Basics of Experimental Design for fMRI: Block Designs

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
fMRI Methods Lecture6 – Signal & Noise
Advertisements

Artificial Intelligence 12. Two Layer ANNs
Memory and Cognition Lecture 10: Filling in the gaps… ENCODING RETRIEVAL Seeing Word Hearing Word MTL.
When zero is not zero: The problem of ambiguous baseline conditions in fMRI Stark & Squire (2001) By Mike Toulis November 12, 2002.
Chapter 4: The Visual Cortex and Beyond
H.S. Physical Science Chapters 1 and 2
1 st Level Analysis: design matrix, contrasts, GLM Clare Palmer & Misun Kim Methods for Dummies
Detecting Conflict-Related Changes in the ACC Judy Savitskaya 1, Jack Grinband 1,3, Tor Wager 2, Vincent P. Ferrera 3, Joy Hirsch 1,3 1.Program for Imaging.
Section 1 fMRI for Newbies
Data Mining Methodology 1. Why have a Methodology  Don’t want to learn things that aren’t true May not represent any underlying reality ○ Spurious correlation.
Discuss the use of technology in investigating cognitive processes
THE DISTRIBUTION OF SAMPLE MEANS How samples can tell us about populations.
Copyright © 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter 29 Multiple Regression.
Introduction to Functional MRI Last Update: January 14, 2013 Last Course: Psychology 9223, W2013 Jody Culham Brain and Mind.
Statistical Issues in Research Planning and Evaluation
Neural Correlates of Visual Awareness. A Hard Problem Are all organisms conscious?
Designing a behavioral experiment
Signal to noise ratio (SNR) and data quality. Coils Source: Joe Gati Head coil homogenous signal moderate SNR Surface coil highest signal at hotspot high.
Opportunity to Participate EEG studies of vision/hearing/decision making – takes about 2 hours Sign up at – Keep checking.
HST 583 fMRI DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION Neural Signal Processing for Functional Neuroimaging Emery N. Brown Neuroscience Statistics Research Laboratory.
FMRI Basic Experimental Design – block design.. So you want to do an fMRI study? Average cost of performing an fMRI experiment in 1998: Your Salary Average.
Efficiency in Experimental Design Catherine Jones MfD2004.
Writing Workshop Find the relevant literature –Use the review journals as a first approach e.g. Nature Reviews Neuroscience Trends in Neuroscience Trends.
Searching for the NCC We can measure all sorts of neural correlates of these processes…so we can see the neural correlates of consciousness right? So what’s.
Post-test review session Tuesday Nov in TH241.
I. Face Perception II. Visual Imagery. Is Face Recognition Special? Arguments have been made for both functional and neuroanatomical specialization for.
An Inference Procedure
Four Main Approaches Experimental cognitive psychology Cognitive neuropsychology Computational cognitive science Cognitive neuroscience.
Visual Imagery One of the greatest problems confronting psychology is the nature of mental representation. Part of this debate is the nature of representations.
Experiment Design 4: Theoretical + Operational Def’ns Martin Ch. 7.
Basics of Experimental Design for fMRI
1 Psych 5500/6500 The t Test for a Single Group Mean (Part 5): Outliers Fall, 2008.
The Experimental Approach September 15, 2009Introduction to Cognitive Science Lecture 3: The Experimental Approach.
Experimental Design Tali Sharot & Christian Kaul With slides taken from presentations by: Tor Wager Christian Ruff.
Sarah Keedy Aug. 22, 2014 Intro to Functional and Anatomical Brain MRI Research Seminar Series PLANNING MRI RESEARCH TO ADDRESS YOUR SCIENTIFIC QUESTION.
Efficiency – practical Get better fMRI results Dummy-in-chief Joel Winston Design matrix and.
© Curriculum Foundation1 Section 2 The nature of the assessment task Section 2 The nature of the assessment task There are three key questions: What are.
Finish: Overview of the History of Cog Psych Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 04/01 /2015: Lecture 01-3 This Powerpoint presentation.
Memory and Cognition PSY 324 Chapter 2: Cognition and the Brain Part II: Localization of Function Dr. Ellen Campana Arizona State University.
Attention Modulates Responses in the Human Lateral Geniculate Nucleus Nature Neuroscience, 2002, 5(11): Presented by Juan Mo.
Susceptibility Induced Loss of Signal: Comparing PET and fMRI on a Semantic Task Devlin et al. (in press)
Basics of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. How MRI Works Put a person inside a big magnetic field Transmit radio waves into the person –These "energize"
Signal and noise. Tiny signals in lots of noise RestPressing hands Absolute difference % signal difference.
Issues in Experimental Design fMRI Graduate Course October 30, 2002.
FMRI Methods Lecture7 – Review: analyses & statistics.
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging ; What is it and what can it do? Heather Rupp Common Themes in Reproductive Diversity Kinsey Institute Indiana University.
Using PET. We ’ ve seen how PET measures brain activity We ’ ve seen how PET measures brain activity How can we use it to measure the “ mind ” that works.
Basics of Experimental Design for fMRI Last Update: November 2008.
WHAT IS THE NATURE OF SCIENCE?. SCIENTIFIC WORLD VIEW 1.The Universe Is Understandable. 2.The Universe Is a Vast Single System In Which the Basic Rules.
Cognition, Brain and Consciousness: An Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience Edited by Bernard J. Baars and Nicole M. Gage 2007 Academic Press Chapter.
Foundations (cont.) Complexity Testing explanations in psychology Cognitive Neuroscience.
The effects of working memory load on negative priming in an N-back task Ewald Neumann Brain-Inspired Cognitive Systems (BICS) July, 2010.
fMRI Task Design Robert M. Roth, Ph.D.
FMRI Basic Experimental Design – block design.. So you want to do an fMRI study? Average cost of performing an fMRI experiment in 1998: Your Salary Average.
Stats 845 Applied Statistics. This Course will cover: 1.Regression –Non Linear Regression –Multiple Regression 2.Analysis of Variance and Experimental.
Experimental Design Econ 176, Fall Some Terminology Session: A single meeting at which observations are made on a group of subjects. Experiment:
Week 6. Statistics etc. GRS LX 865 Topics in Linguistics.
CSC321 Lecture 5 Applying backpropagation to shape recognition Geoffrey Hinton.
Orienting Attention to Semantic Categories T Cristescu, JT Devlin, AC Nobre Dept. Experimental Psychology and FMRIB Centre, University of Oxford, Oxford,
Basics of Experimental Design for fMRI: Block Designs Last Update: January 18, 2012 Last Course: Psychology 9223, W2010, University.
Copyright © 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter 11 Understanding Randomness.
The Law of Averages. What does the law of average say? We know that, from the definition of probability, in the long run the frequency of some event will.
Chapter 2 Cognitive Neuroscience. Some Questions to Consider What is cognitive neuroscience, and why is it necessary? How is information transmitted from.
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)
Basics of Experimental Design for fMRI
fMRI Basic Experimental Design – block design.
The Components of the Phenomenon of Repetition Suppression
Advances in Event-Related fMRI Design
Signal and Noise in fMRI
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Presentation transcript:

Basics of Experimental Design for fMRI: Block Designs Jody Culham Brain and Mind Institute Department of Psychology University of Western Ontario http://www.fmri4newbies.com/ Basics of Experimental Design for fMRI: Block Designs Last Update: February 4, 2013 Last Course: Psychology 9223, W2013, Western University

Asking the Right Question

“Attending a poster session at a recent meeting, I was reminded of the old adage ‘To the man who has only a hammer, the whole world looks like a nail.’ In this case, however, instead of a hammer we had a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) machine and instead of nails we had a study. Many of the studies summarized in the posters did not seem to be designed to answer questions about the functioning of the brain; neither did they seem to bear on specific questions about the roles of particular brain regions. Rather, they could best be described as ‘exploratory’. People were asked to engage in some task while the activity in their brains was monitored, and this activity was then interpreted post hoc.” -- Stephen M. Kosslyn (1999). If neuroimaging is the answer, what is the question? Phil Trans R Soc Lond B, 354, 1283-1294.

Brains Needed "...the single most critical piece of equipment is still the researcher's own brain. All the equipment in the world will not help us if we do not know how to use it properly, which requires more than just knowing how to operate it. Aristotle would not necessarily have been more profound had he owned a laptop and known how to program. What is badly needed now, with all these scanners whirring away, is an understanding of exactly what we are observing, and seeing, and measuring, and wondering about." -- Endel Tulving, interview in Cognitive Neuroscience (2002, Gazzaniga , Ivry & Mangun, Eds., NY: Norton, p. 323)

Toys Are Not Enough “Expensive equipment doesn’t merit a lousy study.” -- Louis Sokoloff

Localization for Localization’s Sake Danckert et al., 2004, Neuropsychologia Clinical Research presurgical planning understanding functional reorganization Basic Research implicating well-established areas in cognitive tasks

Danger Zone: Reverse Inference “[Mitt Romney’s] still photos prompted a significant amount of activity in the amygdala, indicating voter anxiety…” Iacoboni et al., 2007, New York Times Op-Ed, This is Your Brain on Politics

Danger Zone: Reverse Inference “The Romney amygdala activation might indicate anxiety, or any number of other feelings that are associated with the amygdala -- anger, happiness or even sexual excitement” Martha Farah, Neuroethics and Law Blog

Why You Shouldn’t Use fMRI It’s the most expensive approach If you’re interested in behavior, study behavior EEG/ERP/MEG have better temporal resolution TMS and neuropsychology speak more directly to causality fMRI activation may be epiphenomonal neurophysiology/eCoG give more direct access to neural processing Epiphenomena Huettel et al. fMRI

Behaviorism (circa 1950s) Black Box Stimulus Response

Cognitive Science (circa 1980s) Attention Recognition Stimulus Perception Response Memory Decision Making Too many empty boxes?

Early fMRI (1990s) “Blobology”

The Future of fMRI? Networks Map of semantic space derived from fMRI Gallant Lab

What Can fMRI Add? explicit testing of models derived from other approaches inform and constrain theories of cognition whole brain coverage that can constrain or direct data from other approaches (neurophysiology, ERPs, ECoG) investigation of neural mechanisms of elaborated human functions (language, math, tool use) correlations between brain and behavior help to understand clinical disorders or development look at coding and connections between brain regions

What have we learned about the face area? The face area is activated: when faces are perceived or imagined  correlation between brain and behavior for stimuli at the fovea  cues to brain organization by circular patterns  cues/constraints for modelling in certain areas of the monkey brain  cues to brain evolution for other categories of objects that subjects have extensive experience with  debate regarding nature/nurture to some degree by other categories of objects  debate regarding distributed vs. modular coding in the brain The fusiform face area may be impaired: in some but not all patients who have problems recognizing faces in people with autism  understanding of brain disorders

Finding the Right Experiment

So you want to do an fMRI study? Typical cost of performing an fMRI experiment: $500/hour!!! Average cost of performing a thought experiment: Your Salary CONCLUSION: Unless you are Bill Gates, a thought experiment is much more efficient!

Thought Experiments What do you hope to find? What would that tell you about the cognitive process involved?  Would it add anything to what is already known from other techniques?  Could the same question be asked more easily, more cheaply or better with other techniques? What would be the alternative outcomes (and/or null hypothesis)?  Or is there not really any plausible alternative (in which case the experiment may not be worth doing)?  If the alternative outcome occurred, would the study still be interesting?  If the alternative outcome is not interesting, is the hoped-for outcome likely enough to justify the attempt?  What would the “headline” be if it worked? Is it sexy enough to warrant the time, funding and effort? “Ideas are cheap.” -- Jody’s former supervisor, Jane Raymond Good experimenters generate many ideas and ensure that only the fittest survive What are the possible confounds? Can you control for those confounds? Has the experiment already been done?  “A year of research can save you an hour on PubMed!”

Three Stages of an Experiment Sledgehammer Approach brute force experiment powerful stimulus don’t try to control for everything run a couple of subjects -- see if it looks promising if it doesn’t look great, tweak the stimulus or task try to be a subject yourself so you can notice any problems with stimuli or subject strategies Real Experiment at some point, you have to stop changing things and collect enough subjects run with the same conditions to publish it incorporate appropriate control conditions random effects analysis requires at least 10 subjects can run all subjects in one or two days pro: minimize setup and variability con: “bad magnet day” means a lot of wasted time Whipped Cream after the real experiment works, then think about a “whipped cream” version going straight to whipped cream is a huge endeavor, especially if you’re new to imaging mixed metaphor: “never sacrifice the meat & potatoes to get the gravy” (“never sacrifice the hot chocolate to get the whipped cream” doesn’t have quite the same punch)

Testing Patients fMRI is the art of the barely possible neuropsychology is the art of the barely possible combining fMRI and neuropsychology can be very valuable BUT it’s (art of the barely possible)2 If you want to test a paradigm in patients or special groups (either single cases or group studies), I recommend developing a robust paradigm in control subjects first It’s generally a bad idea to use patients for pilot testing

Understanding Subtraction Logic

Mental Chronometry use reaction times to infer cognitive processes fundamental tool for behavioral experiments in cognitive science F. C. Donders Dutch physiologist 1818-1889

Classic Example T1: Simple Reaction Time Detect Stimulus Press Button Hit button when you see a light Detect Stimulus Press Button T2: Discrimination Reaction Time Hit button when light is green but not red Detect Stimulus Discriminate Color Press Button T3: Choice Reaction Time Hit left button when light is green and right button when light is red Detect Stimulus Discriminate Color Choose Button Press Button Time

Subtraction Logic Detect Stimulus Press Button Discriminate Color T2 - = Discriminate Color

Subtraction Logic Detect Stimulus Discriminate Color Choose Button Press Button T3 - Detect Stimulus Press Button Discriminate Color T2 = Choose Button

Limitations of Subtraction Logic Assumption of pure insertion You can insert a component process into a task without disrupting the other components Widely criticized

Top Ten Things Sex and Brain Imaging Have in Common 10. It's not how big the region is, it's what you do with it.  9. Both involve heavy PETting.  8. It's important to select regions of interest.  7. Experts agree that timing is critical.  6. Both require correction for motion.  5. Experimentation is everything.  4. You often can't get access when you need it.  3. You always hope for multiple activations.  2. Both make a lot of noise.  1. Both are better when the assumption of pure insertion is met. Now you should get this joke! Source: students in the Dartmouth McDonnell-Pew Summer Institute

Subtraction Logic: Brain Imaging Example Hypothesis (circa early 1990s): Some areas of the brain are specialized for perceiving objects Simplest design: Compare pictures of objects vs. a control stimulus that is not an object seeing pictures like seeing pictures like minus = object perception Malach et al., 1995, PNAS

Objects > Textures Lateral Occipital Complex (LOC) Malach et al., 1995, PNAS

fMRI Subtraction - =

Other Differences Is subtraction logic valid here? What else could differ between objects and textures? Objects > Textures object shapes irregular shapes familiarity namability visual features (e.g., brightness, contrast, etc.) actability attention-grabbing

Other Subtractions > > > Lateral Occipital Complex Grill-Spector et al., 1998, Neuron Visual Cortex (V1) > > Kourtzi & Kanwisher, 2000, J Neurosci > Malach et al., 1995, PNAS

Dealing with Attentional Confounds fMRI data seem highly susceptible to the amount of attention drawn to the stimulus or devoted to the task. How can you ensure that activation is not simply due to an attentional confound? Add an attentional requirement to all stimuli or tasks. Example: Add a “one back” task subject must hit a button whenever a stimulus repeats the repetition detection is much harder for the scrambled shapes any activation for the intact shapes cannot be due only to attention Time Other common confounds that reviewers love to hate: eye movements motor movements

Change only one thing between conditions! As in Donders’ method, in functional imaging studies, two paired conditions should differ by the inclusion/exclusion of a single mental process How do we control the mental operations that subjects carry out in the scanner? Manipulate the stimulus works best for automatic mental processes Manipulate the task works best for controlled mental processes DON’T DO BOTH AT ONCE!!! Source: Nancy Kanwisher

Beware the “Brain Localizer” Can have multiple comparisons/baselines Most common baseline = rest In some fields the baseline may be straightforward For example, in vision studies, the baseline is often fixation on a point on an otherwise blank screen Be careful that you don’t try to subtract too much Reaching – rest = visual stimulus + localization of stimulus + arm movement + somatosensory feedback + response planning + … “Our task activated the occipito-temporo-parieto-fronto-subcortical network” Another name for this is “the brain”!

What are people doing during “rest”? What are people really doing during rest? Daydreaming, thinking “Gawd this is boring. I wonder how long I’ve been in here. I went at 2:00. It must be about 3:30 now…” Remembering, imagining “I gotta remember to pick up a carton of milk on the way home” Attending to bodily sensations “I really have to pee!”, “My back hurts”, “Get me outta here!” Getting drowsy “Zzzzzz… I only closed my eyes for a second… really!”

Problems with a Rest Baseline? For some tasks (e.g., memory studies), rest is a poor, uncontrolled baseline memory structures (e.g., medial temporal lobes) may be DEactivated in a task compared to rest To get a non-memory baseline, some memory researchers put a low-memory task in the baseline condition e.g., hearing numbers and categorizing them as even or odd Parahippocampal Cortex Stark et al., 2001, PNAS

Default Mode Network Fox and Raichle, 2007, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. red/yellow = areas that tend to be activated during tasks task > resting baseline blue/green = areas that tend to be deactivated during tasks task < resting baseline

Interpreting Activations vs. Deactivations A “rest” baseline is needed to discriminate between these two possibilities fMRI ACTIVATION (% BSC) Rest baseline Stimulus/Task Onset TIME More activation for blue than yellow More deactivation for yellow than blue If negative betas don’t make sense for your theory and you included a rest baseline, you can eliminate them with a conjunction analysis + yellow - blue AND + yellow AND + blue

Is concurrent behavioral data necessary? “Ideally, a concurrent, observable and measureable behavioral response, such as a yes or no bar-press response, measuring accuracy or reaction time, should verify task performance.” -- Mark Cohen & Susan Bookheimer, TINS, 1994 “I wonder whether PET research so far has taken the methods of experimental psychology too seriously. In standard psychology we need to have the subject do some task with an externalizable yes-or-no answer so that we have some reaction times and error rates to analyze – those are our only data. But with neuroimaging you’re looking at the brain directly so you literally don’t need the button press… I wonder whether we can be more clever in figuring out how to get subjects to think certain kinds of thoughts silently, without forcing them to do some arbitrary classification task as well. I suspect that when you have people do some artificial task and look at their brains, the strongest activity you’ll see is in the parts of the brain that are responsible for doing artificial tasks. -- Steve Pinker, interview in the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1994 Source: Nancy Kanwisher

Choosing a Block Design

Parameters for Neuroimaging You decide: number of slices slice orientation slice thickness in-plane resolution (field of view and matrix size) volume acquisition time (usually = TR) length of a run number of runs duration and sequence of epochs within each run counterbalancing within or between subjects Your physicist can help you decide: pulse sequence (e.g., gradient echo vs. spin echo) k-space sampling (e.g., echo-planar vs. spiral imaging) TR, TE, flip angle, etc.

Tradeoffs Number of slices vs. volume acquisition time “fMRI is like trying to assemble a ship in a bottle – every which way you try to move, you encounter a constraint” -- Mel Goodale Number of slices vs. volume acquisition time the more slices you take, the longer you need to acquire them e.g., 30 slices in 2 sec vs. 45 slices in 3 sec Number of slices vs. in-plane resolution the higher your in-plane resolution, the fewer slices you can acquire in a constant volume acquisition time e.g., in 2 sec, 7 slices at 1.5 x 1.5 mm resolution (128 x 128 matrix) vs. 28 slices at 3 mm x 3 mm resolution (64 x 64 matrix)

More Power to Ya! Statistical Power the probability of rejecting the null hypothesis when it is actually false “if there’s an effect, how likely are you to find it”? Effect size bigger effects, more power e.g., LO localizer (intact vs. scrambled objects) -- 1 run is usually enough looking for activation during imagery of objects might require many more runs Sample size larger n, more power more subjects longer runs more runs per subject Signal:Noise Ratio better SNR, more power higher magnetic field multi-channel coils fewer artifacts (physical noise, physiological noise)

Put your conditions in the same run! As far as possible, put the two conditions you want to compare within the same run. Why? subjects get drowsy and bored magnet may have different amounts of noise from one run to another (e.g., spike) some stats (e.g., z-normalization) may affect stats differently between runs Common flawed logic: Run1: A – baseline Run2: B – baseline “A – 0 was significant, B – 0 was not,  Area X is activated by A more than B” By this logic, there is higher activation for Places than Faces in the data to the left. Do you agree? BOLD Activation (%) Bottom line: If you want to compare A vs. B, compare A vs. B! Simple, eh? Faces Places Error bars = 95% confidence limits

Run Duration How long should a run be? Short enough that the subject can remain comfortable without moving or swallowing Long enough that you’re not wasting a lot of time restarting the scanner My ideal is ~6 ± 2 minutes

Simple Example Experiment: LO Localizer Lateral Occipital Complex responds when subject views objects Blank Screen TIME Intact Objects Scrambled Objects (Unit: Volumes) One volume (12 slices) every 2 seconds for 272 seconds (4 minutes, 32 seconds) Condition changes every 16 seconds (8 volumes)

Options for Block Design Sequences That design was only one of many possibilities. Let’s consider some of the other options and the pros and cons of each. Let’s assume we want to have an LO localizer We need at least two conditions: but we could consider including a third condition Let’s assume that in all cases we need 2 sec/volume to cover the range of slices we require Let’s also assume a total run duration of 136 volumes (x 2 sec = 272 sec = 4 min, 16 sec We’ll start with 2 condition designs…

Convolution of Single Trials Neuronal Activity BOLD Signal Haemodynamic Function Time Time Slide from Matt Brown

Block Design: Short Equal Epochs raw time course HRF-convolved time course Time (2 s volumes) Alternation every 4 sec (2 volumes) signal amplitude is weakened by HRF because signal doesn’t have enough time to return to baseline not to far from range of breathing frequency (every 4-10 sec)  could lead to respiratory artifacts if design is a task manipulation, subject is constantly changing tasks, gets confused

Block Design: Short Unequal Epochs raw time course HRF-convolved time course Time (2 s volumes) 4 sec stimuli (2 volumes) with 8 sec (4 volumes) baseline we’ve gained back most of the HRF-based amplitude loss but the other problems still remain now we’re spending most of our time sampling the baseline

Block Design: Long Epochs The other extreme… raw time course HRF-convolved time course Time (2 s volumes) Alternation Every 68 sec (34 volumes) more noise at low frequencies linear trend confound subject will get bored very few repetitions – hard to do eyeball test of significance

Find the “Sweet Spots” Respiration every 4-10 sec (0.3 Hz) moving chest distorts susceptibility Cardiac Cycle every ~1 sec (0.9 Hz) pulsing motion, blood changes Solutions gating avoiding paradigms at those frequencies You want your paradigm frequency to be in a “sweet spot” away from the noise

Block Design: Medium Epochs raw time course HRF-convolved time course Time (2 s volumes) Every 16 sec (8 volumes) allows enough time for signal to oscillate fully not near artifact frequencies enough repetitions to see cycles by eye a reasonable time for subjects to keep doing the same thing

Block Design: Other Niceties truncated too soon Time (2 s volumes) If you start and end with a baseline condition, you’re less likely to lose information with linear trend removal and you can use the last epoch in an event related average

Block Design Sequences: Three Conditions Suppose you want to add a third condition to act as a more neutral baseline For example, if you wanted to identify visual areas as well as object-selective areas, you could include resting fixation as the baseline. That would allow two subtractions scrambled - fixation  visual areas intact - scrambled  object-selective areas That would also help you discriminate differences in activations from differences in deactivations Now the options increase. For simplicity, let’s keep the epoch duration at 16 sec.

Block Design: Repeating Sequence We could just order the epochs in a repeating sequence… Problem: There might be order effects Solution: Counterbalance with another order Problem: If you lose a run (e.g., to head motion), you lose counterbalancing)

Block Design: Random Sequence We could make multiple runs with the order of conditions randomized… Problem: Randomization can be flukey Problem: To avoid flukiness, you’d want to have different randomization for different runs and different subjects, but then you’re going to spend ages defining protocols for analysis

Block Design: Regular Baseline We could have a fixation baseline between all stimulus conditions (either with regular or random order) Benefit: With event-related averaging, this regular baseline design provides nice clear time courses, even for a block design Problem: You’re spending half of your scan time collecting the condition you care the least about

But I have 4 conditions to compare! Here are a couple of options. A. Orderly progression Pro: Simple Con: May be some confounds (e.g., linear trend if you predict green&blue > pink&yellow) B. Random order in each run Pro: order effects should average out Con: pain to make various protocols, no possibility to average all data into one time course, many frequencies involved

C. Kanwisher lab clustered design sets of four main condition epochs separated by baseline epochs each main condition appears at each location in sequence of four two counterbalanced orders (1st half of first order same as 2nd half of second order and vice versa) – can even rearrange data from 2nd order to allow averaging with 1st order Pro: spends most of your n on key conditions, provides more repetitions Con: not great for event-related averaging because orders are not balanced (e.g., in top order, blue is preceded by the baseline 1X, by green 2X, by yellow 1X and by pink 0X. As you can imagine, the more conditions you try to shove in a run, the thornier ordering issues are and the fewer n you have for each condition.

But I have 8 conditions to compare! Just don’t. In my experience, any block design experiment with more than four conditions becomes unmanageable and incomprehensible Event-related designs might still be an option… stay tuned…

EXTRA SLIDES

Prepare Well: Subjects recruit and screen your subjects well in advance safety screening best to let them read through and self-screen beforehand so you don’t get any embarrassing situations (e.g., discussions about IUDs, pregnancy) eye glasses handedness make sure your subjects know how to be good subjects http://www.ssc.uwo.ca/psychology/culhamlab/Jody_web/Subject_Info/firsttime_subjects.htm make sure you and the subjects can contact each other in case of problems or delays if possible, be a subject yourself to see what the pitfalls and strategies might be remember to bring: subject fees (and receipt book) consent and screening forms

Prepare Well: Experiments test all equipment in advance test software under realistic circumstances (same computer, timing and duration as fMRI experiments) make sure you know all of the parameters the technician will want (e.g., pulse sequence, timing, slices and orientation) at RRI, prepare a spreadsheet with mouseclicks and stopwatch times check the timing as you go, especially at the beginning of an experiment keep accurate log notes as you go check with the technician regularly to ensure that your log notes record the same run number as the scanner attach your timing spreadsheet to the log notes for that subject write down any problems that arose (e.g., “subject missed second last trial”; “subject drowsy through first ~third of run”)

Prepare Well: Postprocessing move data to secure location as soon as possible save one backup in the rawest form possible if advances in reconstruction occur, you will need unprocessed data to use them save other backups at natural points (e.g., backup and delete 2D data once you’ve made 3D data) have redundancy don’t put all backups on the same CD/DVD or you’re toast if one is damaged (CDs aren’t forever like we once thought) save full projects to one DVD (or HD partition) once you’re done so you can reload an entire project if you need to reanalyze keep a subject archive …

Dealing with Frustration Murphy's law acts with particular vigour in fMR imaging:  Number of pieces of equipment required in an fMRI experiment: ~50 Probability of any one piece of equipment working in a session: 95% Probability of everything working in a session: 0.95^50 = 7.6% Sign that used to be at the 1.5 T at MGH Solution for a good imaging session = $4 million magnet + $3 roll of duct tape

How NOT to do an imaging experiment ask a stupid question e.g., “I wonder what lights up for nose picking vs. rest” compare poorly-defined conditions that differ in many respects use a paradigm from another technique (e.g., cognitive psychology) without optimizing any of the timing for fMRI, e.g., 1 minute epochs be naively optimistic go straight for the “whipped cream” experiment without starting with a “sledgehammer” experiment never look at raw data, time courses or individual data, just plunk it all into one big stat model and look at what comes out publish a long list of activated foci in every possible comparison don’t use any statistical corrections write a long discussion on why your task activates the subcortico-occipito-parieto-temporo-frontal network