Download presentation
Presentation is loading. Please wait.
1
Chapter 9 Pt 1 Language
2
Language – shared symbolic system for communication.
Natural Language: Emerged from peoples’ attempts to communicate. culturally agreed upon (arbitrary) symbolic system - refers to things not currently present. / to abstract concepts (e.g., love) Nearly five thousand languages are spoken in the world today.
3
Is language Learned or do humans have an innate ability to learn language?
4
7th Century BC – Egypt Psamtik – thought language was innate (inborn)!
He sought to discover the origin of language by conducting an experiment with two children. He gave two newborn babies to a shepherd, with the instructions that no one should speak to them, but that the shepherd should feed and care for them while listening to determine their first words. The hypothesis was that the first word would be uttered in the root language of all people.
5
When one of the children cried "bekos" with outstretched arms the shepherd concluded that the word was Phrygian because that was the sound of Phrygian word for "bread." Thus, they concluded that the Phrygians were an older people than the Egyptians, and that Phrygian was the original language of men.
6
We are not born speaking. Language must be acquired
We are not born speaking! Language must be acquired. If we think of all that is entailed in knowing a language, it seems quite a challenge. Child acquire language very quickly. By 5 they have mastered grammar. 3 year old talking about a monster on TV.
7
Noam Chomsky The ability to learn language is instinctive
His theory explains why all babies language development follows a pattern. Humans have a Language Acquisition Device (LAD) – a structure within the brain that allows babies to absorb and understand the rules of language they are being exposed to. The brain is able to analyse the language and work out the system that the language uses.
8
Explains why children can quickly understand and then use their language creatively and correctly without ever being formally taught or ‘knowing’ the rules
9
All languages have a Grammar
Nouns and verbs Subjects and objects Consonants and vowels Basic word order (English is Subject, Verb, Object) e.g., The man kicked the Ball. Greenberg (1963) – in 98% of languages subject comes before the object.
10
Pidgin and Creoles Languages
Pidgin is a simplified language that develops as a means of communication between two or more groups that do not have a language in common. Commonly used in trade. A pidgin is not the native language of any speech community, but is instead learned as a second language. A pidgin may be built from words, sounds, or body language from multiple other languages and cultures. Pidgins allow people or a group of people to communicate with each other without having any similarities in language and do not have any rules, as long as both parties are able to understand each other.
11
Creoles have been nativized by children as their primary language, with the result that they have features of natural languages that are normally missing from pidgins. Hawaii Creole
12
Language Bioprogram Hypothesis Bickerton (1984)
Creolization occurs when the linguistic exposure of children in a community consists of a highly unstructured pidgin. Children use their innate language capacity to transform the pidgin into a language with a highly structured grammar. As this capacity is universal, the grammars of these new languages have many similarities.
13
Bickerton (1983), ungrammatical utterances made by English-speaking children (2 – 4 years) are very similar to perfectly grammatical sentences of English-based creole languages. Child Creole Where I can put it? Where I can put om? Hawaii Daddy throw the nother rock Daddy t'row one neda rock'tone Jamaica I go full Angela bucket Guyana Lookit a boy play ball Luku one boy a play ball Nobody don't like me Nobody no like me I no like do that Johnny big more than me Let Daddy get pen write it Make Daddy get pen write am I more better than Johnny
14
The Birth of New Sign Language in Nicaragua
15
What About English? History of English (to 2.15) The Roman army departed in 410 AD. Mercenary soldiers brought in who were Angles and Saxons from northern Germany. Norman conquest of England in Ruling class spoke a dialect of French. Lower class spoke a dialect of German.
16
Psycholinguistics The study of language as it is learned and used by people. Looks at the “pragmatics” of natural language use.
17
Language Universals Features that are common to all languages.
Semanticity - language conveys meaning!! Semantic universals - All languages have pronouns. - All Languages distinguish between male and female; living and non-living
18
Semanticity Language signals have meaning Four legged animal
Common pet Fur Chases cats Barks Etc. “dog” The meaning of linguistic elements aren’t always apparent. Some may serve syntax or functional roles (e.g. AND)
19
2. Arbitrariness no necessary or natural relationship
between the words of a given language and the concepts that they represent. For example, there is nothing in the word "tree" that connects it to the concept of a tree; which is why Spanish can use a totally different sign for the same concept: "árbol"; and so on with other languages.
20
Arbitrariness No resemblance between the language signal and the thing that it represents “dog” “hund” There are exceptions to this. “the hissing of a snake” hiss sounds like the actual sound made by the snake. This is called Onomatopoeia “chien” “perro”
21
Combination of signs (words) to produce complete thoughts are different from one language to the other. No set of rules can claim to be the "right" one. For example, in English you say "I like beer", whereas in Spanish you would say "Me gusta la cerveza". The literal translation of the latter would be something like: "Beer is agreeable to me", which sounds strange in English. Neither of these formulations has a better claim to accuracy, correctness or truth than the other.
22
3. Flexibility Because the connection between the word and the meaning are arbitrary – we can change them and invent new ones. Symbols can be combined to make endless new meaningful words (e.g., nonmicrowavable or anitdisestablishmentarianism)
23
4. Naming We assign names to everything we see, feel and can conceive of. When there is a need for a name – we produce one. How many English words have you witnessed the birth of???
24
5. Displacement We can communicate about things that are not currently present. ~ other locations, times, realities.
25
6. Productivity Infinite number of (meaningful statements) that can be produced. We can say things we have never said before, never heard before. We generate language (on line) rather than repeat it. We learn patterns and rules of producing meaning and then are free to use these rules to produce a way of conveying thoughts to other people.
26
Productivity Our use of language is extremely creative.
We have a limited amount of linguistic elements (e.g., sounds and words), but can combine those elements in novel ways. “I was tired of cleaning up after my dog in my backyard so I taught him to pole vault.” Even though you’ve never heard this sentence before you can understand it effortlessly
27
Language Form: Phonology and Orthography
A phoneme is a sound, or set of similar speech sounds, which are perceived as a single distinctive sound by speakers of the language or dialect in question. For example, the "c/k" sounds in cat and kitten represent the English phoneme /k/.
28
There are 44 phonemes in English (in the standard British model and 40 in American English). Since there are only 26 letters in the alphabet (Orthography), sometimes letter combinations need to be used to make a phoneme. A letter (or combination of letters) can represent different phonemes. “Ch” is a good example: chef = /ʃef/ choir = /kwaɪə/ cheese = /tʃi:z/
29
Phonemic Rules Each language also has a set of rules for combining phonemes (what can and cannot go together).
30
English Spelling English’s history as a creole shows especially in spelling - numerous irregular or exception words. Comb Yacht Paradigm Though Danger, Anger, Hanger
31
Morphology Morphemes – smallest units that convey meaning (words and prefixes and suffixes). Free Morphemes – can stand alone (e.g., words)
32
Bounded Morphemes – must be attached to other morphemes (e. g
Bounded Morphemes – must be attached to other morphemes (e.g., the plural “s”). Can add grammatical features (e.g., plural, past tense, etc.). Change meaning (e.g., “un”, “dis” or “ultra”
33
Syntax (Grammar) Rule that specify ordering of words by grammatical properties (e.g., nouns and verbs). Word order makes a difference! Dog bites man. Man bites dog. Bites man dog.
34
Phrase Structure - organization of sentence constituents Hierarchical Sentences Phrases -words with grammatical roles (e.g., verbs, nouns, adjectives etc.)
35
Phrase-structure rules are a way to describe a given language's syntax
Phrase-structure rules are a way to describe a given language's syntax. They are used to break down a natural language sentence into its constituent parts. What is a constituent? A word or group of words that function as a unit and can make up larger grammatical units.
36
Tree diagram - levels of constituents
Sentence Noun Phrase Verb Phrase Article Adj Noun Verb Noun Phrase Article adj Noun The red squirrel buried the large nut.
37
Sentences can be rearranged as long as the constitutes are intact.
E.g., George/ ran /up the mountain. Up the mountain/George/ran. Martha/ stood up /her blind date. Up her blind date Martha stood.
38
the red squirrel buried the large nut
the red squirrel the large nut buried the large nut the red squirrel buried the large nut buried by the red squirrel buried the large nut the red squirrel buried the red squirrel the large nut Rearrangement of Constituents follow rules.
39
Phrase structure rules
Rules that determine… what goes into a phrase (‘constituents’) how the constituents are ordered
40
Transformational Grammar (Chomsky)
1. Surface Structure - actual words used 2. Deep structure - underlying meaning - abstract
41
The same deep structure can by conveyed
using several different surface structures. The cat has a big bushy tail. The feline has a big fluffy tail. The cat’s tail is big and bushy. The big bushy tail is the cat’s.
42
Similar surface structures can have very
different deep structure. The lady hit the man with the umbrella.
43
Transformational Grammar rules translates
Kernels to other surface structures. E.g., John smelled the cookies (KERNAL, active) The cookies were smelled by John (passive) Did John smell the cookies? (interrogative) Were the cookies smelled by John? (passive, interrogative) Were the cookies not smelled by John? (passive, interrogative, negation)
44
In general, more transformations, the
longer comprehension takes. Take home lesson: – when possible, speak and write in the active voice.
45
Semantics The relationship between linguistic elements (e.g., words and phrases) and their underlying meaning. Meaning can often be expressed with different words (phrases). Words (phrases) often can refer to different meanings. Mary stood up tall vs. Mary stood up Tom.
46
Pragmatics Pragmatics is a subfield of linguistics that studies the ways in which context contributes to meaning. Pragmatics encompasses speech act theory, conversational implications, and interactions. Views language as a social exchange.
47
Principles of Communication: Grice’s Maxims
Cooperative Principle A basic underlying assumption we make when we speak to one another is that we are trying to cooperate with one another to construct meaningful conversations. Violations of these maxims can be indicators of sarcasm or non-literal meaning.
48
Maxims Quantity : Make your contribution as informative as is required, but not more, or less, than is required. Quality: Do not say that which you believe to be false or for which you lack evidence. Relation: Be relevant Manner: Be clear, brief and orderly
49
Turn Taking The process by which people alternate between speaking and listening. Overlap of turns (when two or more participants talk at the same time) occurs in about 5% of cases and this suggests that speakers know how, when and where to enter. They signal that one turn has come to an end and another should begin.
50
Turn Taking Cues Intonation Eye Gaze Gestures
In general the current speaker chooses the next speaker Ways of hanging on to ones turn. Hedges (meaningless sounds or repetitions)
51
OVERLAPPING RULES Where, despite the rules, overlapping talk occurs (5%), studies revealed the operation of a system: one speaker drops out rapidly as soon as one speaker thus ‘gets into the clear’, he typically recycles precisely the part of the turn obscured by the overlap. If one speaker does not immediately drop out, there is available a competitive allocation system, whereby the speaker who ‘upgrades’ most, wins the floor. (upgrading = increased amplitude, slowing tempo, lengthened vowels, etc.)
52
Maxim of Quantity : Make your contribution as informative as is required, but not more, or less, than is required. Often violated (33% of time) Perhaps figuring out what is needed is cognitively difficult – leading to errors.
53
Enhancing Communication
Gestures More Gestures used when people think they are communicating with others rather than to a machine (Mol et al., 2009). Speakers adjust their gestures to adjust to the listener’s needs
54
Speakers underestimate the value of Gestures to the listener (Gerwing & Allison, 2009).
Speakers used words and gestures to describe the layout of an apartment. Speakers judged only 25% of their gestures as providing essential information that was missing from the speech Actual analysis indicated that almost all gestures (97%) contributed information that was not in the words.
55
Figure 9.2 The number of times participants conveyed information about location, relative location, size, and shape by gestures and by words. From Gerwing and Allison (2009). Copyright © 2009 John Benjamins Publishing Company.
56
Gestures commonly used to convey spatial information (Bevelas, 2008)
57
(Bevelas, 2008) Speakers also use gestures on the phone (not for communication). Gestures on phone are fewer and smaller than face-to-face. Speaker describing the top of the skirt (telephone condition). Arrows indicate the size and direction of the gestures.
58
Restricting gestures reduced number of descriptions participants were able to give a name to (Frick-Horbury & Guttentag, 1998) Example: A thin oval tablet with a hole for the thumb at one end by which a painter holds it and mixes different shades of pigment on it. (Pallette)
59
Discourse Markers a word or phrase that is relatively syntax-independent and does not change the meaning of the sentence, and has a somewhat empty meaning.
60
Discourse Markers Analysis shows these seemingly meaningless utterances serve roles in conversation. “um” – indicate problems deciding what to say next. “you know” – check for understanding. “like” – mark of sarcasm (avoid in job interviews) “oh and so” – change of topic Oh - change is relevant to speaker So – change is relevant to listener
61
Prosodic Cues Rhythm, stress and intonation
Used to disambiguate the meaning of sentences.
62
Psychopaths produce more beats
Hare made another intriguing discovery by observing the hand gestures (called beats) people make while speaking. Research has shown that such gestures do more than add visual emphasis to our words (many people gesture while they're on the telephone, for example); it seems they actually help our brains find words. That's why the frequency of beats increases when someone is having trouble finding words, or is speaking a second language instead of his or her mother tongue. In a 1991 paper, Hare and his colleagues reported that psychopaths, especially when talking about things they should find emotional, such as their families, produce a higher frequency of beats than normal people. It's as if emotional language is a second language -- a foreign language, in effect -- to the psychopath.
63
Common Ground Mutual beliefs, expectations and knowledge
Speakers more likely to make incorrect local assumptions (what the listener knows or is attending to) than to make incorrect global (preferred language, general knowledge, shared personal experiences).
64
Judging Common Ground takes Cognitive Resources
Horton and Keysar (1996) speakers describe objects for listeners. When descriptions made under no time constraints incorporated common ground with the listener, common ground was not used when the speakers were under time pressure.
65
Clark & Krych (2004) Pairs of participants were videotaped as a director instructed a builder in assembling 10 Lego models. Three conditions: directors could see the builders’ workspace; they could not see workspace gave instructions by audiotape. Partners were much slower when directors could not see the builders workspace, and they made many more errors when the instructions were audiotaped (5% vs. 39%).
66
In condition one, directors often rapidly altered what they said to maximize common ground. Monitoring and Adjustment model assumes that common ground does not play a role in the initial plan of utterances. Initial plan is not designed for the specific knowledge of the addressee. Speakers plan their utterances using information which is available to them regardless of whether or not the information they use is part of the common ground with the addressee.
67
When their workspace was visible, builders communicated with directors by exhibiting, poising, pointing at, placing, and orienting blocks, and by eye gaze, head nods, and head shakes, all timed with precision. Directors often responded by altering their utterances midcourse.
68
Speech Perception Speech is difficult to decode because: Language is spoken very quickly (10 phonemes per sec). Energy breaks do not correspond to breaks between words. Ch 8 pt 2
69
Coarticulation In spoken language, producing a word requires a speaker to coordinate five to six different parts of your vocal tract (tongue, lips, larynx, etc.) with millisecond precision.
70
Coarticulation The overlap of articulation in space and time. Production of one speech sound overlaps the production of the next. The speaker adjusts the shape of their articulatory apparatus (tongue, lips, mouth) in different ways depending on what sounds come next. (e.g., H is Happy vs. Home). They do this in anticipation of the next sound.
71
Invariance Problem Difficult to identify core features that correspond to particular phonemes.
72
Categorical Perception
Differences can be perceived as gradual and quantitative, as with different shades of gray, or they can be perceived as more abrupt and qualitative, as with different colors. The first is called continuous perception and the second categorical perception.
73
Phonetic Boundary
74
Despite differences in speakers voice, pitch, accent, and or enunciation, we perceive phonemes within a category as all the same. Due to specialized neurons (Wernicke's area in the left Temporal lobe) that act like feature detectors and respond to specific phonemes.
75
How do these phoneme specialized detectors develop? They are learned.
Habituation studies on young infants show that they can distinguish between phonemes that are not used in their native language, but by two years old they are specialized in detecting differences in phonemes of their own language.
76
E.g., People exposed to English can easily distinguish between /r/ and /l/ but many Japanese speakers cannot. In English, phoneme separate phoneme detectors respond to /r/ than do to /l/. In Asian languages these two sounds are perceived as the same.
77
More on Development By the time a child is 10, they lose the ability to detect differences between phonemes that are not used in languages that they have been exposed to. This is one reason why it is difficult for older children and adults to pick up new languages.
78
Speech Perception Involves both Bottom-up and Top-down processes.
Degraded auditory input impairs speech perception (bottom-up) Using knowledge about words and context, we fill-in unclear auditory information (Top-down) Ch 8 pt 2
79
Word Superiority Effect
People have better recognition of letters presented within words as compared to isolated letters and to letters presented within non-words. Why, processing occurs simultaneously at the feature, letter and word level. Processing is added (sped-up) by top-down information (words) when they can be used to help disambiguate information at lower levels.
80
Word Superiority Effect
81
Phoneme Restoration Effect
The brain's way of resolving those imperfections in our speech. Sounds actually missing from a speech signal can be restored by the brain and may appear to be heard. Demo audio clip 1 and 2
82
Context Effects Top Down Processes
Lexical Identification Shift Ganong (1980) demonstrated a bias to perceive phonemes so they form words. For example sounds that could be either /d/ or /t/ tend to be heard as /t/ when followed by "ask" (to make the word "task") and as /d/ when followed by "ash" (to make the word "dash"). Ch 8 pt 2
83
Try this example: identify the phoneme at the end of each utterance, is it /s/ (as in "lease") or /S/ (as in "leash")? Ch 8 pt 2
84
Lexicon Recognition and Access
Lexicon – mental dictionary Most people know more than 60,000 different words, and well-educated people know more than 100,000.
85
Factors that Effect Word Recognition
Lexical frequency Size of lexical neighborhood Morphological complexity Context (i.e., semantic priming effects)
86
Sweeney (1979) Ambiguous sentences containing words with multiple meanings. “He measured the floor with his ruler.”
87
Followed by letter string
A) a word related to the implied meaning “inch” was presentd B) a word related to alternative meaning “king” was presented C) an unrelated word “Pill” D) a non-word “jokt” Lexical decision task If both meanings activated A and B should be equally fast (primed)
88
Varied the delay between the sentence and
the letter string. 400 millisecond delay responses to A and B facilitated. Over 700 milliseconds, only the A was facilitated. So both are activated, but one fades quickly.
89
Disambiguation and telling a joke. Why timing is important
Disambiguation and telling a joke. Why timing is important. Humor is an emotional reaction to violation of schema. Some jokes rely on setting the listener up for one interpretation, and then providing more information that negates the original interpretation. Timing - If both meanings are activated, schema is not violated. Need to pause to allow alternate meaning to fade.
90
Reading – Eye Tracking Studies
Gaze Duration Eye Movement Fixations Saccades Regression Video Ch 8 pt 2
91
Readers typically fixate about 80% of content words (nouns, verbs, adjectives). Only 20% of function words (pronouns, conjunctions, articles) are fixated. Words not fixated are short, common or predictable. Longer fixations occur on longer, unusual or unpredictable words. Ch 8 pt 2
92
Understanding Sentences
Grammar – Rules of combining words within a sentence. Parsing - analyzing a sentence into its component categories and functions. Ch 8 pt 2
93
Syntax/Grammar – word order and combination critical to meaning:
“He showed her the boys pants.” “He showed her boys the pants.” Same words, different order produces different meaning. Ch 8 pt 2
94
When making sense of sentences we use both semantic and syntactic (grammatical) information. Sentence meaning can however be ambiguous … sometimes because of the grammatical structure. Ch 8 pt 2
95
Garden Path (Structurally Ambiguous) Sentences
Grammatically correct sentences that starts in such a way that a reader's most likely interpretation will be incorrect; the reader is lured into a parse that turns out to be a dead end. e.g., The horse raced by the barn fell Ch 8 pt 2
96
Garden Path Sentence Examples
The raft floated down the river sank. The florist sent the flowers was pleased. The cotton clothing is made from grows in Mississippi. They told the boy that the girl met the story. The daughter of the king's son admires himself.
97
Parsing Four major possibilities:
Syntactic analysis generally precedes (and influences) semantic analysis Semantic analysis usually occurs prior to syntactic analysis Syntactic and semantic analysis occur at the same time, in parallel.
98
Sytax-first model Pick one syntactical structure early on, later have to revise if it isn’t the right one. Simpler More Complex
99
Activation Strength creates Syntactic Heuristics (rules of thumb) Principles Simple structure preferred over more complex structures. Ch 8 pt 2
100
Newer Research Semantics can override the syntax-first approach. e.g., The police arrested the mastermind behind the hideout, but they forgot to read him his rights. (simpler structure – but leads to ambiguity).
101
Changing one word changes the ambiguity, but the simpler structure does not make sense. e.g., The police arrested the mastermind behind the crime, but they forgot to read him his rights.
102
Results If choosing syntax first, the “crime” sentence should be difficult because it requires a more complex sentence structure, but reading times were faster for the “crime” sentence than for the “hideout” sentence.
103
Conclusion Both semantics and syntax are interpreted as the sentence is processed. When the semantics is ambiguous, simpler structures are first considered, then abandoned for more complex ones, slowing down the process.
104
Chapter 9 Language Pt 2
105
Speech Errors Given the complexities of speaking, it's not surprising that about one slip of the tongue on average occurs per every 500 sentences spoken. Errors tell us a great deal about speech production.
106
Language Production General Rule of Speech Errors: mistakes are typically at the expense of meaning. Proper syntax is maintained. What does this tell us? We commit to a syntactic format, before we assign lexical units to the utterance.
107
Production of Speech Stage 1: Conceptualization – non-verbal meaning that we wish to communicate.
108
Stage 2: Formulization – Grammatical processing where the message is mapped into linguistic units.
A: functional stage: select semantically appropriate items and assign them functional roles (noun, verb, object). B: positional stage: syntactic structure corresponding to the functional roles is built, and lexical items are inserted into the syntactical structure.
109
Tree diagram - levels of constituents
Sentence Noun Phrase Verb Phrase Article Adj Noun Verb Noun Phrase Article adj Noun The Grey squirrel buried the large nut.
110
How do we know that this is how utterance are built?
Evidence from Speech Errors Word Order Errors e.g., “The Grey nut was buried by the large squirrel”. Syntactic Category Rule Word errors occur at the “functional level”. Words with same grammatical function are most likely to be exchanged. In 99% of cases nouns switched with nouns, verbs with verbs but not nouns with verbs (Hotopf, 1980).
111
Word Exchanges are most likely to occur across phrases.
Garrett (1980) studied errors in spontaneous speech. -83% of word exchanges occurred across phrase boundaries. Suggests all words are available in parallel but were mapped to the phrase structure incorrectly.
112
Tip-of-the Tongue Have access to the semantic and syntactic representation of the word, but not the phonological. Semantic and phonological stages are separate.
113
Brown and McNeil (1966) – Is the feeling of knowing an illusion?
Task: Retrieve the word corresponding to its provided definition e.g. “A musical instrument comprising a frame holding a series of tubes struck by hammers” Participants were asked to indicate if they were in a ToT state If so, guess the number of syllables and any other information about the word (e.g. first letter) Results: Participants are better at remembering associated information than they were at producing the actual word (e.g. XYLOPHONE)
114
Schreifer, Meyer & Levelt (1990)
Presented speakers with object pictures (e.g., dog) to be named while they ignored a word simultaneously presented through headphones (prime). Manipulated type of prime Word was either phonologically related (e.g., dot) or semantically related (e.g., cat) or unrelated (e.g., ship). Manipulated timing 150 ms before , with the presentation of the picture or 150 ms after the picture
115
Results Dependent variable was the time taken to name the picture. When the prime was semantically related, it interfered with naming only in the early and simultaneous conditions. When the prime was phonologically related it interfered with naming only in the delayed condition. Indicates that Semantic and Phonological stages are separate.
117
3. Articulation - Converting Structure to Sounds
Generating the spoken sentence occurs a phrase at a time. Evidence In spontaneous speech pauses are longer between phases then within them. - pause is used to generate the next phrase. - when people repeat or correct themselves, they tend to repeat the entire phrase.
118
Spoonerisms A spoonerism is an error in speech or deliberate play on words in which corresponding consonants, vowels, or morphemes are switched.
119
Reverend Spooner Oxford University lecturer in history, philosophy, and divinity (1876 to 1889). His tendency to get words and sounds mixed up could happen at any time, but especially when he was agitated. He reprimanded one student for "fighting a liar in the quadrangle" and another who "hissed my mystery lecture." To the latter he added in disgust, "You have tasted two worms."
120
Patriotic fervor excited Spooner as well
Patriotic fervor excited Spooner as well. He raised his toast to Her Highness Victoria: "Three cheers for our queer old dean!"
121
During WWI he reassured his students, "When our boys come home from France, we will have the hags flung out." He praised Britain's farmers as "noble tons of soil."
122
Sound (phonemic) Errors: Getting your “Mirds Wixed” Phoneme production errors.
Occur because of mis-timing or mis-ordering of sounds at the positional stage. An unfortunate example from Fox News.
123
Phonemic Errors occur within phrase boundaries.
- can be between words with different grammatical roles. - tend to share the same position in the word (first phoneme for first phonemes). These errors are at the positional level. They occur in ordering the phonemes.
124
Acquiring Language Even before birth fetuses gain familiarity and knowledge about language. DeCasper, Lecanuet, Busnel, Granier-Deferre and Maugeais (1994) Pregnant women recited a short child’s rhyme, “the target,” aloud each day between the thirty-third and thirty-seventh weeks of their fetuses’ gestation. Then their fetuses were stimulated with tape recordings of the target and a control rhyme. The target elicited a decrease in fetal heart-rate whereas the control did not. Thus, fetuses’ exposure to specific speech sounds can affect their subsequent reactions to those sounds. More generally, the result suggests that third trimester fetuses become familiar with recurrent, maternal speech sounds.
125
By 6 months of age By 6 months of age, infants can recognize their own names and simple words such as “no”.
126
Between 6 months and a year
Can recognize names of familiar objects, foods and body parts.
127
Babbling and Cooing Begins as early as 6 weeks Vowel sounds come first (cooing) By four to 5 months consonants are added (babbling) By 10 months babbling is more complex and word like. By 12 months babbling begins to follow phonological rules
128
Age one to two Toddlers repeat words or sounds they hear you say, like the last word in a sentence. But they often leave off endings or beginnings of words. For example, they may say "daw" for "dog" or "noo-noo's" for "noodles.“ They are beginning to associate words with objects.
129
Age one to two First words refer to concrete and familiar. (name objects, actions, and sensations) such as Mommy, BaBa (bottle), go, hot. Nouns appear before verbs. Naming errors tell us a lot about the schema children are forming Overextensions – use a word to refer to more than adult do (e.g., doggy for all four legged animals). Underextensions – using car to refer to one. car, but not to all cars
130
Age one to two: Pointing at objects and naming them.
Not language yet, but still an important stage! When a toddler points at something, generally the first thing you do is look at what they are pointing at. This pointing is your child initiating joint attention. That is, the child wants you to attend to the same item of interest as they are attending to. Joint attention is a very important communication and social skill.
131
Age one to two The frequency with which infants engage in joint attention is related to their language acquisition, even when general cognition is controlled for (e.g., Morales et al., 2000; Mundy et al., 2007).
132
Vocabulary By age two a child generally has about two to three hundred words, but are not really using language because they are limited to referring to the “here and now”. They Performatives – not words, but are actions the child takes in certain situations. E.g., we say bye-bye when we wave to Dad at the door.
133
True Words Words are used to refer to things that are not currently present. This is the true language, the ability to refer to things that are not currently present! The same one word utterance can mean many things “Baba” can mean, “this is my bottle”, or “I want my bottle” or “I will feed my bottle to the puppy”.
134
By two years they start combining words.
Telegraphic speech – two word combinations that have grammar but leave out functional words such as articles (e.g., the and a) and prepositions ( e.g., by and for). "Mommy bye-bye" or "me milk." The word combinations become more complex and show an understanding of grammar! “Doggie bite Tommy” is not the same as “Tommy bite Doggie”
135
Age Three Begin using full sentences, can form questions, make negative statements and use grammar! Vocabulary grows rapidly! Display understanding of symbolic and abstract language like "now," feelings like "sad," and spatial concepts like "in."
136
Nature vs. Nurture B.F. Skinner and Behaviorists claim it is ALL nurture (i.e., conditioning). Children whose mothers corrected them on word choice and pronunciation actually advanced more slowly than those with mothers who were generally accepting.
137
Nature vs. Nurture Chomsky suggested that children are innately prewired to with knowledge about language and that language acquisition is a maturational process. Argued that children learn language too rapidly for it to be conditioning. There must be something that aids the learning.
138
Chomsky Not just repeating what adults say. Instead they are learning the rules (grammar) of language. Adults do not say things like “I goed to the store” or “I broomed the floor” but children do. They have figured out grammatical rules, and they apply them where adults never would. Not just mimicking adults.
139
Interactionist Approach
Both Nature and Nurture are important. Children seem to be innately predisposed to learn language But the their social interactions with other also play a big role.
140
Child-Directed Speech
Parents direct approximately 300 to 400 utterances an hour to their children. Simplified speech that provides infants cues that aid in their language learning. Aid with phonology, vocabulary, grammar and pragmatics
141
Phonology Separate phrases more distinctly (longer pauses).
Speak more s-l-o-w-l-y. Exaggerated ‘singsong’ intonation. Exaggerated difference between questions, statements and commands. Higher and wider range of pitch.
142
Vocabulary and Semantics
Use of concrete nouns (e.g. train, cat) and dynamic verbs (e.g. give, put). Adopt child’s own words for things (e.g.baba). Frequent use of child’s name and absence of pronouns. Content is often highly redundant Children whose caregivers talk to them more have larger vocabularies.
143
Grammar Repeated sentence frames: ‘That’s a …’
Fewer complex sentence and passives. Omission of past tense and inflections. Use of RECASTINGS: where the child’s vocabulary is put into a new utterance. Framing where a word is repeatedly used in different syntactic contexts e.g. Here’s a big ball! Throw me the ball. Where’s the ball?
144
Grammar Use of EXPANSIONS: where the adult ‘fills out’ the child’s utterance. Use of RECASTINGS: where the child’s vocabulary is put into a new utterance. Framing is where a word is repeated used in different syntactic contexts e.g. Here’s a big ball! Throw me the ball. Where’s the ball?
145
Pragmatics Lots of gesture and body language. Eye Contact and Smiling
Stopping frequently for child to respond. Supportive language – adults encourage conversation
Similar presentations
© 2025 SlidePlayer.com Inc.
All rights reserved.