Thinking about Probabilities CCC8001. Assignment Watch episode 1 of season 1 of “Ancient Aliens.”

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Cognitive Biases 4.
Advertisements

Thinking about Probabilities CCC8001. Assignment Watch episode 1 of season 1 of Ancient Aliens.
Probability and Induction
Authority 2. HW 8: AGAIN HW 8 I wanted to bring up a couple of issues from grading HW 8. Even people who got problem #1 exactly right didn’t think about.
Formal Probability Theory
MAT 1000 Mathematics in Today's World. Last Time 1.What does a sample tell us about the population? 2.Practical problems in sample surveys.
Probability Distributions CSLU 2850.Lo1 Spring 2008 Cameron McInally Fordham University May contain work from the Creative Commons.
1 Intuitive Irrationality: Reasons for Unreason. 2 Epistemology Branch of philosophy focused on how people acquire knowledge about the world Descriptive.
Risk Thomas Lumley Department of Statistics University of Auckland.
Hypothesis Testing A hypothesis is a claim or statement about a property of a population (in our case, about the mean or a proportion of the population)
Sample Size.
Misconceptions and Fallacies Concerning Probability Assessments.
Intro The Idea of Probability Myths about Randomness
When Intuition Differs from Relative Frequency
Cognitive Biases 2 Incomplete and Unrepresentative Data.
Testing Hypotheses About Proportions Chapter 20. Hypotheses Hypotheses are working models that we adopt temporarily. Our starting hypothesis is called.
Extraordinary Claims. “A wise man… proportions his belief to the evidence.” – David Hume.
Chapter 5 Understanding Randomness
Statistics Introduction.
Processing physical evidence discovering, recognizing and examining it; collecting, recording and identifying it; packaging, conveying and storing it;
Thinking, Deciding and Problem Solving
Judgment in Managerial Decision Making 8e Chapter 3 Common Biases
Stat 321 – Day 11 Review. Announcements Exam Thursday  Review sheet on web  Review problems and solutions on web  Covering chapters 1, 2; HW 1-3; Lab.
Heuristics and Biases. Normative Model Bayes rule tells you how you should reason with probabilities – it is a normative model But do people reason like.
Reasoning with Uncertainty. Often, we want to reason from observable information to unobservable information We want to calculate how our prior beliefs.
Inductive Reasoning Bayes Rule. Urn problem (1) A B A die throw determines from which urn to select balls. For outcomes 1,2, and 3, balls are picked from.
ETHICS BOWL CONSEQUENTIALism.
Heuristics & Biases. Bayes Rule Prior Beliefs Evidence Posterior Probability.
Decision Making. Test Yourself: Decision Making and the Availability Heuristic 1) Which is a more likely cause of death in the United States: being killed.
Statistical Analysis – Chapter 4 Normal Distribution
Cognitive Biases I. Optional Reading Today’s lecture is based primarily on: “How We Know What Isn’t So,” Chapter 1. By Thomas Gilovich, a psychologist.
The Marriage Problem Finding an Optimal Stopping Procedure.
The Man Born Blind (John 9) Now as Jesus passed by, He saw a man who was blind from birth. And His disciples asked Him, saying, “Rabbi, who sinned, this.
Key Stone Problem… Key Stone Problem… next Set 22 © 2007 Herbert I. Gross.
Cognitive Biases 4 Fallacies Involving Probability.
Confirmation Bias. Critical Thinking Among our critical thinking questions were: Does the evidence really support the claim? Is there other evidence that.
Grade 7 Source Analysis The Fight!. Are all primary sources reliable? What are the good points and bad points of primary sources. What is bias– could.
Exam 1 Median: 74 Quartiles: 68, 84 Interquartile range: 16 Mean: 74.9 Standard deviation: 12.5 z = -1: 62.4 z = -1: 87.4 z = -1z = +1 Worst Question:
Chapter 8 Measuring Chapter 81. Measurement We measure a property of a person or thing when we assign a number to represent the property. We often use.
Understanding Probability and Long-Term Expectations Example from Student - Kalyani Thampi I have an example of "chance" that I thought about mentioning.
Extending the Definition of Exponents © Math As A Second Language All Rights Reserved next #10 Taking the Fear out of Math 2 -8.
Lesson  Rebecca and Tova have math class right after lunch.  Rebecca always eats a hot lunch on days when she has an exam, because she has a theory.
+ The Practice of Statistics, 4 th edition – For AP* STARNES, YATES, MOORE Chapter 6: Random Variables Section 6.3 Day 1 Binomial and Geometric Random.
Lecture 15 – Decision making 1 Decision making occurs when you have several alternatives and you choose among them. There are two characteristics of good.
Hypothesis Testing. The 2 nd type of formal statistical inference Our goal is to assess the evidence provided by data from a sample about some claim concerning.
Rick Snodgrass Pilate John 18:28-40, 19:1-16, Romans 5:6-8.
LESSON TWO ECONOMIC RATIONALITY Subtopic 10 – Statistical Reasoning Created by The North Carolina School of Science and Math forThe North Carolina School.
Copyright © 2006 Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Pearson Addison-Wesley Slide
Chapter 21: More About Tests
Welcome to MM570 Psychological Statistics
What happened here!?!?!?! What happened here!?!?!?! What is your Claim, Warrant, and Evidence?
Exercise 2-6: Ecological fallacy. Exercise 2-7: Regression artefact: Lord’s paradox.
Sight Words.
Week 6. Statistics etc. GRS LX 865 Topics in Linguistics.
Representativeness Heuristic Then: Framing Effects Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 6/2 /2015: Lecture 10-2 This Powerpoint.
Adding and Subtracting Decimals © Math As A Second Language All Rights Reserved next #8 Taking the Fear out of Math 8.25 – 3.5.
Cognitive Biases 4 Fallacies Involving Probability.
Journal 9/8/15 Is there anything in your life that you are 100% certain about? Anything you know for sure? Objective Tonight’s Homework To learn about.
Notes for the Exam.  is just a fancy way of saying that you're stating your main point.  A claim tells what you think is true about a topic based on.
The Law of Averages. What does the law of average say? We know that, from the definition of probability, in the long run the frequency of some event will.
A. Judgment Heuristics Definition: Rule of thumb; quick decision guide When are heuristics used? - When making intuitive judgments about relative likelihoods.
Heuristics and Biases Thomas R. Stewart, Ph.D. Center for Policy Research Rockefeller College of Public Affairs and Policy University at Albany State University.
The Representativeness Heuristic then: Risk Attitude and Framing Effects Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 6/1/2016: Lecture.
Exercise 2-7: Regression artefact: Lord’s paradox
Section Testing a Proportion
How small probabilities affect our life?
Thomas Lumley Department of Statistics University of Auckland
Thomas Lumley Department of Statistics University of Auckland
Midterm Discussion.
HEURISTICS.
Presentation transcript:

Thinking about Probabilities CCC8001

Assignment Watch episode 1 of season 1 of “Ancient Aliens.”

Assignment Find one thing that is said, shown, or presented in the episode that is misleading. I want you to describe it to me, then to explain why it is misleading.

Assignment What you describe to me can be misleading for any reason, not just a reason we’ve talked about in class. Just describe it, and tell me the reason why it is misleading.

PROBABILITY BIASES

Conjunction Fallacy Which of the following is most likely to happen? a. There will not be a final exam in this class. b. There will not be a final exam in this class, because the instructor has to leave the country. c. Lingnan University closes and there will not be a final exam in this class.

The Killer Ah Jong is a martial arts expert. He’s a high ranking member of the Triads, and he’s killed hundreds of people. Friends describe him as “dangerous.”

The Killer

What Is Most Likely? (a) Ah Jong sews women’s dresses. (b) Ah Jong sews women’s dresses so the police won’t think he’s a gangster (c) Ah Jong sews women’s dresses as part of his court-ordered rehabilitation.

Conjunction Fallacy Linda is 31 years old, single, outspoken, and very bright. She majored in philosophy. As a student, she was deeply concerned with issues of discrimination and social justice, and also participated in anti-nuclear demonstrations. Which is more probable? a. Linda is a bank teller. b. Linda is a bank teller and is active in the feminist movement.

Conjunction Fallacy The correct answer is (a): a. There will not be a final exam. a. Ah Jong sews women’s dresses. a. Linda is a bank teller.

Conjunction Fallacy Suppose: 20,000 people meet the description (“31 years old, single, outspoken, and very bright. She majored in philosophy…”). How many of them are feminist bank tellers? Pick any number: 5,000.

Conjunction Fallacy How many of them are bank tellers (feminist or not feminist)? It has to be more than 5,000. That’s the number of feminist bank tellers. You have to add in the number of non-feminist bank tellers who meet the description. Let’s say that’s only 1 person.

Conjunction Fallacy Then the probability that Linda is a bank teller AND is active in the feminist movement is 5,000 out of 20,000 or 25%. And the probability that she is a bank teller [maybe a feminist, maybe not] is 5,001 out of 20,000 > 25%.

Mathematics Always, the probability of two events happening (Linda being a bank teller AND Linda being a feminist) is less than the probability of just one of those events happening (for example, Linda being a bank teller). The illusion that the opposite is true especially occurs in cases where one event explains the other.

George For example, suppose I tell you that there is a man named “George.” George turned water into wine, healed the sick, brought a dead person back to life, and came back to life himself after he died. What is the probability that George did all these things? How likely is it?

George You can say whatever you like. 1%, 10%, 99%. But suppose I add to the story. I say “George was the son of God. That’s why he had all these powers.”

George Many people will say that it’s more likely that George was the son of God AND did all these things than it is that he did all these things. But that can’t be true. “A & B” is always less (or equally) probable than A, or than B. For A & B to happen, A has to happen and also B has to happen.

Debiasing We can avoid this bias if we ask the question differently: There are 100 persons who fit the description above (that is, Linda’s). How many of them are: Bank tellers? ____ of 100 Bank tellers and active in the feminist movement? ____ of 100

Frequencies This shows that it’s good to translate percentages and probabilities into frequencies (number of X out of number of Y). We are less susceptible to representativeness bias when things are phrased in this way.

REPRESENTATIVENESS

Representativeness Our (false) judgment that Linda is more likely to be a feminist bank teller than to just be a bank teller is an example of how we judge the truth of claims based on how “representative” they are.

Representativeness Consider again our case of coin flips that seem non-random, due to clustering. Since coins land 50% heads and 50% tails, “XO” and “OX” are representative of this even split, whereas “XX” and “OO” don’t represent it. So sequences with clustering seem non-random, even if they are (random).

Representativeness Representativeness influences our other judgments as well. It’s hard to accept that two very tall parents tend to, on average, have less tall children (as regression to the mean requires). Children who are as tall as their parents are more representative of their parents’ heights.

Heuristics and Biases Representativeness is often a good heuristic. A heuristic is a strategy that is easy to use in problem solving but doesn’t always work when applied. There is often no good reason to distinguish between heuristics and biases.

Representativeness is a good heuristic (sometimes) because (sometimes) things are representative.

THE BASE RATE NEGLECT FALLACY

Base Rate Fallacy There are ½ million people in Russia are affected by HIV/ AIDS. There are 150 million people in Russia.

Base Rate Fallacy Imagine that the government decides this is bad and that they should test everyone for HIV/ AIDS.

They develop a test with the following features: If someone has HIV/ AIDS, then 95% of the time the test will be positive (correct), and only 5% of the time will it be negative (incorrect). If someone does not have HIV/ AIDS, then 95% of the time the test will be negative (correct), and only 5% of the time will it be positive (incorrect).

The Test If someone has HIV/ AIDS, then : 95% of the time the test will be positive (correct) 5% of the time will it be negative (incorrect)

The Test If someone does not have HIV/ AIDS, then: 95% of the time the test will be negative (correct) 5% of the time will it be positive (incorrect)

You Get Tested Suppose you are a Russian who gets tested for HIV/ AIDS under the government program. The test comes out positive. How likely are you to have HIV/ AIDS?

Test = YesTest = No HIV/AIDS = Yes True Positives False Negatives HIV/ AIDS = No False Positives True Negatives

True Positives We know that there are 500,000 people in Russia who have HIV/AIDS. How many will get a positive test result?

The Test If someone has HIV/ AIDS, then : 95% of the time the test will be positive (correct) 5% of the time will it be negative (incorrect)

True Positives 500,000 x 95% = 475,000

Test = YesTest = No HIV/AIDS = Yes True Positives False Negatives HIV/ AIDS = No False Positives True Negatives

Test = YesTest = No HIV/AIDS = Yes 475,000False Negatives HIV/ AIDS = No False Positives True Negatives

False Negatives? How many people who have HIV/AIDS will test negative? 500,000 – 475,000 = 25, ,000 x 5% = 25,000

Test = YesTest = No HIV/AIDS = Yes 475,000False Negatives HIV/ AIDS = No False Positives True Negatives

Test = YesTest = No HIV/AIDS = Yes 475,00025,000 HIV/ AIDS = No False Positives True Negatives

We also know that there are 150 million – 500,000 people in Russia who do not have HIV/AIDS. How many of them will correctly test negative?

The Test If someone does not have HIV/ AIDS, then: 95% of the time the test will be negative (correct) 5% of the time will it be positive (incorrect)

True Negatives (150,000,000 – 500,000) x 95% = ?

True Negatives 149,500,000 x 95% = ?

True Negatives 149,500,000 x 95% = 142,025,000

Test = YesTest = No HIV/AIDS = Yes 475,00025,000 HIV/ AIDS = No False Negatives True Negatives

Test = YesTest = No HIV/AIDS = Yes 475,00025,000 HIV/ AIDS = No False Negatives 142,025,000

False Positives How many people who do not have HIV/AIDS will falsely test positive for the disease? 149,500,000 – 142,025,000 = 7,475, ,500,000 x 5% = 7,475,000

Test = YesTest = No HIV/AIDS = Yes 475,00025,000 HIV/ AIDS = No False Positives 142,025,000

Test = YesTest = No HIV/AIDS = Yes 475,00025,000 HIV/ AIDS = No 7,475,000142,025,000

Test = YesTest = No HIV/AIDS = Yes 475,00025,000 HIV/ AIDS = No 7,475,000142,025,000

You tested positive. What percentage of people who tested positive truly had HIV/AIDS?

Test = YesTest = No HIV/AIDS = Yes 475,00025,000 HIV/ AIDS = No 7,475,000142,025,000 Totals

Test = YesTest = No HIV/AIDS = Yes 475,00025,000 HIV/ AIDS = No 7,475,000142,025,000 Totals7,950,000

Your Chances True Positives ÷ All Positives = 475,000 ÷ 7,950,000 = 6%

Whether a test is good or worth doing depends not only on how accurate it is (95% true positive, 95% true negative), but also on how prevalent the condition being tested for is. Very rare conditions require very sensitive tests, whereas very prevalent conditions only need minorly accurate tests.

Base Rates The “base rate” is the percentage of people in the population who have a certain property. The base rate of HIV/AIDS cases is the percentage of people who have HIV/AIDS in the population. The base rate of bearded people is the percentage of people who have beards in the population, etc.

Base Rate Neglect The “base rate neglect fallacy” is the fallacy of ignoring the base rate when making a judgment. For example, if I assumed you were a terrorist, because you tested positive, I would be committing the base rate neglect fallacy. I should assume you’re still probably not a terrorist.

Base Rates As we have seen, base rates matter. If the base rate of a condition is very low (small percentage of AIDS cases), then even very accurate tests (95% true positive, 95% true negative) can be useless. In our example only 6% of people who tested positive for AIDS had the disease!

Base Rate Neglect The base rate neglect fallacy has nothing at all to do with the number of people there are in the world. Nothing. It has to do with the probability of a variable taking on a certain value, for instance, the probability that someone’s height = 1.5m, the probability that someone has AIDS, etc.

Base Rates This is the “base rate” of people who are 1.5m tall, and the “base rate” of people with AIDS. If 1 in 100 people are terrorists, then the rate of terrorists is 1 in 100 and the probability that a randomly selected person is a terrorist is 1 in 100.

Base Rates We call this the base rate, because it is the probability that someone is a terrorist when we don’t know anything else about them. It might be that the base rate of terrorists is 1 in 100, but the rate of terrorists among people who are holding rocket launchers is 1 in 2, and the rate of terrorists among retirees is 1 in 500.

Tests The base rate neglect fallacy happens when we have a test that is meant to detect the value of a variable. For example we might have a test that tells us whether someone has AIDS or not, or whether someone is driving over the speed limit, or whether they are drunk.

As the base rate decreases, the number of true positives decreases.

Base Rate YesNo Test = YesTrue PositiveFalse Positive Test = NoFalse NegativeTrue Negative

Importance of Base Rates Therefore, the proportion of false positives out of total positives increases: False Positive ÷ False Positive + True Positive

Importance of Base Rates And the proportion of true positives out of total positives decreases: True Positive ÷ False Positive + True Positive

What Does It Mean? It means that even very good tests (low false positive rate, low false negative rate) cannot reliably detect conditions with low base rates.

Base Rate Neglect Fallacy The base rate neglect fallacy happens when: 1.There is a low base rate of some condition. 2.We have a test for that condition. 3.Someone tests positive. 4.We assume that means they have the condition, ignoring the unreliability of tests for conditions with low base rates.

Base Rate Neglect Humans have a tendency to ignore base rates.

Base Rate Neglect John is a man wearing outstanding gothic inspired clothing with long black hair who listens to death metal. How likely is it that he is a Christian and how likely is it that he is a Satanist?

It Matters The Hong Kong police stop people 2 million times every year. Only 22,500 of these cases (1%) are found to be offenses. Even if the police have a very low false positive rate it’s clear that the base rate of offenses does not warrant these stops.

Prosecutor’s Fallacy The base rate neglect fallacy is often called the prosecutor’s fallacy, as I shall explain.

Murder! Let’s suppose that there has been a murder. There is almost no evidence to go on except that the police find one hair at the crime scene.

You are the Suspect If someone is the killer, there is a 100% chance that their DNA will match the hair’s DNA. The police have a database that contains the DNA of everyone in Hong Kong. They run the DNA in the hair through their database and discover that you are a match!

Comprehension Question If you have been following along you should be able to answer this question: What is the probability that you are the murderer, given that you are a DNA match for the hair?

Answer If you said 100%, then you have just committed the base rate neglect fallacy. The correct answer is “Much lower, because the base rate of people who committed this murder out of the Hong Kong population as a whole is 1 in 7 million.”

Perfect Conditions for Fallacy Here’s what we have: 1.A low base rate (only 1 person who committed this murder in the world). 2.A test for whether someone is the murderer. 3.You, who’ve tested positive on this test. 4.And the police who think you did it!

Let’s Look at the Numbers We know that if you are the murderer, then there is a 100% chance of a DNA match. But what is the false positive rate? How likely is a randomly selected person will match the DNA?

False Results Here’s a quote from “False result fear over DNA tests,” Nick Paton Walsh, The Guardian: “Researchers had asked the labs to match a series of DNA samples. They knew which ones were from the same person, but found that in over 1 per cent of cases the labs falsely matched samples, or failed to notice a match.”

Let’s assume that half of the cases where “labs falsely matched samples, or failed to notice a match.” were cases where they falsely matched samples. So the probability of a false positive is ½ x 1% = 0.5%, or 5 in 1,000.

Since there are 7 million people in Hong Kong, we expect about 0.5% x 7 million = 35,000 of them to match the hair’s DNA. Actually, it’s 35, , because the true killer is a match, and not by accident.

So we expect that there are 35,001 DNA matches in all of Hong Kong. And only one of them is the murderer. So what is the probability that you are the murderer? 1 in 35,001. That’s way less than 100%.

Important Things to Remember There are three important things to remember: 1.If the test is more accurate (fewer false positives), then it’s more reliable 2.If the base rate is higher, the test is more reliable. 3.If the police have other reasons to suspect you, the test is more reliable.

1. If the test is more reliable… Theoretically, DNA tests only return a false positive about 1 in 3 billion times. In that case, we’d expect only.002 false positives in all of Hong Kong. So your chances of being guilty would be 1 in 1.002, or 99.8%. Still, that’s lower than 100%.

2. If there base rate is higher… Maybe the person who died was stabbed 5,000 times, once each by 5,000 different people. So there are 5,000 murderers. Then with the previous false positive number at 35,000, you have a 5,000 in 40,000 chance of being one of the killers, or 12.5%.

3. If the police have some other reason to suspect you… To figure out your chances of being guilty, we looked at the probability that a randomly selected person from HK would be a DNA match. We were assuming you were randomly selected. But what if you weren’t randomly selected? What if the police tested you because you had a reason to kill the victim?

Reason to Suspect You Then we would have to look at not the probability that a randomly selected person would match, but the probability that a person who had reason to kill the victim would match. Suppose there are 5 people who had reasons to kill the victim, and the killer is one of them.

Much Higher Chance Then your chances are: Let K = you’re the killer and M = you’re a match P(K/ M) = [P(K) x P(M/ K)] ÷ P(M) = [(1/5) x 100%] ÷ P(M) = 0.2 ÷ [( ) ÷ 5] = 97.6%