Visual-Graphic Symbol Acquisition by Pre-School Age Children with Developmental and Language Delays Andrea E. Barton 1, Mary Ann Romski, Ph. D. 2, Rose.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Program Evaluation of Music Therapy: Social Joint Attention Behaviors In Preschool-Aged Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Karen L. Herzel, Brenda.
Advertisements

The Increase in Verbal Operants Following the Implementation of Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Devices with Children on the Autism Spectrum.
Mathematics Grade Level Considerations for High School.
Developmental Profile 3
Merry Christmas and Happy New Year 2007 The Beery- Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration Present by Asst. Prof. Dr. Nuntanee Satiansukpong.
Understanding Intelligence Tests
Early Literacy Domains Distance Learning Events brought to you by the Center for Early Literacy Learning 1.
Assessing Preference for Attention in a Child Diagnosed with Autism Jodi Ogle, Cierra Micke, Kelly Paulson, Carrie Haessly, Kevin Schlichenmeyer, Matt.
Social inclusion of young children with Autistic Spectrum Disorder in Australian early childhood programs Sue Walker and Donna Berthelsen Queensland University.
Assessment Considerations for Young Children with Cleft Palate Introduction CLEFT LIP AND/OR PALATE Cleft lip and/or palate (CLP) is the fourth most common.
PLS-5 Training.
V v Motor Skills and Young Children with Autism Olivia Paradis & Megan MacDonald, PhD Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR COLLEGE OF PUBLIC HEALTH AND.
Ecological Validity in Assessment Ecologically valid assessments are sensitive to – chronological and developmental ages – medical status – physical and.
Chapter 6 Treatment of Language Delays and Disorders in Preschool Children.
Putting Together the Pieces: Meaning Matters in Children’s Plural Comprehension Craig Van Pay, Areanna Lakowske & Jennifer Zapf.
Literacy and the Arts The use of music to teach literacy and music education and their effects on childhood literacy.
Using a combined blocking procedure to teach color discrimination to a child with autism Gladys Williams, Luis Antonio Perez-Gonzalez, & Anna Beatriz Muller.
Early Reading First Year 3 ( ) Testing Battery Karen Erickson, PhD & Hillary Harper, M.S. CCC-SLP.
The Effects of Increased Cognitive Demands on the Written Discourse Ability of Young Adolescents Ashleigh Elaine Zumwalt Eastern Illinois University.
The Effects of Enhanced Milieu Teaching on Children with Down Syndrome
Assessment of Special Education Students
Symbols, Symbols What to use????. Clinical Scenario Why do Speech Pathologists make use of graphic symbols? Graphic symbols may take many forms.
© 2009 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Students with Communication Disorders Chapter 7.
Aligning developmental and educational diagnosis: Principles and implications Andreas Demetriou University of Nicosia, Cyprus Presented at the conference.
Pilot: Customizing a Commercially Available Digital Game to Assess Cognitive Function William C. M. Grenhart, John F. Sprufera, Jason C. Allaire, & Anne.
Communication and Vision WHEN WORLDS COLLIDE 1. When Worlds Collide Pre-symbolic and Symbolic Communication Assessing communication skills of children.
Background Purposes of the Study Methods Amanda Rumpca and Dr. Marie Stadler, Ph.D. CCC-SLP  Communication Sciences and Disorders  University of Wisconsin-Eau.
NHS nd International Conference on Newborn Hearing Screening, Diagnosis, and Intervention Como, Italy May 31, 2002 Progress in Specific Language.
Discussion The Effect of Auditory Sensory Abnormalities on Language Development in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder  Autism Spectrum Disorder.
Phonological awareness and ‘silent-reading’: The benefits of intervention and early intervention in reading for children who have Down syndrome. Kathy.
Indiana’s Early Learning Development Framework
Do jobs identified as high and low preference in a video assessment correspond with high levels of performance on the jobs identified?
Acknowledgments: Data for this study were collected as part of the CIHR Team: GO4KIDDS: Great Outcomes for Kids Impacted by Severe Developmental Disabilities.
+ Asperger’s Syndrome and Assistive Technology. + Introduction This study was done on children with Autism Spectrum Conditions (ASC). Children with ASC.
Rhythmic Performances of Young Children Chet-Yeng Loong, Ph.D.
Presenter: Nadiya Destiny Case Study: Andre.
Strategies that Address the Specific Learning Needs of English Language Learners in Mathematics Presented by ESL Department Pittsburgh Public Schools Dr.
Method Participants Participants were 68 preschoolers, between the ages of 29 and 59 months of age. The sample was comprised of 32 male participants and.
Chapter 6 Cognitive and Learning Characteristics © Taylor & Francis 2015.
Communication and Vision
BDI Training. Agenda General Information about BDI Administering the BDI Scoring the BDI Watch BDI on 2 year old – score protocol.
Communication, Language & Literacy ECE 491 Capstone Assignment Presented by Dena Farah The goal of the Center for Early Literacy Learning (CELL) is to.
One Step at a Time: Presentation 6 LISTENING SKILLS Introduction Initial Screen Skills Checklist Classroom Intervention Lesson Planning Teaching Method.
Communication Autism Spectrum Disorder Augmentative Communication Week 6.
© 2010 Pearson Education, Inc. All Rights Reserved. 1  Two Major Types  Language disorders include formulating and comprehending spoken messages. ▪ Categories:
Tiered Instruction In Pre-K Presented by: Tina McClanahan CMS Pre-k Literacy Facilitator January 2010.
Phonological development in lexically precocious 2-year-olds by Smith, McGregor & Demille Presented by: Marrian B. Bufete.
Introduction The authors of this research would like to thank the University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire’s Office of Research and Sponsored Programs for financial.
Modifications for Students with significant disabilities.
Preschool Language Scale-3 Presented by Sarah Stockton, Andrea Mick, Amy Howard, Manda Clements, Kristina Latta, and Thekla Vei.
Talk To Me With Visuals Aided Language Stimulation.
What Can My ELLs Do? Grade Level Cluster K-2 A Quick Reference Guide for Planning Instructional Tasks for English Language Learners.
Georgia will lead the nation in improving student achievement. 1 Georgia Performance Standards Day 3: Assessment FOR Learning.
Preschool Language Scale 4 th Edition Year published: 2002 Type of test: Norm-referenced.
Learning Target Cycles Chris Coombes
Assessments for Children Birth to 3: Part 1 Minnesota Child Development Inventory Colorado School for the Deaf and the Blind November 6, 2013.
Differential Ability Scales (DAS-II)
Investigating the Use of Video Modeling to Teach the Expressive Use of Personal Pronouns to Children with Autism Katie Lichtblau and Kevin P. Klatt Psychology.
E-LAP TRAINING. Who Can Administer? The Manual does not give guidelines as to who can and can’t administer the E-LAP.
Language Disorders in Children CSD 101 Introduction to Communication Sciences and Disorders.
Early Childhood Mathematics Number Readiness Chapter 6.
Background Purposes of the Study Methods Elayne Hansen and Dr. Marie Stadler, Ph.D. CCC-SLP  Communication Sciences and Disorders  University of Wisconsin-Eau.
Katherine Miller ECSE 500 Language/Communication Intervention Plan.
APPLICATION OF MOTOR LEARNING TO DEVELOPMENTAL APRAXIA OF SPEECH Melissa M. Mueller, B.A. Carlin F. Hageman, Ph.D. Angela N. Burda, Ph.D. Ken M. Bleile,
Effects of Word Concreteness and Spacing on EFL Vocabulary Acquisition 吴翼飞 (南京工业大学,外国语言文学学院,江苏 南京211816) Introduction Vocabulary acquisition is of great.
Late talkers (Delayed Onset)
Reporting Assessment Information (Assessment Report/ITP)
Differentiated Learning
Chapter 3: How Standardized Test….
Perceptual-Motor Deficits in Children with down syndrome: Implications for Intervention Study by: Naznin Virji-Babul, Kimberly Kerns, Eric Zhou, Asha.
Presentation transcript:

Visual-Graphic Symbol Acquisition by Pre-School Age Children with Developmental and Language Delays Andrea E. Barton 1, Mary Ann Romski, Ph. D. 2, Rose A. Sevcik, Ph. D. 3 Department of Psychology Georgia State University Atlanta, GA Center for Research on Atypical Development and Learning at GSU Abstract The process of language acquisition requires an individual to organize and interpret the world through a system of symbols and referents. For children with severe intellectual disabilities and language delays, the ability to link a symbol to its referent may be a difficult task. Issues such as the visual complexity and iconicity of a symbol arise when deciding what symbols to select as a medium to teach language. This study examines the ability of 4 pre-school age children with developmental and language delays to acquire the meanings of Blissymbols and lexigrams. Although there was a range of performance, lexigrams and Blissymbols were learned equally well by the four participants in comprehension and production. Comprehension of the symbol sets is assessed to further our knowledge of the factors influencing augmented language learning by young children. elements chosen from a pool of nine geometric forms. Lexigrams were randomly assigned meaning and appeared white on a black background. Vocabulary Six vocabulary items that the child did not comprehend were selected and paired with one symbol each. Three were paired with their corresponding Blissymbol, and three were paired with a lexigram. The lexigram for each vocabulary item was equated in terms of visual complexity (1, 2, 3, or 4 elements) with each Blissymbol. Phase One A four part representational matching task, and a lexigram and Blissymbol identity matching assessment were done. Phase Two Six vocabulary items not comprehended by the participant were represented as symbols and displayed in a 2X3 array on a computer screen overlaid with a touch-sensitive screen. Figure 1 illustrates a sample of one child’s computer display. When a child touched a symbol on the screen, its corresponding color photograph referent was displayed along with a digitized speech output of the referent’s label. The computer returned to the 2 X 3 array of symbols and randomly relocated them after a 3-second period of display. Each session consisted of an average of 14 experiences (range from 10-26) per symbol, for an average of 90 trials (range from 76 to 129) per session. Eight total sessions were conducted over a period of 4 weeks. Assessing Comprehension and Production of Symbols Comprehension and production of symbol meanings was assessed after 2, 4, 6, and 8 computer sessions. Comprehension of the symbols was measured by asking the participant to match the same 3” X 5” color photograph to the target symbol displayed in an array of three symbols. Four trials per symbol were administered. Discussion Although there was a range of performance, lexigrams and Blissymbols were learned equally well by the four participants in comprehension and production. These findings suggest that regardless of extant representational ability, preschoolers with developmental and language delays were able to establish symbol-referent relationships when given experience with them. The participant’s variable skill in matching and in learning the symbol-referent relationships may have been influenced by the comprehension skill each child brought to the task. The child’s ability to follow the same symbol around the computer screen as it randomly relocated may be an indication of their emergent understanding of the “stands for” relationship of the symbol to its referent. Understanding that symbols have this relationship to their referent is key in the ability to acquire a visual-graphic or spoken lexicon. These findings suggest that when learning to use an augmentative communication system, whether the symbols are iconic or arbitrary may not be critical to the young child’s ability to learn them. The symbol- referent relationship regardless of iconicity must be understood before the child is able to successfully communicate using an augmentative communication system. Limitations and future research. Given the sample size, research should be expanded to a larger, more heterogeneous sample of preschool age participants. Alternate methods of in-session behavior or child-centered assessment should be explored to better facilitate attentiveness to the task. Future research may also examine any differences in the ability to learn the auditory labels alone of the Blissymbols and lexigrams versus their photographic referents to further our understanding of the factors that influence symbol-referent learning in pre-school age children with developmental and language delays. Production skill was assessed using a similar method. The participants were given a 3” X 5” index card of the symbol to match to the 3” X 5” index card of the color photograph. Phase Three Two weeks after completion of Phase Two, each participant played an interactive board game with the investigator to assess retention and elicit production of the six symbols with which they had experience. The game board consisted of the photographic referents as the path from start to finish. The symbols were attached to a board with an arrow for spinning in the center. The participant spun the arrow to see which symbol it landed on, and then removed the symbol from the spinning board and placed it on the appropriate photographic referent on the game board. Results Representational and Identity Matching Tables 2 and 3 present the children’s performance on the representational and identity matching tasks. Acquisition of Symbols vs. Comprehension Skill Table 4 compares the participants’ receptive and expressive language age equivalent scores on the SICD-R to the number of Blissymbols and lexigrams they evidenced knowledge of in assessment. The participant with the lowest receptive language score evidenced only emergent learning, while the participant with the highest receptive language score learned all 6 symbols. The participants’ ability to follow the same symbol around the computer screen as it randomly relocated may be an indication of their emergent understanding of the symbol-referent relationship. Table 5 presents the mean number of periods of symbol tracking each participant performed. Generalization With the exception of P4, participants were unable to perform on the generalization task. Participants Four male preschool children (mean CA = 37.5 months, range 28 to 44 months) with both developmental and language delays were recruited as participants. All participants were ambulatory and could cross the midline to view the entire array of symbols. Table 1 describes each participants’ performance at the onset of the study. Method Stimuli Blissymbols (Archer, 1974) were symbols presented in black on a white background and ranged in degree of iconicity to its referent. For the purposes of this study, only Blissymbols with a 3.5 or above translucency rating were chosen to ensure all Blissymbols used were iconic. Lexigrams (Rumbaugh, 1977) were an arbitrary symbol system that can be composed of one, two, three, or four P= Participant; CA= Chronological Age; PPVT-III= Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, III; Vineland= Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales; SICD-R= Sequenced Inventory of Communication Development, Revised Table 1: Participant’s Descriptive Information Figure 1: Example of Computer Display Participant CA (months) DiagnosesPPVT-III (months) Vineland (months) SICD-R (months) P139 Apraxia, Developmental delay no basal score 21 Receptive 8 Expressive 8 P239 Developmental Delay no basal score 2416 P328 Apraxia, Sensory integration disorder, Developmental delay no basal score P4 44 No formal diagnosis ParticipantLexigramsBlissymbols P P P P Mean ParticipantObject to Object Object to Photograph Object to Line Drawing Photograph to Line Drawing P P P P Mean Table 2: Performance on 4 representational tasks Table 3: Lexigram and Blissymbol Discrimination tasks Table 4: Symbols Emerging or Learned in Comprehension and Production E= Emerging- a score in assessment from 0.50 to 0.75 L= Learned- a score in assessment of 0.75 or greater SICD-R= Sequenced Inventory of Communication Development, Revised ParticipantSICD-R (months)ComprehensionProduction ReceptiveExpressiveBlissymbolsLexigramsBlissymbolsLexigrams P E 1E P E 1E 2L, 1E 2L P E 1E 1L 2E 2E 1L P L 3L 3L 3L Participant Mean Periods of TrackingAssessing Performance P1 Lexigrams15(5)1Emerging in production Blissymbols18(4) P2 Lexigrams 18(5)1Emerging in comprehension 2 Learned in production Blissymbols12(6)2 Learned in production 1Emerging in production P3 Lexigrams 14(5)1 Learned in comprehension and production Blissymbols11(4)2 Emerging in production 1 Emerging in comprehension P4 Lexigrams 12(4)3 Learned in comprehension and production Blissymbols12(6)3 Learned in comprehension and production Table 5: Mean number of Periods of Tracking Symbol of Intent* *2 or more activations of the same symbol consecutively; ()= maximum number of consecutive activations in one period This research was supported in part by NIH DC to Dr. Rose A. Sevcik 1 Work done at Georgia State University, now at the University of Kansas, 2 Georgia State University-Department of Communication, 3 Georgia State University-Department of Psychology *Do not quote without author’s permission*