The Care and Feeding of Loudness Models J. D. (jj) Johnston Chief Scientist Neural Audio Kirkland, Washington, USA.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
SOUND PRESSURE, POWER AND LOUDNESS MUSICAL ACOUSTICS Science of Sound Chapter 6.
Advertisements

Why do we hear what we hear? James D. Johnston Chief Scientist DTS, Inc.
A primer by J. D. Johnston Microsoft Corporation
Why do we hear what we hear? James D. Johnston Chief Scientist, DTS, Inc.
Copyright © 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter 29 Multiple Regression.
DFT/FFT and Wavelets ● Additive Synthesis demonstration (wave addition) ● Standard Definitions ● Computing the DFT and FFT ● Sine and cosine wave multiplication.
CS 551/651: Structure of Spoken Language Lecture 11: Overview of Sound Perception, Part II John-Paul Hosom Fall 2010.
Identifying Frequencies. Terms: LoudnessPitchTimbre.
Digital Power Measurements Demystified Presented by Sunrise Telecom Broadband … a step ahead.
TRANSMISSION FUNDAMENTALS Review
SIMS-201 Characteristics of Audio Signals Sampling of Audio Signals Introduction to Audio Information.
Physics of Sounds Overview Properties of vibrating systems Free and forced vibrations Resonance and frequency response Sound waves in air Frequency, wavelength,
Structure of human ear Understanding the processes of human auditory system are key for posting requirements for architectural acoustics. This gives us.
IT-101 Section 001 Lecture #8 Introduction to Information Technology.
3/23/2002 Copyright James D. Johnston Permission granted for any educational use. 1 What can we hear? James D. Johnston home.comcast.net/~retired_old_jj.
SUBJECTIVE ATTRIBUTES OF SOUND Acoustics of Concert Halls and Rooms Science of Sound, Chapters 5,6,7 Loudness, Timbre.
Signal processing and Audio storage Equalization Effect processors Recording and playback.
EE2F1 Speech & Audio Technology Sept. 26, 2002 SLIDE 1 THE UNIVERSITY OF BIRMINGHAM ELECTRONIC, ELECTRICAL & COMPUTER ENGINEERING Digital Systems & Vision.
Equalization Changing the curve. What is an EQ? An Equalizer –Is generally a frequency-specific amplifier –Is made up of filters (passive or active) –Is.
Measurement of Sound Decibel Notation Types of Sounds
1 Live Sound Reinforcement Audio measurements. 2 Live Sound Reinforcement One of the most common terms you will come across when handling any type of.
© 2010 Pearson Education, Inc. Conceptual Physics 11 th Edition Chapter 21: MUSICAL SOUNDS Noise and Music Musical Sounds Pitch Sound Intensity and Loudness.
Harmonics, Timbre & The Frequency Domain
© Curriculum Foundation1 Section 2 The nature of the assessment task Section 2 The nature of the assessment task There are three key questions: What are.
Random Processes and LSI Systems What happedns when a random signal is processed by an LSI system? This is illustrated below, where x(n) and y(n) are random.
1 Live Sound Reinforcement Equalizers and other signal processing equipment.
IE341: Human Factors Engineering Prof. Mohamed Zaki Ramadan Lecture 6 – Auditory Displays.
Computing with Digital Media: A Study of Humans and Technology Mark Guzdial, School of Interactive Computing.
Wireless and Mobile Computing Transmission Fundamentals Lecture 2.
Sound. Sound Waves travel as compressions & expansions Alternating regions of compressed and expanded air These regions move away from source as longitudinal.
Dynamic Range and Dynamic Range Processors
Lecture 9 Fourier Transforms Remember homework 1 for submission 31/10/08 Remember Phils Problems and your notes.
Dither and Noise Shaping
1 Loudness and Pitch Be sure to complete the loudness and pitch interactive tutorial at … chophysics/pitch/loudnesspitch.html.
David Meredith Aalborg University
Chapter 7: Loudness and Pitch. Loudness (1) Auditory Sensitivity: Minimum audible pressure (MAP) and Minimum audible field (MAF) Equal loudness contours.
Copyright (c) 2006 by James D. Johnston. Permission granted for any educational use. Audio vs. Video What’s the difference? J.D. Johnston With contributions.
Operational Definitions In our last class, we discussed (a) what it means to quantify psychological variables and (b) the different scales of measurement.
The Physics Of Sound Why do we hear what we hear?
Sound
Time series Model assessment. Tourist arrivals to NZ Period is quarterly.
1 RF (Radio Frequency) technology BASIC TELECOMMUNICATIONS.
SOUND PRESSURE, POWER AND LOUDNESS MUSICAL ACOUSTICS Science of Sound Chapter 6.
Hearing: Physiology and Psychoacoustics 9. The Function of Hearing The basics Nature of sound Anatomy and physiology of the auditory system How we perceive.
Encoding and Simple Manipulation
Loudness level (phon) An equal-loudness contour is a measure of sound pressure (dB SPL), over the frequency spectrum, for which a listener perceives a.
EQUALIZATION E.Q.. What is equalization? The manipulation of tone by increasing or decreasing frequency ranges with tone controls, filters or equalizers.
Pitch Perception Or, what happens to the sound from the air outside your head to your brain….
Adding and Subtracting Decimals © Math As A Second Language All Rights Reserved next #8 Taking the Fear out of Math 8.25 – 3.5.
Introduction to psycho-acoustics: Some basic auditory attributes For audio demonstrations, click on any loudspeaker icons you see....
Sinyal, Power, Spectrum. A graph of the intensity plotted against the frequency (showing the amount of each color) is the frequency spectrum of the light.
CS Spring 2014 CS 414 – Multimedia Systems Design Lecture 3 – Digital Audio Representation Klara Nahrstedt Spring 2014.
The Physics Of Sound Why do we hear what we hear? (Turn on your speakers)
Signal Analyzers. Introduction In the first 14 chapters we discussed measurement techniques in the time domain, that is, measurement of parameters that.
Copyright © Cengage Learning. All rights reserved. 5 Joint Probability Distributions and Random Samples.
Regression Analysis: A statistical procedure used to find relations among a set of variables B. Klinkenberg G
SOUND PRESSURE, POWER AND LOUDNESS
The accuracy of averages We learned how to make inference from the sample to the population: Counting the percentages. Here we begin to learn how to make.
ARCHITECTURAL ACOUSTICS
Cohesion : A SENSE OF FLOW Coherence : A SENSE OF THE WHOLE 王詳勛 & 張鴻翌.
Physics Mrs. Dimler SOUND.  Every sound wave begins with a vibrating object, such as the vibrating prong of a tuning fork. Tuning fork and air molecules.
Measurement Error In practice, if the same thing is measured several times, each result is thrown off by chance error, and the error changes from measurement.
The Care and Feeding of Loudness Models J. D. (jj) Johnston Neural Audio Kirkland, Washington, USA.
Loudness level (phon) An equal-loudness contour is a measure of sound pressure (dB SPL), over the frequency spectrum, for which a listener perceives a.
Loudness level (phon) An equal-loudness contour is a measure of sound pressure (dB SPL), over the frequency spectrum, for which a listener perceives a.
A primer by J. D. Johnston Microsoft Corporation
Lecture 2: Frequency & Time Domains presented by David Shires
Speech Perception (acoustic cues)
A primer by J. D. Johnston Microsoft Corporation
EE Audio Signals and Systems
Presentation transcript:

The Care and Feeding of Loudness Models J. D. (jj) Johnston Chief Scientist Neural Audio Kirkland, Washington, USA

What’s the big deal? (why do I yammer on about loudness vs. intensity, anyhow?) Language exists to communicate. If we don’t use the same meanings to the words (leaving out philosophical issues for now), communication is difficult. Hence, a few definitions and words to help us in the discussions. This understanding will also help explain some of the reasons why different compression sounds so different.

Intensity – What is it? Intensity is the EXTERNAL MEASURED LEVEL OF A SOUND. There are many measures of intensity. For now, we will use the common psychoacoustic (not mechanical engineering) definition of SPL, or “sound pressure level”. – There are assumptions built into SPL. Let’s keep it simple for now and forget them. – SPL, usually measured in dB, is a measure of acoustic energy in the atmosphere.

What is Loudness? Loudness is the INTERNAL, SUBJECTIVE EXPERIENCE OF HOW LOUD A SIGNAL IS. The term loudness dates back at least to Fletcher, if not beyond. Loudness is not intensity Loudness and intensity can be mostly related by a complex calculation, however: – Every listener is a bit different – Hearing injuries affect loudness in many ways – Intensity does NOT equal loudness. The relationship is complex, and could constitute an entire tutorial in and of itself. – In the worst cases, intensity is a very poor substitute for loudness, and vice versa.

When do we use Intensity? Intensity is an OBJECTIVE measure. It measures the actual fluctuations in air pressure in the atmosphere that constitute sound. – We use intensity when that is what we need to know. – We do NOT use intensity when we want to know how loud a sound is to a listener.

When do we use Loudness When we are trying to describe the experience that the listener has. When we are trying to estimate psychoacoustic parameters. When we want to know why somebody is shouting “turn that **** thing down!” and the level (intensity) isn’t that high. When we want to know why somebody is shouting “TURN UP THE SOUND” when the intensity is already excessive. When we want to match loudness, NOT level, across audio selections, either full-bandwidth or from remotes/phones.

What is Loudness? Loudness is the perceived strength of an audio signal. Loudness is something that works differently inside of a critical bandwidth and outside of a critical bandwidth Loudness differences between frequencies depends substantially on the presentation intensity at both low and high frequencies.

Everyone’s Seen This. But what does it mean?

Equal Loudness Curves Show loudness equivalent between tones presented sequentially Doesn’t show what happens when you have multiple frequencies present at once Doesn’t account for growth of sensation, that’s a different experiment

What now? To make a very long story short, we know some things. – Inside of a critical bandwidth (or ERB), loudness grows as the 1/3.5 power of the power present in that band. – When energy is spread over critical bandwidths, the loudness ADDS across frequency outside of a critical bandwidth, but is compressed inside each.

Implications? Single band weighting filters can’t get it right. – They can get it moderately right for wideband signals with similar spectrum, where spectrum is smoothed on a critical band basis. This means that for some “typical signals” they aren’t too far off. There’s no mention of time here yet. – Loudness is sensed across frequency at a given time. That’s called “partial loudness” Getting that far is easy. NOW What?

The time domain While a variety of experiments have shown that the sum of partial loudnesses is a good measure of total loudness for a given instant, there is a lack of work on what it means when either partial or total loudness varies over time. And, that is where we are today.

Some other issues In order to get it right, you have to know the intensity at the playback site (i.e. volume control setting, efficiency, acoustics, etc) – Good luck with that. Distortion, especially in the upper (70-120Hz) region can throw off loudness measurement by a phenominal amount.

Implications? Single band weighting filters can’t get it right. – They can get it moderately right for wideband signals with similar spectrum, where spectrum is smoothed on a critical band basis. This means that for some “typical signals” they aren’t too far off. There’s no mention of time here yet. – Loudness is sensed across frequency at a given time. That’s called “partial loudness” Getting that far is easy. NOW What?

The time domain While a variety of experiments have shown that the sum of partial loudnesses is a good measure of total loudness for a given instant, there is a lack of work on what it means when either partial or total loudness varies over time. And, that is where we are today.

Some issues In order to get it right, you have to know the intensity at the playback site (i.e. volume control setting, efficiency, acoustics, etc) – Good luck with that. – This creates particularly difficult issues below 500Hz. Distortion, especially in the upper (70-120Hz) region can throw off loudness measurement by a phenominal amount.

Bass distortion Consider a 90 Hz sine wave. – Harmonics at 180 and 270 – Each of those harmonics is separated by a critical bandwidth. – Remember compression? If the teensy woofer has 20dB SNR (that would be a very good woofer), the total loudness would scale to something like – 1^1/4 +.01^1/4 +.01^1/4 = 1.63

The point? Overall, loudness models for extended periods are still in development. – We don’t know if loudness or annoyance, or something else, is what people adjust volume controls for – We don’t know if it’s peak or average, or some of both – We don’t know if everyone responds in the same fashion

Maybe they don’t all act the same In a very small (and therefore quite inconclusive) test, we found: – Some people seemed to set a level balance based on something like average – Others tended more toward equalizing peaks. This experiment compared a signal to a “homogenized” signal that had exactly the same average spectrum. The sample size is very small (4-8 subjects), and it can only be taken with an extreme grain of salt at this time. None the less, responses were quite bimodal, not “random”, to at least some extent.

What Else? We don’t know how well people agree on long term vs short term preferences – Some people seem to care about peak – Some care about something kinda-sorta like average We need a system that can be adapted at the point of playback, NOT at the source. – Then, just maybe, we might get some dynamic range back

Bass distortion Consider a 90 Hz sine wave. – Harmonics at 180 and 270 – Each of those harmonics is separated by a critical bandwidth. – Remember compression? If the woofer has 20dB SNR (that would be a very good woofer), the total loudness would scale to something like – 1^1/4 +.01^1/4 +.01^1/4 = 1.63

The point? Overall, loudness models for extended periods are still in development. – We don’t know if loudness or annoyance, or something else, is what people adjust volume controls for – We don’t know if it’s peak or average, or some of both – We don’t know if everyone responds in the same fashion

What Else? We don’t know how well people agree on long term vs short term preferences – Some people seem to care about peak – Some care about something kinda-sorta like average We need a system that can be adapted at the point of playback, NOT at the source. – Then, just maybe, we might get some dynamic range back