Volume 50, Issue 4, Pages (May 2006)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Soyoun Kim, Jaewon Hwang, Daeyeol Lee  Neuron 
Advertisements

Volume 63, Issue 3, Pages (August 2009)
Volume 46, Issue 5, Pages (June 2005)
Takafumi Minamimoto, Richard C. Saunders, Barry J. Richmond  Neuron 
Volume 60, Issue 4, Pages (November 2008)
Elizabeth V. Goldfarb, Marvin M. Chun, Elizabeth A. Phelps  Neuron 
Todd S Braver, Jeremy R Reynolds, David I Donaldson  Neuron 
The Nose Smells What the Eye Sees
Neurodegenerative Diseases Target Large-Scale Human Brain Networks
Volume 95, Issue 1, Pages e3 (July 2017)
Rei Akaishi, Kazumasa Umeda, Asako Nagase, Katsuyuki Sakai  Neuron 
Volume 55, Issue 3, Pages (August 2007)
Medial Prefrontal Cortex Predicts Internally Driven Strategy Shifts
Volume 95, Issue 5, Pages e5 (August 2017)
Volume 44, Issue 6, Pages (December 2004)
Volume 72, Issue 5, Pages (December 2011)
Frontal Cortex and the Discovery of Abstract Action Rules
When more means less Current Biology
Alan N. Hampton, Ralph Adolphs, J. Michael Tyszka, John P. O'Doherty 
Todd S Braver, Jeremy R Reynolds, David I Donaldson  Neuron 
Martin O'Neill, Wolfram Schultz  Neuron 
Volume 63, Issue 3, Pages (August 2009)
Volume 81, Issue 6, Pages (March 2014)
Perceptual Learning and Decision-Making in Human Medial Frontal Cortex
Activity in Both Hippocampus and Perirhinal Cortex Predicts the Memory Strength of Subsequently Remembered Information  Yael Shrager, C. Brock Kirwan,
Volume 53, Issue 6, Pages (March 2007)
Roman F. Loonis, Scott L. Brincat, Evan G. Antzoulatos, Earl K. Miller 
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Selective Entrainment of Theta Oscillations in the Dorsal Stream Causally Enhances Auditory Working Memory Performance  Philippe Albouy, Aurélien Weiss,
Neural Correlates of Visual Working Memory
Parallel Interdigitated Distributed Networks within the Individual Estimated by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity  Rodrigo M. Braga, Randy L. Buckner 
Volume 96, Issue 6, Pages e6 (December 2017)
Between Thoughts and Actions: Motivationally Salient Cues Invigorate Mental Action in the Human Brain  Avi Mendelsohn, Alex Pine, Daniela Schiller  Neuron 
Human Orbitofrontal Cortex Represents a Cognitive Map of State Space
Volume 53, Issue 2, Pages (January 2007)
Resolving Emotional Conflict: A Role for the Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Modulating Activity in the Amygdala  Amit Etkin, Tobias Egner, Daniel.
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
A. Saez, M. Rigotti, S. Ostojic, S. Fusi, C.D. Salzman  Neuron 
Volume 62, Issue 6, Pages (June 2009)
Action Selection and Action Value in Frontal-Striatal Circuits
Volume 81, Issue 5, Pages (March 2014)
Volume 56, Issue 1, Pages (October 2007)
Parietal and Frontal Cortex Encode Stimulus-Specific Mnemonic Representations during Visual Working Memory  Edward F. Ester, Thomas C. Sprague, John T.
The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain
Volume 50, Issue 3, Pages (May 2006)
Neural Mechanisms of Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff
Rei Akaishi, Kazumasa Umeda, Asako Nagase, Katsuyuki Sakai  Neuron 
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Value-Based Modulations in Human Visual Cortex
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Volume 59, Issue 5, Pages (September 2008)
Social Signals in Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex
Visual Feature-Tolerance in the Reading Network
Nicholas E. Bowman, Konrad P. Kording, Jay A. Gottfried  Neuron 
Volume 76, Issue 4, Pages (November 2012)
Peter H. Rudebeck, Andrew R. Mitz, Ravi V. Chacko, Elisabeth A. Murray 
Orienting Attention Based on Long-Term Memory Experience
Arielle Tambini, Nicholas Ketz, Lila Davachi  Neuron 
John T. Serences, Geoffrey M. Boynton  Neuron 
Medial Prefrontal Cortex Predicts Internally Driven Strategy Shifts
Volume 47, Issue 6, Pages (September 2005)
David Badre, Bradley B. Doll, Nicole M. Long, Michael J. Frank  Neuron 
Perceptual Classification in a Rapidly Changing Environment
Caroline A. Montojo, Susan M. Courtney  Neuron 
Resolution of Uncertainty in Prefrontal Cortex
Similarity Breeds Proximity: Pattern Similarity within and across Contexts Is Related to Later Mnemonic Judgments of Temporal Proximity  Youssef Ezzyat,
Common Prefrontal Regions Coactivate with Dissociable Posterior Regions during Controlled Semantic and Phonological Tasks  Brian T Gold, Randy L Buckner 
Volume 61, Issue 6, Pages (March 2009)
Volume 72, Issue 5, Pages (December 2011)
Visual Feature-Tolerance in the Reading Network
Presentation transcript:

Volume 50, Issue 4, Pages 643-653 (May 2006) Anterior Cingulate and Posterior Parietal Cortices Are Sensitive to Dissociable Forms of Conflict in a Task-Switching Paradigm  Conor Liston, Shanna Matalon, Todd A. Hare, Matthew C. Davidson, B.J. Casey  Neuron  Volume 50, Issue 4, Pages 643-653 (May 2006) DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.015 Copyright © 2006 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Task Paradigm The task cue and square-wave stimuli were presented simultaneously for 1500 ms followed by a 500 ms fixation cross. Repeat trials were preceded by two to five trials of the same dimension. Shift trials were preceded by two to five trials of the opposing dimension. Shift trials and repeat trials were followed by a variable intertrial interval of 0.5–12.5 s (mean 5.0 s). Neuron 2006 50, 643-653DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.015) Copyright © 2006 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 High Conflict Trial Types Were Associated with Significant Costs in Task Performance (A) Shift trials were significantly slower [middle panel: F(1,18) = 388.03, p < 0.001] and less accurate than repeat trials [bottom panel: F(1,18) = 6.50, p = 0.02]. Error bars = SEM. (B) Response conflict was associated with impairments in reaction time [middle panel: F(1,18) = 50.1, p < 0.001] and accuracy [bottom panel: F(1,18) = 4.37, p = 0.05], but these effects were driven by shift trials. Whereas high conflict shifts were slower (t = 3.81, p < 0.001) and less accurate (t = 1.95, p = 0.05) than low conflict shifts, the effects of response conflict on repeat trial RT (reaction time) (t = 1.81, p = 0.07) and accuracy (t = 1.27, p = 0.21) did not reach significance. This was reflected in an interaction between response conflict and task switching for reaction time [F(1,18) = 14.79, p < 0.001]. Error bars = SEM. (C) Stimulus conflict was associated with impairments in reaction time [F(2,36) = 36.14, p < 0.001]. There was also an effect of stimulus conflict on accuracy that was specific to shifts but not repeats, as reflected in an interaction between stimulus conflict and task switching [F(2,36) = 9.35, p < 0.001]. Error bars = SEM. Main effects: ∗ p < 0.05, ∗∗∗p < 0.001. Interactions: +p < 0.001. Neuron 2006 50, 643-653DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.015) Copyright © 2006 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Task Switching Engaged a Network of Frontoparietal Structures Shift trials contrasted with repeat trials engaged a network of prefrontal and parietal structures. 3D renderings of areas engaged by the shift-repeat contrast (main panel) are paired with coronal sections (bottom panels). These include bilateral DLPFC (orange), bilateral ACC (yellow), and bilateral posterior parietal cortex (BA 7/40; violet). Neuron 2006 50, 643-653DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.015) Copyright © 2006 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Response Conflict and Stimulus Conflict Engaged Dissociable Networks of Frontoparietal Structures (A) High response conflict shift trials relative to low response conflict shift trials (green regions in [C]) engaged a network of structures, including orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC) and inferior aspects of posterior parietal cortex (PPC). No significant differences were observed in these areas for high versus low response conflict repeat trials. (B) High stimulus conflict shifts relative to low stimulus conflict shifts (red regions in [C]) engaged a distinct network of structures including anterior prefrontal cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and dorsal aspects of posterior parietal cortex. A similar pattern was observed for repeat trials. (C) 3D rendering of regions engaged in the high versus low response conflict contrast (green) and the high versus low stimulus conflict contrast (red). In general, stimulus conflict was associated with increased activity in a network of structures located dorsal to those sensitive to response conflict. Neuron 2006 50, 643-653DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.015) Copyright © 2006 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 5 Activity in ACC and PPC Increased with Increasing Conflict (A) As predicted, the conflict index was significantly higher for shift trials than repeat trials (t = 1.85, p = 0.033, one-tailed). Error bars = SEM. (B) Activity (percent change in BOLD signal from run-average baseline) in anterior cingulate cortex (left) and posterior parietal cortex (right) is plotted against the conflict index. Activity in these regions increased with increasing conflict as indexed by the product of activities in color- and motion-sensitive regions (r = 0.49 [ACC], r = 0.48 [PPC], p < 0.001). ∗p < 0.05, ∗∗∗p < 0.001. Neuron 2006 50, 643-653DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.015) Copyright © 2006 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 6 Activity in Both ACC and Dorsal PPC Predicted Increased Activity in DLPFC and Enhanced Behavioral Performance on Subsequent Trials (A) Activity in ACC (left: p < 0.001) and dorsal PPC (right: p < 0.001) on high conflict trials predicted increased DLPFC activity on the subsequent trial. ACC and dorsal PPC activities are plotted in six quantiles against the means for DLPFC activity (percent change in BOLD signal from run-average baseline). Error bars = SEM. (B) Activity in ACC (left: t = 2.00, p = 0.047) and dorsal PPC (right: t = 2.70, p < 0.007) on the preceding shift trial was significantly higher for high adjustment (fast) trials than for low adjustment (slow) trials. See text for details. Error bars = SEM. ∗p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01, ∗∗∗p < 0.001. Neuron 2006 50, 643-653DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.015) Copyright © 2006 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions