Disorders of Syntax and Morphology

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Language and Cognition Colombo, June 2011 Day 8 Aphasia: disorders of comprehension.
Advertisements

1 FON 218: Neurolinguistics APHASIA APHASIA Wanda Jakobsen Wanda Jakobsen.
Chapter 13 Language
Types of Aphasia “Classifications are a necessary evil” Antonio Damasio (1998) Ling 411 – 05.
Aphasia A disorder caused by damage to the parts of the brain that control language. It can make it hard to read, or write and to comprehend or produce.
Human Communication.
Jennifer Nazar.  A language disorder produced by brain damage.  Most studies come from those who have aphasias.  Study behaviors associated with the.
Aphasia “Impairment of central language abilities in the speech modality following brain damage.“ In contrast to: peripheral speech problems (dysarthria)
Ch13. Biological Foundations of Language
Speech/Language Function BCS 242 Neuropsychology Fall 2004.
Language Disorders October 12, Types of Disorders Aphasia: acquired disorder of language due to brain damage Dysarthria: disorder of motor apparatus.
“He speaks fine; he doesn’t need speech therapy! What is speech and language? Presented by: D’Anna Nowack M.S. CCC/SLP.
Language and Aphasia CSE 140 etc.. Outline Review the relationships between lesions and linguistic effects Review of the traditional picture about Broca’s.
Introduction to Neuropsychology Language. Example Exam Questions 1. How have neuropsychological investigations informed our current understanding about.
Language and Brain. Is Language Situated in our Brain? Neurolinguistics is the study of how the brain processes language.
APHASIA. What is it?  “Acquired language dysfunction due to neurological injury or disease”  Most common cause is stroke (about 25-40% of stroke patients.
Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity Marcel Adam Just, Vladimir.
Anatomy of language processing Mark Harju. Most components for language processing are located in the left hemisphere Most components for language processing.
Ling 001, Spring 2009 Language and the Brain. Background Remember some things we have seen thus far: –Abilities in grammar are not directly correlated.
1 Chapter 19: Higher mental functions Chris Rorden University of South Carolina Norman J. Arnold School of Public Health Department of Communication Sciences.
Aphasia: Symptoms and Syndromes
Brain Lateralization Left Brain vs Right Brain. Corpus callosum Bridge between left and right hemispheres of the brain.
Language processing What are the components of language, and how do we process them?
Notes: Exam corrections – due on Thursday, November 12 Last Exam Concrete vs Abstract words.
Lateralization & The Split Brain and Cortical Localization of Language.
APHASIA. What is Aphasia? Aphasia is a total or partial loss of the ability to use words.
The Nature of Human Language
Despite adjustments to the Wernicke-Lichtheim model, there remained disorders which could not be explained. Later models (e.g., Heilman’s) have included.
1 Language disorders We can learn a lot by looking at system failure –Which parts are connected to which Examine the relation between listening/speaking.
Speech and Language Test Language.
Psycholinguistics.
University of Split Danica Škara, PhD Office hours: Tuesday, 14:00-15:00h PSYCHOLINGUISTICS AND COGNITIVE ASPECTS.
Aphasia: Symptoms and Syndromes Ling 411 – 04. Simple Functions / Complex Functions A question came to me in class when you mentioned that localization.
Brain and Behavior Chapter 1.
Last Lecture Dichotic Listening Dichotic Listening The corpus callosum & resource allocation The corpus callosum & resource allocation Handedness Handedness.
"One brain, two languages-- educating our bilingual students in the light of Neuroscience“ Dr. Luz Mary Rincon.
Language and the Brain Understanding how language is represented and processed in the brain.
BEKA EDGAR RACHEL FARMER RACHEL MCKELROY SARA RUFFNER Aphasias.
Disorders of Syntax and Morphology Ling 411 – 08 Goodglass 1993: Chapter 6.
CSD 2230 HUMAN COMMUNICATION DISORDERS Topic 6 Language Disorders Adult Disorders Aphasia and Right Hemisphere Injury.
Outline  1. Brain Structure  2. Module theory: Language and brain  3. Aphasia  4. Summary.
ADULT LANGUAGE DISORDERS
Language Loss Language and Psychology. Frontal lobe – Speech Occipital lobe – Vision Cerebellum – Movement Parietal lobe – Touch Pons – Breathing and.
Language By Angela Moss Tanisha Flowers Reginald Alexander.
ADULT LANGUAGE DISORDERS Week 1 Jan 13, Text Book LaPointe, L. L. (2005). Aphasia and Related Neurogenic Language Disorders. 3rd edition, Thieme,
Four lobes of the cerebral cortex FRONTAL LOBE OCCIPITAL LOBE TEMPORAL LOBE PARIETAL LOBE.
PhD MD MBBS Faculty of Medicine Al Maarefa Colleges of Science & Technology Faculty of Medicine Al Maarefa Colleges of Science & Technology Lecture – 12:
1.  What is Speech ?  Speech is complex form of communication in which spoken words convey ideas.  When we speak, first we understand. 2.
Brain Damage and Locations of Linguistic Functions Ling 411 – 07.
Introduction to Linguistics Class #4 Brain and Language (chapter 2)
Types of Aphasia Ling 411 – 05. Simple Functions / Complex Functions: Speaking and Understanding How is simplicity/complexity determined? What about "understanding.
1 Separable Processing of Consonants and Vowels Alfonso Caramazza, Doriana Chialant, Rita Capasso & Gabriele Miceli (Jan. 2000) Nature. Vol 403:
The Brain & Stroke. Frontal Lobe Function Planning & organisation Fine motor sequencing Impulse control Auditory sequencing Problem solving Abstract thought.
Language. The system of spoken or written communication used by a particular country, people, community, etc., typically consisting of words used within.
Language and the brain Introduction to Linguistics.
Welcome Back Pick up a Packet! UAA, Cody Augdahl, 2005.
Ling 001, Spring 2010 Language and the Brain. Background Remember some things we talked about in the first half of the course: –Abilities in grammar are.
COMMON FEATURES OF SPOKEN LANGUAGES Arbitrary link of sound and meaning occurs in absence of referent (displacement) small set of phonemes (/e/, /sh/,..)
CEREBRUM Dr. Jamila EL Medany. Objectives At the end of the lecture, the student should be able to:  List the parts of the cerebral hemisphere (cortex,
A cerebral hemisphere is defined as one of the two regions of the brain that are delineated by the body's median plane.
Cerebral Cortex 2.
1 Cerebrum November 6, 2013 Chapter 13: Dr. Diane M. Jaworski Frontal Temporal Occipita l Parietal.
By: Angela D. Friederici Presented By: Karol Krzywon.
Neurological Basis for Speech and Language
Language: An Overview Language is a brain function
Function of Brain in Communication (Language)
NeuroLinguistics Mam Lubna Umar.
CEREBRUM Dr. Jamila EL Medany.
Study of the neural bases of language development and use
Presentation transcript:

Disorders of Syntax and Morphology Ling 411 – 08 Disorders of Syntax and Morphology Goodglass 1993: Chapter 6

Major Language Areas Supramarginal gyrus (Goldstein’s area) Angular gyrus (Geschwinds’s area) Wernicke’s area Exner’s area Broca’s area Superior parietal lobule

Agrammatism Usually associated with Broca’s aphasia Generally present in Broca’s aphasia But other aphasics also have grammatical dysfunctions Paragrammatism – common in Wernicke aphasia A lot of variation among different patients

Agrammatism vs. Paragrammatism Paragrammatism – too much speech Normal or excessive fluency Use of inappropriate words Neologisms No lack of function words and inflections But not always used appropriately Common in Wernicke’s aphasia Agrammatism – not enough speech Lack of fluency Omission (NOT deletion!) of function words and inflections Common in Broca’s aphasia

Omission vs. Deletion Goodglass (106): Sentences with a deleted main verb (“Joan and I . . . Coffee”) may continue to appear. . . . misuse or deletions of morphology . . . Is he talking about deletion or omission? Deletion implies that it was first there, and then removed Omission – it wasn’t put in at all Goodglass is following a practice that was common among linguists at the time he wrote the book

Broca’s Aphasia Damage to frontal lobe Largely intact comprehension Mainly, inferior frontal gyrus Largely intact comprehension Nonfluent, agrammatic speech “Telegraphic speech” – Abundance of content words (e.g., nouns) Lack of function words (e.g. prepositions) Impaired verb processing Bates, Chen, Tzeng, Li & Opie, 1991; Damasio & Tranel, 1993; Daniele, Giustolisi, Silveri, Colosimo & Gainotti, 1994; Lamb & Zhang, 2010; Shapiro & Caramazza, 2003

Verbal short-term memory deficit (in Broca aphasia) Patients can readily point to individual objects or body parts named by the examiner But when asked to point to the same items in a specific sequence they often fail at the level of only two or three items Benson & Ardila 124 How to explain?

Subtypes of Broca aphasia Type I A.k.a. little Broca aphasia Milder defects Less extensive damage Better prognosis Type II Symptoms worse More extensive damage These are not distinct, but variations Two spans along a scale

Agrammatism: an early observation (1819) Deleuze (1819), referring to a French-speaking patient: The patient “used exclusively the infinitive of verbs and used no pronouns. … She produced absolutely no conjugated verb.” Goodglass 1993: 104

Example of agrammatic speech Examiner: Can you tell me about why you came back to the hospital? Patient: yes … eh … Monday … eh … dad … Peter Hogan and dad … hospital. Er … two … er … doctors … and … er … thirty minutes … and ... er … yes … hospital. And … er … Wednesday … Wednesday. Nine o’clock. And … er … Thursday, ten o’clock … doctors … two … two … doctors… and … er … teeth … fine. E: Not exactly your teeth … your g- P: Gum … gum … E: What did they do to them? P: And er … doctor and girl … and er … and er gum … Goodglass 1993: 107

Some features of agrammatism Telegraphic speech Short utterances Omission of grammatical functors Relative abundance of substantives Verbs are uncommon, rare in some patients When present, uninflected or –ing form For French aphasics, infinitive form Use of word order is generally spared Comprehension is impaired for complex sentences

Problems in the study of agrammatism Must be distinguished from paragrammatism Grammatical aberrations – even among Broca aphasics – vary from patient to patient Linguistics has not (yet) provided clear answers to important basic questions: What normal grammatical functions are How they operate

Syntax First, we need to dispel the notion that syntax is one capacity, that can be lost (or spared) as a unit Syntax can be understood as a set of constructions Learned by children (and others) one by one Like vocabulary Some can be lost, others spared, in aphasia It is a label of the grammarian for multiple things Word order is often spared in Broca’s aphasia while a lot of syntax is lost

Stability of word order in agrammatism Agrammatic patients can usually handle word order in both production and comprehension Evidence (comprehension) passive sentences misconstrued The horse was kicked by the dog Broca’s aphasic: horse as kicker Passive marker not apprehended Canonical word order guides the interpretation Possibly aided by conceptual knowledge

Reading and writing in agrammatism Agrammatic difficulties are also seen in Oral reading Writing to dictation Repetition But: Some patients are agrammatic in speech but not in writing (Goodglass 1993: 110) Some can repeat correctly How to explain? Menn & Obler (1990) describe some patients who are less agrammatic in oral reading than in spontaneous speech (Goodglass 1993:111)

Variation among agrammatics (Goodglass 1993:107) Syntax and morphology (study of agrammatic French aphasics) Some patients have fairly good syntax but defective morphology Some patients have fairly good morphology but defective syntax Both types of patients fail to use inflected verb forms Gleason et al. observations (1975) Some patients use –s plural marker but not articles Other patients use articles but not –s plural marker

Loss of the use of relational markers in receptive agrammatism (118) E.g. father’s sister Ex: Is “my father’s sister” a man or a woman? Patient answers randomly Unable to grasp the relational function of –’s Command given in testing: Ex: Touch the comb with the pencil Patient may touch the pencil with the comb Perhaps picks up comb because the word comb comes first in the instruction Locative relations somewhat less fragile in back of/in front of, over/under, before/after

Linguistic structure in the cortex: What we learn from agrammatism Agrammatism is generally associated with Broca’s aphasia Therefore, the grammatical skills lost in Broca’s aphasia must be supported at least in part by either Broca’s area, or Area(s) adjacent to Broca’s area In other words: There must be something in or near Broca’s area that is essential for correct grammatical production And grammatical comprehension – Receptive agrammatism

Receptive processing in Broca’s aphasia? Problem: Broca’s area is in frontal lobe Frontal lobe is supposed to be for motor production Motor production is top-down processing Receptive functions involve bottom-up processing Comprehension involves sensory processing in frontal lobe? Bottom-up (receptive) processing in frontal lobe?

Receptive agrammatism in Broca’s aphasia Two avenues to explanation The role of short-term memory, and Broca’s area in short-term memory Maybe the frontal lobe can have receptive function To explore this possibility we must first examine the phenomenon of imagery

For perspective, A related problem: Imagery Types of sensory imagery Visual Auditory Somatosensory Cf. Motor imagery

Visual Imagery Visual images of people, buildings, etc. What is a visual image? What does it consist of? Is it a little picture? If so, where are the eyes to see it? What is it drawn on? Where is the visual perception system to interpret it? If not, what?

Auditory Imagery Auditory images of words, music, etc. We can hear things in our heads What is an auditory image? What does it consist of? Sound? There is no air inside the head to vibrate What hears it? Little ears inside the head?

How Imagery Operates It’s unlikely that visual imagery uses some mechanism independent of that for vision Therefore, it must use (some of) the same neural connections used in perception For visual imagery, pathways in the occipital lobe For auditory imagery, pathways in the temporal lobe For tactile imagery, pathways in the parietal lobe Imagery is activation of some of the same neural pathways that get activated upon receiving input from sense organs

Anatomical consequences Consequences of imaging explanation Top-down processing in perceptual areas Perceptual pathways must have parallel pathways of opposite direction Why are imagined scenes less vivid than those resulting from input to the eyes?

Bidirectional Processing Imagery requires top-down processing Using pathways that typically operate bottom-up Therefore, perceptual pathways must generally be bidirectional Anatomical evidence supports the hypothesis Reciprocal pairs of cortico-cortical axons

Bidirectional Connections Most corticocortical connections are bidirectional An established finding from neuroanatomy It’s not because the connecting nerve fibers (axons) are themselves bidirectional It’s because we find different but roughly parallel fibers going in opposite directions

Bidirectional Processing in Frontal Lobe? Frontal lobe processing: typically top-down But there is a large amount of uniformity in cortical structure Hypothesis: Bottom-up processing also in frontal lobe From perceptual (i.e. posterior) areas to locations in frontal lobe We already have seen evidence: the arcuate fasciculus

Bidirectional connections in frontal lobe Would explain how Broca’s area is involved in receptive grammatical processing Would account for the finding that interpretation of prepositions and verbs is a frontal lobe function Finding from the study of agrammatism

Attempts to explain agrammatism Many theories have been proposed Cf. Goodglass 1993:111ff Some intriguing ideas Loss of relational use of words (Jakobson, Luria) Difficulty with markers of such relationships Impairment of inner speech (Luria) Hence, impairment of auditory working memory Difficulty with unstressed words (Goodglass, Kean) Substantive words are commonly stressed Functors are generally unstressed

Caution in interpreting Agrammatism may not be just one phenomenon Syntax is not one structure but several All agrammatics and probably all Broca’s aphasics are deficient in use of verbs Other phenomena of agrammatism show more variability The problem (or part of the problem) may not be grammar as such: Short-term memory – the inner speech loop Phonology: stressed vs. unstressed words

Phonological factors Function words are (in general) unstressed Maybe the difficulty is in production of unstressed words Intriguing finding of Goodglass et al. Function words May be produced after a stressed word But almost never produced initially Production starts with stressed word Even with repetition Open the door > Open the door The door is open > Door is open

More evidence on relational markers (119f) Grammatical particles that do not mark relations are exempt from omission and Japanese clause-final particles Emphatic yo Question marker ka Confirmation-seeking particle ne Verbs always have a syntactic implication I.e. relationship to one or more nouns Menn & Obler: Impairment affecting grammatical elements that mark relationships within the sentence

Nouns and Verbs: Back Brain & Front Brain (?) “A Neurolinguistic Universal” –E. Bates Verb deficit in Broca’s aphasia Noun deficit in Wernicke’s aphasia Suggests that Verbs are represented in frontal lobe Nouns are represented in or near temporal lobe – angular gyrus and/or supramarginal gyrus) and/or middle temporal gyrus Supports what we derive from the proximity principle

Proceed with Caution! We already know that a noun or a verb has a complex cortical representation Therefore it is not in a single location Rather, a functional web So what are we talking about? The cardinal node of the functional web of a noun The cardinal node of the functional web of a verb

A patient with non-fluent aphasia Patient ROX (McCarthy & Warrington 1985) Impaired production and comprehension of verbs Excellent production and comprehension of nouns Had no difficulty imitating common actions But had difficulty in performing same actions in response to verbal commands E.g., confused open and close In action-naming test, sometimes substituted nouns for verbs or omitted verbs: “chairing” for sitting “The man is a sack of potatoes” for a man carrying a sack of potatoes

Verb deficit and agrammatism: Why? Syntactic hypothesis Verbs are by their nature syntactically complex Nouns are not complex – they can stand alone Semantic hypothesis – using proximity Verbs represent processes and processes are managed by the frontal lobe Nouns represent things, and things are known mainly through perception, which is managed by the occipital, temporal, and parietal lobes

Noun-Verb vis-à-vis Speech & Writing (908b) Patient S.J.D. Written naming of verbs defective But oral naming okay Nouns okay for both writing and speaking Patient H.W. Oral naming of verbs defective But written naming okay Comparable results independently of mode of stimulus – picture naming, reading, writing to dictation (More, next slide..)

Inputs: pictures, oral dictation, reading More on H.W. & S.J.D. Noun-Verb vis-à-vis Speech-Writing (Rapp & Caramazza 908-9) Inputs: pictures, oral dictation, reading Tasks: (1) speaking, (2) writing Example: There’s a crack in the mirror (crack as n.) Don’t crack the mirror (crack as v.) S.J.D. crack as n. correctly produced, both modalities crack as v. correct only in spoken modality H.W. – the opposite modality effect Data from Caramazza & Hillis (1991)

Broca’s Area: A closer look

Subdivisions of Broca’s area Broca’s area includes two different (but adjacent) Brodmann areas BA 44 – Pars Opercularis BA 45 – Pars Triangularis (Some people also include the Pars orbitalis, just inferior to the pars triangularis)

Frontal Operculum Operculum: little cover The part of the frontal lobe that covers (part of) the Sylvian fissure and anterior insula Adjacent to and inside the anterior portion of Sylvian fissure Opposite it (across Sylvian fissure) in temporal lobe is the temporal operculum

Subdivisions of Broca’s area Another view

Major Language Areas Supramarginal gyrus (Goldstein’s area) Angular gyrus (Geschwinds’s area) Wernicke’s area Broca’s area Superior parietal lobule Brodmann area 37

Left hemi-sphere, showing middle cerebral artery Frontal Operculum

A closer look at Broca’s aphasia Broca’s original patient Lesion was extensive Not just Broca’s area but also Adjacent areas Subjacent white matter A tradition has followed Broca Broca’s area held responsible for symptoms of Broca’s aphasia Confounding factor: Broca’s area is only part of the area of damage with Broca’s aphasia

More recent findings Modern imaging methods help Some patients have damage restricted mainly or entirely to just Broca’s area They have less severe symptoms that typically associated with Broca’s aphasia Influential paper: Alexander et al. 1990 Examined Broca patients with different areas of damage Cast doubts on importance of Broca’s area in Broca’s aphasia

Broca’s area and Broca aphasia Maybe it’s not just Broca’s area damage that is responsible for some of the symptoms of “Broca’s aphasia” Maybe some of them result instead from damage to neighboring areas Alexander et al. (1990) propose distinguishing 3 subtypes

Three subtypes in Alexander study Impaired speech initiation Symptom traditionally attributed to transcortical motor aphasia Area of damage: frontal operculum Disturbed articulatory function Area of damage: lower primary motor cortex The classical Broca’s aphasia syndrome More extensive damage

To be continued … (Later)

end