Download presentation
Presentation is loading. Please wait.
1
Language Development in Young Children
2
Objectives To examine different theories of first language development. To explain the importance of interactions and input in the development of first language(s). To identify the steps a young child goes through in acquiring his/her first language(s). To analyze how interactions and input are also important in second language acquisition. To identify where different emotions can affect a person’s sequential language acquisition and to propose ways to work with them.
3
The Story of My Name Pick a partner and decide who will speak first and second First speaker tells the story of his/her name for 3 minutes. Second speaker speaks 3 minutes. Large group Briefly introduce your partner and why their name is important to them. Introduce where you work and where you are from. Every child learns to speak Language is shaped to meet the needs of the speakers who use it. Customs, traditions, values, stories, religion, history, and other manifestations of culture are transmitted to large extent through language (both verbal and nonverbal). Reflection question: Think of an example of a special way of communicating that is cultural.
4
The Story of My Name Who chose your name? When was it chosen? Were any other names considered? Are there any other people in your family with the same name? Do you know anyone else with the same name? Do you share your name with any famous people? Is your name unique? What are your feelings concerning sharing (or not sharing) a name with someone else? Have you always been called the same name? What other names have you been called? By whom? When? What are your feelings about your name? Have they changed through time? In what ways? Every child learns to speak Language is shaped to meet the needs of the speakers who use it. Customs, traditions, values, stories, religion, history, and other manifestations of culture are transmitted to large extent through language (both verbal and nonverbal). Reflection question: Think of an example of a special way of communicating that is cultural.
5
The Story of My Name Questions: How was listening for you?
How was speaking for you? What did learn about names? Extension Activities with Names Children write books about names –interviewing family members Parents write books about their children’s names Write a Book About Naming Traditions Why are names important? (They are markers of identity.) Where do our names come from? (They are chosen by family members; they may have family history associated with them; they are part of a culture; they may change depending on the person using it, a person’s age, circumstances, etc.) What rituals or traditions are involved with names? How are these different in different cultures?
6
What is language? Language is uniquely human
Language is linked to cognition Language is natural Language is culturally bound Language has many varieties and is constantly changing. Every child learns to speak Language is shaped to meet the needs of the speakers who use it. Customs, traditions, values, stories, religion, history, and other manifestations of culture are transmitted to large extent through language (both verbal and nonverbal). Reflection question: Think of an example of a special way of communicating that is cultural.
7
What is language? Language has structure Phonology (sounds)
Morphology (structure of words) Syntax (structure of sentences) Semantics (structure of meaning) Pragmatics (rules and conventions of language) How do you think they learn these?
8
How do we learn language?
Behaviorists believe that children learn by imitating what they have heard and that parental modeling and reinforcement are the major promoters of language acquisition. Nativists believe that innate, biological mechanisms are responsible for language acquisition (Chomsky, 1976 – language acquisition device) and that children just naturally acquire language with minor feedback from the social environment. Described on page 7 of Teachers Guide for Comenius Project
9
How do we learn language?
Interactionists acknowledge both the child’s role and that of the caretaker in the social environment and consider the communication in social interactions to be essential to language acquisition. The process of language acquisition is both a personal and social invention. Parents, teachers, and others shape its development by the way they respond to the language learner. We speak differently to young children than to other adults. We adapt our language. Described on page 7 of Teachers Guide for Comenius Project
10
Interactionism We learn language through social processes.
Social constructivism: Vygotsky viewed cognitive development as the result of dialogue and that the main purpose of language is social. We learn language through social processes.
11
The Importance of Interactions
We know that interactions are important in all development including early language development. Video: Serve and Return Interaction Shape Brain Circuitry at
12
The Importance of Interactions
Infants begin life with brain systems that allow them to acquire any and all languages to which they are exposed. The ability to make different sounds are pruned from their brains if they are not heard and supported in social interactions. The language adults use with infants and very young children known as ‘motherese,’ and is an example of interactionism and the value of social processes in language development.
13
The Importance of Interactions
Video: Watch the video, The Pyramid of Speech and Language Development that shows the building blocks of communication. Discussion Question: How do very early interactions that infants have with caregivers build the foundation for speech development? Needs to be written and translated
14
The Importance of Interactions
A child needs a strong foundation to get to the point where they are talking and using accurate speech in their first language(s). 5. Using accurate speech 4. Talking 3. Understanding of language (words, phrases, sentences) 2. Turn taking Eye contact Shared attention Listening 1. Opportunity/Reason/Motivation to Communicate vFirst of all children need to be bathed in language giving them chances to listen, watch and have their initiations responded to. Eye contact helps them make the neural connections and releases hormones that feel good, reinforcing their participation. They need to listen to begin to differentiate different sounds. They engage in shared attention looking at objects named by an adult. And they learn that conversation is about taking turns. After they learn to differentiate sounds, they begin to understand particular words, phrases, and later even sentences. After they begin to have receptive language, they will begin producing language themselves (talking). However, it is not until they are older that begin to speak accurately.
15
Language as a Developmental Process
Babbling One word Two words Short sentences The Four Stages in Acquiring Language Language Acquisition 1 For example, all animals may be categorized under the word ‘dog’. They are also learning the proper ways to use words and turn taking rules of conversation with such phrases as “bye” and “no”.
16
Language as a Developmental Process
At approximately two years of age, children’s ability to use language suddenly increases rapidly. The size of their vocabulary grows and children begin to string words together in short sentences. Children are acquiring 6 to 10 words a day about the time they are entering preschool. They are also broadening their understanding of the meanings of the words they already know. Described on page 7 of Teachers Guide for Comenius Project
17
Language as a Developmental Process
Children also begin to acquire the more complicated grammar forms during this period. Their understanding of grammatical rules, however, can result in creative mistakes. These mistakes demonstrate that they notice consistent patterns in language and apply them to the language system, as they understand them. Described on pages 8 paragraphs 5 and 4 and page 9 paragraph 1 of Teachers Guide for Comenius Project
18
Language as a Developmental Process
By the time children enter preschool, they have become experienced talkers and are engaged in an extended oral language development. This is because their caregivers have been encouraging them to extend their statements through talking with them. Children also augment their own language experiences by listening to others’ conversations in which they are not active participants. Described on pages 8 paragraphs 5 and 4 and page 9 paragraph 1 of Teachers Guide for Comenius Project
19
Simultaneous and Sequential Bilingualism
Simultaneous acquisition of two languages occurs when children are exposed to both languages from a very early age, sometimes as a result of each parent speaking a separate language with the child or both parents speaking one language and a caregiver speaking another language with the child. If a child learns two languages simultaneously, and if the two languages are developed equally during childhood, then the language development process is expected to be the same in both languages. (Tabors, 2008) Children may learn multiple languages in different ways. They may learn languages either simultaneously as babies (or very young children) which Piper (1998) calls “home bilingualism“ or they can learn them sequentially which Piper calls “school bilingualism“ because many children who later become bilingual learn their second language at school.
20
Simultaneous and Sequential Bilingualism
Sequential acquisition occurs when a child begins to learn a second language after the first language is at least partly established, for example when a young child enters a preschool setting in which her home language is not the language used in the classroom.
21
Simultaneous and Sequential Bilingualism
Sequential language acquisition however is different: Children already have some knowledge of how language works so they are not learning how language works in general but how the new language works and is different from the first language.
22
Simultaneous and Sequential Bilingualism
The acquisition of the second language can take place at any age. The acquisition of the second language may be more dependent on individual characteristics such as aptitude, personality (whether social or not), and psychological factors such as wanting to be like others. However as very young children have more capability to produce different sounds (see previous module) , they do have an advantages in developing the sound system of the second language, whereas later on articulation is already restricted to the sound system of the first language.)
23
Krashen’s Hypothesis Stephen Krashen (1982) presented several hypothesis that today help us understand how children and adults learn new languages: The Acquisition versus Learning Hypothesis. It states that new languages are actually best acquired in the same way that children acquire their first language(s) and not learned by formal teaching. Acquisition requires meaningful interactions in the new language in natural communication in which native speakers are focused on communication and not on form.
24
Krashen’s Hypothesis The Input Hypothesis: Learning to speak the new language requires meaningful input that is at the optimal level. It suggests that learners acquire language by “intaking” and understanding language that is a “little beyond” their current level of competence or “input + 1“. zone of proximal development scaffolds where possible. For instance, a preschool child already understands the phrase, “Get your crayon.” By slightly altering the phrase to “Get my crayons,” the teacher can provide an appropriate linguistic and cognitive challenge—offering new information that builds on prior knowledge and is therefore comprehensible.
25
Krashen’s Hypothesis The Affective Filter Hypothesis: Emotions such as anxiety can directly interfere or assist with in the learning of new language. Some emotions can create a kind of filter that blocks the learner’s ability to learn and use new words or grammatical structures because it can block the input that is needed. Others have added to this list to include high level of empathy for others, a willingness to take risks, and low levels of inhibitions. For example, motivation is a main factor when living in another country. Learning is easier when one is “playing” because it lowers learner anxiety.
26
Krashen’s Hypothesis The three main things that can make the affective filter’s openings larger and allow more input in the new language to reach the leaner include that the learner have: high motivation to learn the language, have low anxiety levels, high self-confidence in ability. Others have added to this list to include high level of empathy for others, a willingness to take risks, and low levels of inhibitions. For example, motivation is a main factor when living in another country. Learning is easier when one is “playing” because it lowers learner anxiety.
27
Krashen’s Hypothesis Discussion: Give personal examples of what helped you or did not help you learn a new language by: Increasing the level and quality of input you received in the new language. Acquiring it in a natural way instead of learning new vocabulary and grammar rules. Lowering or increasing your affective filter by lowering or increasing your anxiety, increasing or lowering your self-confidence, or increasing or lowering your motivation to learn a new language.
28
Developmental Phases in Acquiring Second Language
1. Home language use phase : There may be a period of time when children still use their first language. This is because they do not yet understand that there is difference in the 2 languages. 2. The silent or nonverbal period: When children discover that their home language does not work in the new setting, they enter into a nonverbal period as they collect information about the new language and perhaps spend some time in sound experimentation. In an official-language classroom, this means that the children will enter a period when they do not talk at all. In a bilingual classroom, it means that children will only attempt to talk with those who speak their home language and will no longer try to talk with those who do not. There also appears to be an age component with the length of time children spend in this phase with younger children remaining silent for longer periods of time than older children.
29
Developmental Phases in Acquiring Second Language
3. Going public with the new language: This is when children begin to use individual words and phrases that they have learned. Verbalizations such as “what is that?”, or reciting the names of colors, the alphabet, numbers, etc. or formulaic phrases in situations in which others have been observed to use them such as “yes, no, hello, goodbye, O.K., look, be careful,” etc.
30
Developmental Phases in Acquiring Second Language
4. Productive Language Use in the New Language: This is when children can begin building their own sentences, not just continuing to repeat formulaic phrases or names for people and things. During this process the children must analyze the language being used around them and begin to make guesses about how the language is constructed. Typically, they use everything they already know about their new language, and, not surprisingly, make many mistakes as they work their way through the process of acquiring the more complicated aspects of second language.
31
Developmental Phases in Acquiring Second Language
For older children and adults the process is similar and looks like (Northwest Regional Educational Laboratory, 2003): Silent/receptive or preproduction stage: This stage can last from 10 hours to six months. Students often have up to 500 “receptive” words (words they understand but may not be comfortable using) and can understand new words that are made comprehensible to them. This stage often involves a “silent period” during which students may not speak but can respond using a variety of strategies, including pointing to an object, picture, or person; performing an act, such as standing up or closing a door; gesturing or nodding; or responding with a simple “yes” or “no.”
32
Developmental Phases in Acquiring Second Language
Early production stage: The early production stage can last an additional six months after the initial stage. Students have usually developed close to 1,000 receptive/active words (words they are able to understand and use). During this stage, students can usually speak in one- or two-word phrases and can demonstrate comprehension of new material by giving short answers to simple yes/no, either/or, or who/what/where questions. Speech emergence stage: This stage can last up to another year. Students have usually developed approximately 3,000 words and can use short phrases and simple sentences to communicate. Students begin to use dialogue and can ask simple questions, such as “May I go to the restroom?” and are also able to answer simple questions. Students may produce longer sentences but often with grammatical errors that can interfere with their communication.
33
Developmental Phases in Acquiring Second Language
Intermediate language proficiency stage: Individuals may need up to another year to reach intermediate proficiency after speech emergence. Students will have typically developed close to 6,000 words and can now make complex statements, state opinions, ask for clarification, share thoughts, and speak at greater length. Advanced language proficiency stage: Gaining advanced proficiency in a second language can typically take from five to seven years. By this stage students have developed some specialized content-area vocabulary and can participate fully in grade-level classroom activities if given occasional extra support. Reflection Question: What phase of the process in acquiring a language that is not your first language are you? What would help you progress to the next level?
34
Misconceptions on language development
True or False: Bilingualism causes language delay.
35
Misconceptions on language development
Bilingualism causes language delay. FALSE. While a bilingual child’s vocabulary in each individual language may be smaller than average, his total vocabulary (from both languages) will be at least the same size as a monolingual child. Bilingual children may say their first words slightly later than monolingual children, but still within the normal age range (between 8-15 months). And when bilingual children start to produce short sentences, they develop grammar along the same patterns and timelines as children learning one language. If a bilingual child demonstrates significant delays in language milestones, they could have a language disorder and should be seen by a speech language pathologist.
36
Misconceptions on language development
True or False: When children mix their languages it means that they are confused and having trouble becoming bilingual.
37
Misconceptions on language development
When children mix their languages it means that they are confused and having trouble becoming bilingual. FALSE. When children use both languages within the same sentence or conversation, it is known as “code mixing” or “code switching”. Parents sometimes worry that this mixing is a sign of language delay or confusion. However, code mixing is a natural part of bilingualism. Proficient adult bilinguals code mix when they converse with other bilinguals, and it should be expected that bilingual children will code-mix when speaking with other bilinguals. Many researchers see code mixing as a sign of bilingual proficiency. For example, bilingual children adjust the amount of code-mixing they use to match that of a new conversational partner (someone they’ve never met before who also code mixes) (5). It has also been suggested that children code-mix when they know a word in one language but not the other (13). Furthermore, sometimes code-mixing is used to emphasize something, express emotion, or to highlight what someone else said in the other language. Code-mixing is natural and should be expected in bilingual children.
38
Misconceptions on language development
True or False: A person is not truly bilingual unless he is equally proficient in both languages.
39
Misconceptions on language development
A person is not truly bilingual unless he is equally proficient in both languages. FALSE. It is rare to find an individual who is equally proficient in both languages. Most bilinguals have a “dominant language”, a language of greater proficiency. The dominant language is often influenced by the majority language of the society in which the individual lives. An individual’s dominant language can change with age, circumstance, education, social network, employment, and many other factors. Many researchers see code mixing as a sign of bilingual proficiency. For example, bilingual children adjust the amount of code-mixing they use to match that of a new conversational partner (someone they’ve never met before who also code mixes) (5). It has also been suggested that children code-mix when they know a word in one language but not the other (13). Furthermore, sometimes code-mixing is used to emphasize something, express emotion, or to highlight what someone else said in the other language. Code-mixing is natural and should be expected in bilingual children.
40
Misconceptions on language development
True or False: An individual must learn a second language as a young child in order to become bilingual.
41
Misconceptions on language development
An individual must learn a second language as a young child in order to become bilingual. FALSE. There is a “Critical Period” theory that suggests that there is a window of time (early childhood) during which a second language is most easily learned. This theory has led many people to believe that it is better to learn a second language as a young child. Young children have been found to achieve better native-like pronunciation than older children or adult second language learners. And they seem to achieve better long-term grammatical skills than older learners. But other findings have called the idea of a critical period into question. For example: Older children (in middle elementary school) have been shown to have advantages when learning “academic” English. “Academic” language refers to the specialized vocabulary, grammar, and conversational ability needed to understand and learn in school. This is likely easier for older children because they learn their second language with more advanced cognitive skills than younger children, and with more experience with schooling and literacy (10).
42
Misconceptions on language development
True or False: If a family wants their child to speak the majority language, they should stop speaking their home language with their child.
43
Misconceptions on language development
If you want your child to speak the majority language, you should stop speaking your home language with your child. FALSE. Some parents attempt to speak the majority language to their child because they want their child to learn that language, even if they themselves are not fluent in the majority language. This can mean that conversations and interactions do not feel natural or comfortable between parent and child. There is no evidence that frequent use of the second language in the home is essential for a child to learn a second language. Furthermore, without knowledge of a family’s home language, a child can become isolated from family members who only speak the home language. Research shows that children who have a strong foundation in their home language more easily learn a second language. Children are also at great risk of losing their home language if it is not supported continually at home. Many researchers see code mixing as a sign of bilingual proficiency. For example, bilingual children adjust the amount of code-mixing they use to match that of a new conversational partner (someone they’ve never met before who also code mixes) (5). It has also been suggested that children code-mix when they know a word in one language but not the other (13). Furthermore, sometimes code-mixing is used to emphasize something, express emotion, or to highlight what someone else said in the other language. Code-mixing is natural and should be expected in bilingual children.
Similar presentations
© 2024 SlidePlayer.com Inc.
All rights reserved.