may under-report / over-report at the extremes Linear particularly the case when using proxy variables

522 e.g. proxy measures Want to know effort on childcare, count number of children 1st child is more effort than 19th child Want to know financial status, count income 1st £1 much greater effect on financial status than the 1,000,000th.

523 Exercise 8.5

524 Lesson 9: Non-Linear Analysis in Regression

525 Introduction Non-linear effect occurs Assumption is violated
when the effect of one predictor is not consistent across the range of the IV Assumption is violated expected value of residuals = 0 no longer the case

526 Some Examples

527 A Learning Curve Skill Experience

528 Yerkes-Dodson Law of Arousal
Performance Arousal

529 Enthusiasm Levels over a
Lesson on Regression Enthusiastic Suicidal 3.5 Time

530 Learning Yerkes-Dodson Enthusiasm line changed direction once
line changed direction twice

531 Everything is Non-Linear
Every relationship we look at is non-linear, for two reasons Exam results cannot keep increasing with reading more books Linear in the range we examine For small departures from linearity Cannot detect the difference Non-parsimonious solution

532 Non-Linear Transformations

533 Bending the Line Non-linear regression is hard Transformations
We cheat, and linearise the data Do linear regression Transformations We need to transform the data rather than estimating a curved line which would be very difficult may not work with OLS we can take a straight line, and bend it or take a curved line, and straighten it back to linear (OLS) regression

534 We still do linear regression
Linear in the parameters Y = b1x + b2x2 + … Can do non-linear regression Non-linear in the parameters Much trickier Statistical theory either breaks down OR becomes harder

535 Linear transformations
multiply by a constant add a constant change the slope and the intercept

536 y=2x y=x + 3 y y=x x

537 Linear transformations are no use
alter the slope and intercept don’t alter the standardised parameter estimate Non-linear transformation will bend the slope quadratic transformation y = x2 one change of direction

538 Cubic transformation y = x2 + x3 two changes of direction

539 To estimate a non-linear regression
we don’t actually estimate anything non-linear we transform the x-variable to a non-linear version can estimate that straight line represents the curve we don’t bend the line, we stretch the space around the line, and make it flat

540 Detecting Non-linearity

541 Draw a Scatterplot Draw a scatterplot of y plotted against x
see if it looks a bit non-linear e.g. Education and beginning salary from bank data with line of best fit

542 A Real Example Starting salary and years of education
From employee data.sav

543 Expected value of error (residual) is > 0

544 Use Residual Plot Scatterplot is only good for one variable
use the residual plot (that we used for heteroscedasticity) Good for many variables

545 We want points to lie in a nice straight sausage

546 We don’t want a nasty bent sausage

547 Educational level and starting salary

548 Carrying Out Non-Linear Regression

549 Linear Transformation
Linear transformation doesn’t change interpretation of slope standardised slope se, t, or p of slope R2 Can change effect of a transformation

550 With others does have an effect
Actually more complex with some transformations can add a constant with no effect (e.g. quadratic) With others does have an effect inverse, log Sometimes it is necessary to add a constant negative numbers have no square root 0 has no log

551 Education and Salary Linear Regression
Saw previously that the assumption of expected errors = 0 was violated Anyway … R2 = 0.401, p < 0.001 salbegin =  educ Standardised b1 (educ) = 0.633 Both parameters make sense

552 Add this variable to the equation
Non-linear Effect Compute new variable quadratic educ2 = educ2 Add this variable to the equation R2 = 0.585, p < 0.001 salbegin =  educ  educ2 slightly curious Standardised b1 (educ) = -2.4 b2 (educ2) = 3.1 What is going on?

553 Need hierarchical regression
Collinearity is what is going on Correlation of educ and educ2 r = 0.990 Regression equation becomes difficult (impossible?) to interpret Need hierarchical regression what is the change in R2 is that change significant? R2 (change) = 0.184, p < 0.001

554 While we are at it, let’s look at the cubic effect
R2 (change) = 0.004, p = 0.045  e  e  e3 Standardised: b1(e) = 0.04 b2(e2) = -2.04 b3(e3) = 2.71

555 Keep going while we are ahead?
Fourth Power Keep going while we are ahead? When do we stop?

556 Tricky, given that parameter estimates are a bit nonsensical
Interpretation Tricky, given that parameter estimates are a bit nonsensical Two methods 1: Use R2 change Save predicted values or calculate predicted values to plot line of best fit Save them from equation Plot against IV

557

558 Differentiate with respect to e We said:
s =  e  e  e3 but first we will simplify it to quadratic s =  e  e2 dy/dx = x 2 x e

559 1 year of education at the higher end of the scale, better than 1 year at the lower end of the scale. MBA versus GCSE

560 Differentiate Cubic  e  e  e3 dy/dx = 103 – 206  2  e + 12  3  e2 Can calculate slopes for quadratic and cubic at different values

561

562 A Quick Note on Differentiation
For y = xp dx/dy = pxp-1 For equations such as y =b1x + b2xP dy/dx = b1 + b2pxp-1 y = 3x + 4x2 dy/dx = • 2x

563 Many functions are simple to differentiate
y = b1x + b2x2 + b3x3 dy/dx = b1 + b2 • 2x + b3 • 3 • x2 y = 4x + 5x2 + 6x3 dx/dy = • 2 • x + 6 • 3 • x2 Many functions are simple to differentiate Not all though

564 Splines and Knots Estimate a different slope following an event
Lines are splines Events are knots Event might be known Marriage Might be unknown How many years after brain injury does recovery start

565 Lesson 10: Regression for Counts and Categories
Dichotomous/Nominal outcomes

566 Contents Dichotomous – logistic / probit
General and Generalized Linear Models Dichotomous – logistic / probit Counts – Poisson and negative binomial

567 GLMs and GLMs General linear models Generalized linear models
Ordinary least squares regression based models Identity link function Regression, ANOVA, correlation, etc Generalized linear models More links More error structures General linear models are a subset of generalized linear models

568 Dichotomous Often in social sciences, we have a dichotomous/nominal outcome we will look at dichotomous first, then a quick look at multinomial Dichotomous outcome e.g. guilty/not guilty pass/fail won/lost Alive/dead (used in medicine)

569 Why Won’t OLS Do?

570 Example: PTSD in Veterans
How does length of deployment affect probability of PTSD? Have PTSD, or don’t. We might be interested in severity Army are not If you have PTSD, you need help Not going back Develop a selection procedure Two predictor variables Rank – 1 =Staff Sgt, 5 = Private, Deployment length (months)

571 1st ten cases

572 Just consider score first
outcome PTSD (1 = Yes, 0 = No) Just consider score first Carry out regression Rank as predictor, PTSD as outcome R2 = 0.097, F = 4.1, df = 1, 48, p = b0 = 0.190 b1 = 0.110, p=0.028 Seems OK

573 Residual plot

574 Problems 1 and 2 strange distributions of residuals
parameter estimates may be wrong standard errors will certainly be wrong

575 2nd problem – interpretation
I have rank 2 Pass =  2 = 0.41 I have rank 8 Pass =  8 = 1.07 Seems OK, but What does it mean? Cannot score 0.41 or 1.07 can only score 0 or 1 Cannot be interpreted need a different approach

576 A Different Approach Logistic Regression

577 Logit Transformation In lesson 9, transformed IVs
now transform the outcome Need a transformation which gives us graduated scores (between 0 and 1) No upper limit we can’t predict someone will pass twice No lower limit you can’t do worse than fail

578 Step 1: Convert to Probability
First, stop talking about values talk about probability for each value of score, calculate probability of pass Solves the problem of graduated scales

579 probability of PTSD given a rank of 1 is 0.7

580 Now a score of 0.41 has a meaning But a score of 1.07 has no meaning
This is better Now a score of 0.41 has a meaning a 0.41 probability of pass But a score of 1.07 has no meaning cannot have a probability > 1 (or < 0) Need another transformation

581 Step 2: Convert to Odds-Ratio
Need to remove upper limit Convert to odds Odds, as used by betting shops 5:1, 1:2 Slightly different from odds in speech a 1 in 2 chance odds are 1:1 (evens) 50%

582 Odds ratio = (number of times it happened) / (number of times it didn’t happen)

583 0.8 = 0.8/0.2 = 4 0.2 = 0.2/0.8 = 0.25 equivalent to 4:1 (odds on)
0.8 = 0.8/0.2 = 4 equivalent to 4:1 (odds on) 4 times out of five 0.2 = 0.2/0.8 = 0.25 equivalent to 1:4 (4:1 against) 1 time out of five

584 Now we have solved the upper bound problem
we can interpret 1.07, 2.07, But we still have the zero problem we cannot interpret predicted scores less than zero

585 Step 3: The Log Log10 of a number(x) log(10) = 1 log(100) = 2

586 log(1) = 0 log(0.1) = -1 log( ) = -5

587 Natural Logs and e Don’t use log10 Natural log, ln
Use loge Natural log, ln Has some desirable properties, that log10 doesn’t For us If y = ln(x) + c dy/dx = 1/x Not true for any other logarithm

588 Be careful – calculators and stats packages are not consistent when they use log
Sometimes log10, sometimes loge

589 Take the natural log of the odds ratio Goes from -  +
can interpret any predicted value

590 Putting them all together
Logit transformation log-odds ratio not bounded at zero or one

591

592 Probability gets closer to zero, but never reaches it as logit goes down.

593 Hooray! Problem solved, lesson over
errrmmm… almost Because we are now using log-odds ratio, we can’t use OLS we need a new technique, called Maximum Likelihood (ML) to estimate the parameters

594 Parameter Estimation using ML
ML tries to find estimates of model parameters that are most likely to give rise to the pattern of observations in the sample data All gets a bit complicated OLS is a special case of ML the mean is an ML estimator

595 Don’t have closed form equations
must be solved iteratively estimates parameters that are most likely to give rise to the patterns observed in the data by maximising the likelihood function (LF) We aren’t going to worry about this except to note that sometimes, the estimates do not converge ML cannot find a solution

596 R2 in Logistic Regression
A dichotomous variable doesn’t have variance If you know the mean (proportion) you know the variance You can’t have R2. There are several pseudo-R2 None are perfect There’s something better

597 Logistic Regression in Stata
Exercise 10.1 Two (almost) equivalent commands logistic ptsd rank deployment logit ptsd rank deployment

598 Logit Gives output in log-odds logit ptsd rank deployment
pass | Odds Ratio Std. Err z P>|z| [95% Conf. Interval] deployment | rank |

599 Logistic Gives output in odds ratios No intercept
logit ptsd rank deployment pass | Odds Ratio Std. Err z P>|z| [95% Conf. Interval] deployment | rank |

600 SPSS produces a classification table
And Stata produces it if you ask predictions of model based on cut-off of 0.5 (by default) predicted values x actual values DO NOT USE IT! Will this person go to prison? No. You will be right 99.9% of the time Doesn’t mean you have a good model (Gottman and Murray – Blink)

601

602 Model parameters B SE (B)
Change in the logged odds associated with a change of 1 unit in IV just like OLS regression difficult to interpret SE (B) Standard error Multiply by 1.96 to get 95% CIs

603 Constant i.e. score = 0 B = 1.314 Exp(B) = eB = e1.314 = 3.720
OR = 3.720, p = 1 – (1 / (OR + 1)) = 1 – (1 / ( )) p = 0.788

604 Score 1 Constant b = 1.314 Score B = -0.467
Exp(1.314 – 0.467) = Exp(0.847) = 2.332 OR = 2.332 p = 1 – (1 / ( )) = 0.699

605 Standard Errors and CIs
Symmetrical in B Non-symmetrical (sometimes very) in exp(B)

606 The odds of failing the test are multiplied by 0. 63 (CIs = 0. 408, 0
The odds of failing the test are multiplied by 0.63 (CIs = 0.408, p = 0.033), for every additional point on the aptitude test.

607 Hierarchical Logistic Regression
In OLS regression Use R2 change In logistic regression Use chi-square change Difference in chi-square = chi-square Difference in df = df

608 Hierarchical Logistic Regression
Model 1: Experience Model 2: Experience + Score Model 1: Chi-square =4.83, df = 1 Model 2: Chi-square =5.77, df = 2

609 Difference: gen p = 1 - chi2(1, 1.94) tab p p = 0.332
Chi-square = = 1.94, Df = 2 – 1 = 1 gen p = 1 - chi2(1, 1.94) tab p p = 0.332 P-value from SE = 0.339 Why?

610 More on Standard Errors
Because of Wald standard errors Wald SEs are overestimated Make p-value in estimates is wrong – too high (CIs still correct)

611 Two estimates use slightly different information
P-value says “what if no effect” CI says “what if there is this effect” Variance depends on the hypothesised ratio of the number of people in the two groups Can calculate likelihood ratio based p-values If you can be bothered Some packages provide them automatically

612 Probit Regression Very similar to logistic
much more complex initial transformation (to normal distribution) Very similar results to logistic (multiplied by 1.7) Swap logistic for probit in Stata command Harder to interpret Parameter doesn’t mean something – like log odds

613 Differentiating Between Probit and Logistic
Depends on shape of the error term Normal or logistic Graphs are very similar to each other Could distinguish quality of fit Given enormous sample size Logistic = probit x 1.7 Actually Probit advantage Understand the distribution Logistic advantage Much simpler to get back to the probability

614

615 Infinite Parameters Non-convergence can happen because of infinite parameters Insoluble model Three kinds: Complete separation The groups are completely distinct Pass group all score more than 10 Fail group all score less than 10

616 Quasi-complete separation
Separation with some overlap Pass group all score 10 or more Fail group all score 10 or less Both cases: No convergence Close to this Curious estimates Curious standard errors

617 Categorical Predictors
Can cause separation Especially if correlated Need people in every cell Male Female White Non-White Below Poverty Line Above Poverty Line

618 Logistic Regression and Diagnosis
Logistic regression can be used for diagnostic tests For every score Calculate probability that result is positive Calculate proportion of people with that score (or lower) who have a positive result Calculate c statistic Measure of discriminative power % of all possible cases, where the model gives a higher probability to a correct case than to an incorrect case

619 Perfect c-statistic = 1.0 Random c-statistic = 0.5

620 Sensitivity and Specificity
Probability of saying someone has a positive result – If they do: p(pos)|pos Specificity Probability of saying someone has a negative result If they do: p(neg)|neg

621 C-Statistic, Sensitivity and Specificity
After logistic lroc Gives c-statistic Better than R-squared

622

623 More Advanced Techniques
Multinomial Logistic Regression more than two categories in outcome same procedure one category chosen as reference group odds of being in category other than reference Ordinal multinomial logistic regression For ordinal outcome variables

624 More on Odds Ratios Odds ratios are horrid
We use them because they have nice distributional properties Example: 40% in group 1 get PTSD 60% in group 2 get PTSD What’s the odds ratio? How is this confusing?

625 Alternatives to Odds Ratios
Risk difference 20 percentage points higher Relative risk Probability is 1.5 times higher This is what you would think an odds ratio meant Can we use these in regression? RD – maybe. Sometimes. RR – yes. But we need to do something else first

626 Final Thoughts Logistic Regression can be extended Same issues as OLS
dummy variables non-linear effects interactions Same issues as OLS collinearity outliers

627 Same additional options as regress
xi: cluster robust

628 Poisson Regression

629 Counts and the Poisson Distribution
Von Bortkiewicz (1898) Numbers of Prussian soldiers kicked to death by horses 0 109 1 65 2 22 3 3 1

630 The data fitted a Poisson probability distribution
When counts of events occur, poisson distribution is common E.g. papers published by researchers, police arrests, number of murders, ship accidents Common approach Log transform and treat as normal Problems Censored at 0 Integers only allowed Heteroscedasticity

631 The Poisson Distribution

632

633 Excel has a Poisson function you can use.
Where: y is the count m is the mean of the Poisson distribution In a Poisson distribution The mean = the variance (hence heteroscedasticity issue)) m = s2

634 Poisson Probabilities
Mean 1 2 3 10 Score 0.37 0.14 0.05 0.00 0.27 0.15 0.18 0.22 0.06 0.01 4 0.02 0.09 0.17 5 0.04 0.10 6 7 8 0.11 9 0.13

635 Issues with Estimation
Just as with logistic We can’t predict a mean below zero Don’t predict the mean Predict the log of the mean

636 Poisson Regression in Stata
Adult literacy study Number of sessions attended Count variable Poisson regression

637 poisson sessions tx, irr
sessions | Coef. Std. Err z P>|z| [95% Conf. Interval] tx | _cons | poisson sessions tx, irr sessions | IRR Std. Err z P>|z| [95% Conf. Interval] tx |

638 But was it Poisson? Look at predicted probabilities Predicted means
Compare with actual probabilities Predicted means Control: exp(1.899) = 6.86 Intervention: exp( ) = 5.28 Get means and SDs

639 bysort tx: sum sessions
VObs Mean Std. Dev. sessions | -> tx = 1ariable | Variable | Obs Mean Std. Dev. sessions |

640 Compare predicted probabilities with actual probabilities
Do OK on the means Don’t do OK on the variances Variances are too high Compare predicted probabilities with actual probabilities tab session tx,col nofreq Draw graphs Not horrible Except the zeroes

641

642 Test for Goodness of Fit to Poisson Distribution
After running Poisson estat gof Goodness-of-fit chi2 = Prob > chi2(150) = Highly significant Poisson distribution doesn’t fit

643 Overdispersion Problem in Poisson regression Causes Two solutions
Too many zeroes Causes c2 inflation Standard error deflation Hence p-values too low Higher type I error rate Two solutions Negative binomial regression Robust standard errors

644 Robust Standard Errors
poisson sessions tx, robust | Robust sessions | Coef. Std. Err z P>|z| tx | _cons | Robust SEs are larger

645 Negative Binomial Regression
Adds a ‘hurdle’ to account for the zeroes Called alpha nbreg sessions tx OR nbreg sessions tx, robust

646 Back to Categorical Outcomes
We said: Odds ratios are not good We like relative risk instead What is the ratio of the risks? What analysis technique do we know that gives ratios of means

647 Poisson regression! Wait. It won’t work. The distribution is wrong. Robust estimates!

648 Poisson Regression in SPSS
SPSS 15 (and above), has added it Under generalized linear models

649 Lesson 11: Mediation and Path Analysis

650 Introduction Moderator Mediator All relationships are really mediated
Level of one variable influences effect of another variable Mediator One variable influences another via a third variable All relationships are really mediated are we interested in the mediators? can we make the process more explicit

651 In examples with bank Why? beginning education salary
What is the process? Are we making assumptions about the process? Should we test those assumptions?

652 job skills expectations negotiating skills kudos for bank education beginning salary

653 Direct and Indirect Influences
X may affect Y in two ways Directly – X has a direct (causal) influence on Y (or maybe mediated by other variables) Indirectly – X affects Y via a mediating variable - M

654 e.g. how does going to the pub effect comprehension on a Summer school course
on, say, regression not reading books on regression Having fun in pub in evening less knowledge Anything here?

655 not reading books on regression
Having fun in pub in evening less knowledge fatigue Still needed?

656 Mediators needed to cope with more sophisticated theory in social sciences make explicit assumptions made about processes examine direct and indirect influences

657 Detecting Mediation

658 “Classic Approach” 4 Steps
From Baron and Kenny (1986) To establish that the effect of X on Y is mediated by M Show that X predicts Y Show that X predicts M Show that M predicts Y, controlling for X If effect of X controlling for M is zero, M is complete mediator of the relationship (3 and 4 in same analysis)

659 Enjoy Books  Buy books Read Books
Example: Book habits Enjoy Books Buy books Read Books

660 Three Variables Enjoy Buy Read How much an individual enjoys books
How many books an individual buys (in a year) Read How many books an individual reads (in a year)

661

662 The Theory enjoy buy read

663 Show that X (enjoy) predicts Y (read) b1 = 0.487, p < 0.001
Step 1 Show that X (enjoy) predicts Y (read) b1 = 0.487, p < 0.001 standardised b1 = 0.732 OK

664 Show that X (enjoy) predicts M (buy) b1 = 0.974, p < 0.001
standardised b1 = 0.643 OK

665 3. Show that M (buy) predicts Y (read), controlling for X (enjoy)
b1 = 0.469, p < 0.001 standardised b1 = 0.206 OK

666 If effect of X controlling for M is zero, M is complete mediator of the relationship
(Same as analysis for step 3.) b2 = 0.287, p = 0.001 standardised b2 = 0.431 Hmmmm… Significant, therefore not a complete mediator

667 0.287 (step 4) enjoy read buy 0.206 (from step 3) 0.974 (from step 2)

668 The Mediation Coefficient
Amount of mediation = Step 1 – Step 4 =0.487 – 0.287 = 0.200 OR Step 2 x Step 3 =0.974 x 0.206

669 SE of Mediator enjoy read buy sa = se(a) sb = se(b) a b (from step 2)

670 Sobel test Standard error of mediation coefficient can be calculated a = sa = 0.189 b = sb = 0.054

671 Indirect effect = 0.200 Online Sobel test: se = 0.056
t =3.52, p = 0.001 Online Sobel test:

672 Problems with the Sobel test
Recently Move in methodological literature away from this conventional approach Problems of power: Several tests, all of which must be significant Type I error rate = 0.05 * 0.05 = Must affect power

673 Distributional Assumption
We assume that the sampling distribution of the coefficient is normally distributed Standard error is standard deviation If: a (x  m) is normal and not zero b (m  y) is normal and not zero Then: a × b Is not normally distributed Assumption is violated Test is incorrect

674 Solution: Bootstrap Computer intensive semi-parametric procedure Removes distributional assumption Bootstrapping suggested as alternative For Stata: For SAS, SPSS:

675 Cross Sectional Bias If everything is measured at one time Ideally:
Likely to be bias Ideally: Three variables, measured on three occasions

676 x x x m m m y y y

677 BUT: Stationarity assumption can save us
Kind of hard work Collecting data on three occasions BUT: Stationarity assumption can save us x x x m m m y y y

678 We assume that the effect from M to Y is stable over time
Only need two time points Cole and Maxwell (2003)

679 Power in Mediation Really hard to work out Need to run simulations
Power depends on Size of a Size of b Fritz and Mackinnon (2007) Table of power for different effects

680 More Information on Mediation
Mackinnon, Fritz and Fairchild Annual Review of Psychology Mackinnon Introduction to statistical mediation Iacobucci Mediation analysis (little green book) Mackinnon’s website (Google: mackinnon mediation) Facebook group (No, really)

681 Lesson 12: Moderators in Regression
“different slopes for different folks”

682 Introduction Moderator relationships have many different names
interactions (from ANOVA) multiplicative non-linear (just confusing) non-additive All talking about the same thing

683 A moderated relationship occurs
when the effect of one variable depends upon the level of another variable

684 Where there is collinearity
Hang on … That seems very like a nonlinear relationship Moderator Effect of one variable depends on level of another Non-linear Effect of one variable depends on level of itself Where there is collinearity Can be hard to distinguish between them Paper B5 Should (usually) compare effect sizes

685 e.g. How much it hurts when I drop a computer on my foot depends on
x1: how much alcohol I have drunk x2: how high the computer was dropped from but if x1 is high enough x2 will have no effect

686 e.g. Likelihood of injury in a car accident
depends on x1: speed of car x2: if I was wearing a seatbelt but if x1 is low enough x2 will have no effect

687

688 e.g. number of words (from a list) I can remember
depends on x1: type of words (abstract, e.g. ‘justice’, or concrete, e.g. ‘carrot’) x2: Method of testing (recognition – i.e. multiple choice, or free recall) but if using recognition x1: will not make a difference

689 We looked at three kinds of moderator alcohol x height = pain
continuous x continuous speed x seatbelt = injury continuous x categorical word type x test type categorical x categorical We will look at them in reverse order

690 How do we know to look for moderators?
Theoretical rationale Often the most powerful Many theories predict additive/linear effects Fewer predict moderator effects Presence of heteroscedasticity Clue there may be a moderated relationship missing

691 Two Categorical Predictors

692 Data 2 IVs 20 Participants in one of four groups 5 per group
word type (concrete [e.g. Carrot, table], abstract [e.g. Love, justice) test method (multiple choice, recall ) 20 Participants in one of four groups Concrete, MC Concrete, recall Abstract, MC Abstract, recall 5 per group lesson12.1-words.dta

693

694 Graph of means

695 Procedure for Testing 1: Convert to dummy coding
Already done 2: Calculate interaction term Multiply dummy codes together (Can also use xi: for this) Call interaction mxc

696 Interaction term (wxt)
multiply effect coded variables together

697 3: Carry out regression Hierarchical linear effects first
interaction effect in next block

698 b0(intercept)= 7.0 b1 (mc) = 8.6 b2 (concrete) = 8.2
Mean score when MC = 0 and concrete = 0 b1 (mc) = 8.6 When concrete is zero, effect of MC b2 (concrete) = 8.2 When mc is zero, effect of concrete

699 Given other estimates, what’s the predicted mean of concrete, MC?
b3 (mc x con) = -8.4 grand mean Given other estimates, what’s the predicted mean of concrete, MC? = 23.8 What is it? 15.4

700 Have: Expect: Difference:

701 Back to the Graph Slope for concrete words 15.2-15.4=-0.2
Difference in slopes -8.6-(-0.2) = -8.4 Slope for abstract words =-8.6

702 b associated with interaction
The difference in the slopes OR The change in slope, away from the average, associated with a 1 unit change in the moderating variable

703 Y(conc) = Y = 7 + 8.6  m + 8.2  1 + -8.4  m  1
Another way to look at it Y =  m  c  m  c Examine concrete words group (c = 1) substitute values into the equation Y(conc) = Y =  m   m  1 Y(conc) = Y =  m  m Y(conc) = Y =  m  m Y(conc) = Y =  m

704 Categorical x Continuous

705 Note on Dichotomisation
Very common to see people dichotomise a variable Makes the analysis easier Very bad idea Paper B6

706 Data A chain of 60 supermarkets
examining the relationship between profitability, shop size, and local competition 2 IVs shop size comp (local competition, 0=no, 1=yes) outcome profit

707 Data, ‘lesson 12.2.dta’

708 1st Analysis Two IVs R2=0.367, df=2, 57, p < 0.001
Unstandardised estimates b1 (shopsize) = (p=0.001) b2 (comp) = (p<0.001) Standardised estimates b1 (shopsize) = 0.356 b2 (comp) = 0.448

709 Suspicions Presence of competition is likely to have an effect
Residual plot shows a little heteroscedasticity

710

711 Procedure for Testing Very similar to last time
convert ‘comp’ to dummy coding (if it’s not already) Compute interaction term comp (effect coded) x size Hierarchical regression

712 Result Estimates b1 (shopsize) = 0.12, SE = 0.03
b2 (comp) = -1.67, SE 2.50 b3 (sxc) = -0.10, SE 0.05

713 comp now non-significant
shows importance of hierarchical it obviously is important

714 Interpretation Draw graph with lines of best fit
graph twoway (scatter profit shopsize if comp==1) (lfit profit shopsize if comp==1) (scatter profit shopsize if comp==0) (lfit profit shopsize if comp==0), legend(off)

715

716 Substitute into equation Effects of size
(can ignore the constant) Y=size comp(-1.67) + sizecomp(-0.09) Competition present (comp = 1) Y=size (-1.67) + size1(-0.09) Y=size size (-0.09) Y=size0.03

717 Competition present (x2 = 0)
Y=size comp(-1.67) + sizecomp(-0.09) Competition present (x2 = 0) Y=size (-1.67) + size 0 (-0.09) Y=size0.12

718 Two Continuous Variables

719 Data Bank Employees only using clerical staff 363 cases
predicting starting salary previous experience age age x experience (exercise 6.3)

720 Correlation matrix only one significant

721 Initial Estimates (no moderator) (standardised)
R2 = 0.063, p<0.001 Age at start = -0.37, p<0.001 Previous experience = 0.36, p<0.001 Suppressing each other Age and experience compensate for one another Older, with no experience, bad Younger, with experience, good

722 The Procedure Very similar to previous
create multiplicative interaction term BUT Center variables (subtract mean) Not always necessary Can make life easier

723 Hierarchical regression
two linear effects first moderator effect in second

724 Estimates (standardised)
Change in R2 0.085, p<0.001 Estimates (standardised) b1 (agestart) = -0.52 b2 (prevexp) = 0.93 b3 (age x exp) = -0.56

725 Interpretation 1: Pick-a-Point
Graph is tricky can’t have two continuous variables Choose specific points (pick-a-point) Graph the line of best fit of one variable at others Two ways to pick a point 1: Choose high (z = +1), medium (z = 0) and low (z = -1) Choose ‘sensible’ values – age 20, 50, 80?

726 We know: We can rewrite this as:
Y = e  a  a  e  -0.58 Where a = agestart, and e = experience We can rewrite this as: Y = (e  0.94) + (a  -0.53) + (a  e  -0.58) Take a out of the brackets Y = (e  0.94) + ( e  -0.58)a Bracketed terms are simple intercept and simple slope – intercept and slope for agestart 0= (e  0.10) 1= ( e  -0.58)a Y = 0 + 1a

727 Pick any value of e, and we know the slope for a
Standardised, so it’s easy e = -1 0= (-1  0.94) = 1= (  -0.58)a = -0.05a e = 0 0= (0  0.10) = 0 1= (  -0.58)a = -0.53a e = 1 0= (1  0.10) = 0.10 1= (  -0.58)a = -1.11a

728 Graph the Three Lines

729 Do This in Stata The easy way Create some pseudo cases
Some fake people With sensible scores for the variables Regression equation ‘stays behind’ Calculate predicted scores with predict (Can be in a new dataset)

730

731 Then draw graph drop if _n < 364 graph twoway (lfit pred agestart if prevexp==0, lcolor(red)) (lfit pred agestart if prevexp==85, lcolor(black)) (lfit pred agestart if prevexp==170, lcolor(green)) , legend(off)

732

733 (Also works in SPSS; in SAS, use Proc Score)

734 Interpretation 2: P-Values and CIs
Second way Newer, rarely done Calculate CIs of the slope At any point Calculate p-value Give ranges of significance

735 What do you need? The variance and covariance of the estimates
SPSS doesn’t provide estimates for intercept Need to do it manually In options, exclude intercept Create intercept – c = 1 Use it in the regression

736 Enter information into web page:
Get results Calculations in Bauer and Curran (in press: Multivariate Behavioral Research) Paper B13

737

738 Areas of Significance

739 2 complications 1: Constant differed
2: outcome was logged, hence non-linear effect of 1 unit depends on where the unit is See paper A2

740 Finally …

741 Unlimited Moderators Moderator effects are not limited to 2 variables
linear effects

742 Three Interacting Variables
Age, Sex, Exp Block 1 Block 2 Age x Sex, Age x Exp, Sex x Exp Block 3 Age x Sex x Exp

743 Results All two way interactions significant Three way not significant
Effect of Age depends on sex Effect of experience depends on sex Size of the age x experience interaction does not depend on sex (phew!)

744 Moderated Non-Linear Relationships
Enter non-linear effect Enter non-linear effect x moderator if significant indicates degree of non-linearity differs by moderator

745

746 Lesson 13: Longitudinal Models

747 Advantages of Longitudinal Data
You get more data from the same number of people You can test causal relationships Although you can’t rule them out You can examine change You can control for individual differences

748 Disadvantage of Longitudinal Data
It’s much harder to analyze

749 Longitudinal Research
For comparing repeated measures Clusters are people Data are usually short and fat ID V1 V2 V3 V4 1 2 3 4 7 6 8 5

750 Converting Data ID V X Change data to tall and thin
1 2 3 4 7 6 8 5 Change data to tall and thin Use reshape in stata Use Data, Restructure in SPSS Clusters are ID

751 Predict Salary Change Use exercise5.3-bank salary.dta
Compare beginning salary and salary Would normally use paired samples t-test Difference = $17,403, 95% CIs $16, , $18,

752 Predict Salary Change Don’t take the difference in salary and salbegin
Why not? reg salary agestart salbegin Est: , 95% CIs ,

753 Restructure the Data gen id = _n rename salbegin sal1
rename salary sal2 reshape long sal, i(id) j(t) replace t = t-1

754 Restructure the Data Do it again Do a regression
With data tall and thin Do a regression What do we find? ID Time Cash 1 $18,750 $21,450 2 $12,000 $21,900 3 $13,200 $45,000

755 Results We have violated the independence assumption
We have the wrong answer Simplest way to solve it: regress sal t, cluster(id) Assumes that ID is just an irritant Rather inflexible

756 However … That has one advantage
Missing data doesn’t mean that we exclude the case If data are missing at random (or missing completely at random) estimates will be unbiased

757 If everyone has If half the people have But what if some have
Score at time 1 Score at time 2 Analysis is easy If half the people have But what if some have Time 1 Time 2 Time 1 and 2

758 If No problem Missing at random Missing completely at random
MAR Missing completely at random MCAR (Crappy names) No problem T1 T2 10 15 7 6 8 2 5

759 Interesting … That wasn’t very interesting
What is more interesting is when: We have missing data Which we won’t talk about more (much) We have multiple measurements of the same people Which we will talk about

760 Modelling Change Can plot and assess trajectories over time
How do people change? What predicts the rate of change?

761 Plotting Individuals Salary Person 1 T2 T1

762 Plotting Individuals Person 3 Salary Person 1 Person 2 T2 T1

763

764

765 Estimation Each individual has an intercept
Sampled from the population of intercepts Each individual has a slope Sampled from the population of slopes Can we estimate the average of each And a measure of their variance? Yes! With multilevel models

766 Multilevel Models Can do all kinds of clever things Used when
We won’t worry about most of them Used when Level 1 units (measures) Are nested within Level 2 units (people) Same person measured twice Violated indepence

767 Levels In regression In multilevel models Everything is at one level
We have multiple levels Hierarchical levels (hence hierarchical linear models) Random effects (random effects models) Mixed effects (mixed models)

768 Levels Level 1 units Level 2 units First level of measurement
Are clustered within Level 2 units Second level of measurement

769 Some Equations (This is very hard. It’s not important). In regression
If x is time And we have one person Reference time with I And call it T

770 Single person equation
But what if we have lots of people? We’ve used i for time We’ll use j for people But everything is fixed We want to have some random effects

771 Let’s make intercepts random
Everyone has their own intercept Look! We added a little j Now it’s a multilevel model And we need an equation for the intercept

772 Equation for each person’s intercept
Your intercept (b0j) is equal to: Mean intercept g (Gamma) Plus residual (for that individual) m0j (mu) This is level 2 model Level 2 residuals i.i.d, etc

773 So now we have Or

774 Make Time Random Value of the time parameter can vary Amongst people
Everyone can have a different effect of time

775 Time is random Everyone has a slope parameter So:

776 Or

777 Time Invariant Covariates
Can be added at level 2 We can predict person’s intercept Starting point And rate of change

778 Employee Data Level 1: Level 2: Pay measures Clustered within People
Two of them Clustered within Level 2: People Level 2 measures: age, sex, job, etc

779 Regression with Time Do a regression analysis Time is the predictor
On one person Time is the predictor Get a regression line for that person

780 Fixed vs Random Effects
Fixed effects Effect is the same across all clusters (people) Variation is only measurement error Random effects Effect varies across people Additional parameter in the model Less parsimonious

781 Fixed vs Random Effects
If an effect has variance It might have covariance With any other effects which also have variance More parameters Less parsimony

782 Covariates Two kinds of covariates Time invariant Time variant
Fixed for a person Age when study started Sex Time variant Can change over time Time Marital Status

783 Time Invariant Look at effect of age Add age to the fixed effects
Is that significant? Are random effects (still?) significant

784 Multilevel Models in Stata
Use xtmixed Stata 10 added xtmelogit, xtmepoisson Continuous variables are hard enough In SPSS, continuous variables only

785 Multilevel Models in Stata
xtmixed sal t Does regression Need to tell it about the people: xtmixed sal t ||id:

786 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------
sal | Coef. Std. Err z P>|z| [95% Conf. Interval] t | _cons | Random-effects Parameters | Estimate Std. Err. [95% Conf. Interval] id: Identity | sd(_cons) | sd(Residual) | LR test vs. linear regression: chibar2(01) = Prob >= chibar2 =

787 Average slope Average intercept SD of the slopes SD of the slopes
sal | Coef. Std. Err z P>|z| [95% Conf. Interval] t | _cons | Random-effects Parameters | Estimate Std. Err. [95% Conf. Interval] id: Identity | sd(_cons) | sd(Residual) | LR test vs. linear regression: chibar2(01) = Prob >= chibar2 = Average intercept SD of the slopes SD of the slopes

788 Gives random intercepts only predict rand_int
Let’s look at them predict rand_int xtline rand_int , overlay t(t) i(id) legend(off)

789

790 Random Slopes Everyone has the same slope xtmixed sal t ||id: t
Maybe that’s not true Make slopes random xtmixed sal t ||id: t

791 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------
sal | Coef. Std. Err z P>|z| [95% Conf. Interval] t | _cons | Random-effects Parameters | Estimate Std. Err. [95% Conf. Interval] id: Independent | sd(t) | sd(_cons) | sd(Residual) |

792 predict rand_int_slope, fitte
xtline rand_int_slope , overlay t(t) i(id) legend(off)

793

794 Structure of the Covariances
We have been forcing slope and intercepts to be uncorrelated Let’s correlate them xtmixed sal t ||id: t , cov(un)

795 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------
sal | Coef. Std. Err z P>|z| [95% Conf. Interval] t | _cons | Random-effects Parameters | Estimate Std. Err. [95% Conf. Interval] id: Unstructured | sd(t) | sd(_cons) | corr(t,_cons) | sd(Residual) | e+10

796 Predicting Change Does another variable moderate the effect of time
This means that the effect of time varies As a function of the slope xi: xtmixed sal i.t*agestart ||id: t

797 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------
sal | Coef. Std. Err z P>|z| [95% Conf. Interval] _It_1 | agestart | _ItXagest_1 | _cons | Random-effects Parameters | Estimate Std. Err. [95% Conf. Interval] id: Unstructured | sd(t) | sd(_cons) | corr(t,_cons) | sd(Residual) | e+09

798 Exercises 13.1, 13.2

799 Fixed Effects Models A second way of looking at longitudinal data
Multilevel (mixed) models Assume that intercepts are random Fixed effects models Assume they are fixed If they are fixed they can correlate With all other predictors

800 Fixed Effects Models Allowing intercepts to correlate
Has the effect of controlling for ALL time invariant predictors Even those you didn’t measure Each person is their own control

801 Fixed Effects Models Regression asks: Fixed effects asks:
Are people who are higher on x also higher on y? Fixed effects asks: When a person is higher on x are they also higher on y Effects are within people, not between people

802 Fixed Effects in Stata Make data long, then
xtreg sal t agestart, i(id)

803 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------
sal | Coef. Std. Err t P>|t| [95% Conf. Interval] t | _cons | sigma_u | sigma_e | rho | (fraction of variance due to u_i) F test that all u_i=0: F(473, 473) = Prob > F =

804 Fixed Effects Regression
Can we look at the effect of time invariant predictors? xtreg sal t agestart, i(id) fe Why not?

805 Interactions But we can look at interactions of time invariant predictors xi: xtreg sal i.t*agestart, i(id) fe

806 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------
sal | Coef. Std. Err t P>|t| [95% Conf. Interval] _It_1 | agestart | (dropped) _ItXagest_1 | _cons | sigma_u | sigma_e | rho | (fraction of variance due to u_i)

807 Exercises 13.3, 13.4

808 Bonus Lesson 1: Why Regression?
A little aside, where we look at why regression has such a curious name.

809 Regression The or an act of regression; reversion; return towards the mean; return to an earlier stage of development, as in an adult’s or an adolescent’s behaving like a child (From Latin gradi, to go) So why name a statistical technique which is about prediction and explanation?

810 Tall fathers have shorter sons Short fathers have taller sons
Francis Galton Charles Darwin’s cousin Studying heritability Tall fathers have shorter sons Short fathers have taller sons ‘Filial regression toward mediocrity’ Regression to the mean

811 Galton thought this was biological fact Then did the analysis backward
Evolutionary basis? Then did the analysis backward Tall sons have shorter fathers Short sons have taller fathers Regression to the mean Not biological fact, statistical artefact

812 Other Examples Secrist (1933): The Triumph of Mediocrity in Business
Second albums often tend to not be as good as first Sequel to a film is not as good as the first one Sports Illustrated Cover Jinx Parents think that punishing bad behaviour works, but rewarding good behaviour doesn’t

813 Accident reduction schemes Poor radiologists improve after training
Always reduce accidents Poor radiologists improve after training Any treatment for a cold will work Or for most illnesses Deaths due to methadone in Utah High last year Must take action!

814 Pair Link Diagram An alternative to a scatterplot x y

815 r=1.00 x

816 r=0.00 x

817 From Regression to Correlation
Where do we predict an individual’s score on y will be, based on their score on x? Depends on the correlation r = 1.00 – we know exactly where they will be r = 0.00 – we have no idea r = 0.50 – we have some idea

818 r=1.00 x y Starts here Will end up here

819 Could end anywhere here
Starts here Could end anywhere here x y

820 Probably end somewhere here
Starts here x y

821 Galton Squeeze Diagram
Don’t show individuals Show groups of individuals, from the same (or similar) starting point Shows regression to the mean

822 r=0.00 x Ends here y Group starts here Group starts here

823 r=0.50 x y

824 r=1.00 x y

825 Correlation is amount of regression that doesn’t occur
x y 1 unit r units Correlation is amount of regression that doesn’t occur

826 x y No regression r=1.00

827 x y Some regression r=0.50

828 r=0.00 Lots (maximum) regression r=0.00 x y

829 Formula

830 regression = perfection – correlation
Conclusion Regression towards mean is statistical necessity regression = perfection – correlation Very non-intuitive Interest in regression and correlation From examining the extent of regression towards mean By Pearson – worked with Galton Stuck with curious name See also Paper B3

831 Correcting for regression to the mean
Possible Makes lots of tricky assumptions To appear to do well in your job / life Do something after someone has failed You probably can’t do worse Hospital / school / department / class / study / experiment If it fails, volunteer to do it

832 Bonus Lesson 2: Other Kinds of Regression

833 Introduction We’ve covered a few kinds of regression
There are many more, for specific types of outcomes

834 Beta Regression Used when the outcome variable is beta distributed
Rates and proportions Bounded by zero and 1 Uniform, or strange shaped distributions

835 Cox Proportional Hazards Regression
Type of survival model Used for time to an event When the event might not occur Developed for medical research

836 Cox Proportional Hazards Regression
E.g. How long does it take for a car to break down The car crashes, and is scrapped We’ll never know – but we want to know the information Discarding the data point would lead to bias

837 Competing Risks Survival
Time to multiple events Several things are trying to kill you Which one succeeds

838 Data Mining Techniques
Avoid problems with stepwise regression Used as alternatives to logistic Boosted regression (Stata command: boost) Semi-parametric alternative to logistic regression Least Angle Regression (LARS)

839 Classification trees

840 Seemingly Unrelated Regression
Used for multiple outcomes With correlated error terms Some say it should be seemingly related Stata command: sureg

841 Instrumental Variables Regression
Used for mediator models Although economists don’t call them that. Use ivregress in Stata

842 Quantile Regression Why do we always try to predict the mean?
What about predicting the median? The 25th percentile? That’s quantile regression

843 Non-Parametric Regression
(Might also include LARS/Lasso/Boost) Don’t force any functional form on the relationship LO(W)ESS – locally weighted scatterplot smoothing Will find any relationship

844 Robust Regression (Careful, not sandwich estimators)
Trimmed of outliers, estimated with bootstrap Lots of publications by Wilcox

845 Censored Regression Tobit regression When a measure is censored
E.g. unemployed people work 0 hours.

846


Download ppt "Applying Regression."

Similar presentations


Ads by Google