Chapter 4: Molecular Genetics and Development.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Genetics III: Examples Mutation Analyses- Strengths: Implicate roles for specific genes in animal behaviors Weakness: Lead to misconception that those.
Advertisements

Female reproductive success is largely determined by parental effort Male reproductive success is largely determined by mating effort Because females.
Chap.03 Proximate factors 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所.
The eusocial insects: Isoptera: Termites Hymenoptera: Ants, bees, wasps.
Homework #1 due 2/7 Bonus #1 due 2/28. CB 17.4 Combinations of 3 nucleotides code for each 1 amino acid in a protein.
Hormones and Behavior Hormones Modulate Behavior Hormones Can Be Necessary for a Behavior A particular level of hormone in an animal does not insure a.
The Inheritance of Complex Traits
Chapter 3 Opener A leafcutter ant worker carries a leaf back to the colony while a much smaller worker rides on the leaf to protect her sister against.
Chapter 7 Parental Care Behaviors. 7.1 Some species provide little or no parental care Little or no parental care provided by salmon, but female Nile.
Chapter 52 Behavioral Biology Innate behavior Some behaviors are “preprogrammed” into the nervous system Triggered by a stimulus - can vary Other examples??
The Effect of Social Environment Behavior Development Blotched Gray Arizona Eastern Barred Tiger Salamander US Canada Distribution by Species.
Announcements. Number of eggs / size of litter Hatching order / Asynchrony in hatching Sex of offspring.
Learning Relatively permanent modification of behavior that occurs through practice or experience Learning is not maturation Learning is not always observable.
Announcements. tarantula hawk red shouldered hawk emperor penguin Parental care Very costly behavior - time - energy - vulnerable to predation earwig.
HUMAN REPRODUCTION BIOLOGY 269. COURSE HOMEPAGE: The course syllabus is available online, linked to that homepage.
Take home final exam April 29 th DUE: May 5 th in my office by 1pm 1100 to 1300All classes meeting at 0800 or 0830MWF Exam format How many: MC? (50%) Short.
Alterations in ZENK Expression in Zebra Finches Reared by a Single Parent Tareq Yousef, Jordan Fisk, Tara S. Perrot, and Leslie S. Phillmore Department.
Can genes help explain our evolution? - What type of changes (regulatory or structural mutations?) - How many genes are involved?
What is a QTL? Quantitative trait locus (loci) Region of chromosome that contributes to variation in a quantitative trait Generally used to study “complex.
Genetics 3.1 Genes. Essential Idea: Every living organism inherits a blueprint for life from its parents.
Exams back T 9/30 Bonus #1 is due 10/02 Regulating Gene Expression.
Polyandry.
HONEY BEES! BROUGHT TO YOU BY.
Difference between a monohybrid cross and a dihybrid cross
Courtship: Birdsong.
Differentiation in eusocial colonies
chapter 6: Mendel & heredity
Genetic Variation Genetic Variation in Populations
Organization of Song Learning
Figure 3. Active enhancers located in intergenic DMRs
Chapter 18 Gene Expression.
Heredity The passing of characteristics from parent to offspring.
Sex Linked Inheritance
Sex-Linked Traits Objective 53: I can describe sex-linked traits
Pedigrees.
Pedigrees.
Notes 1 2nd Semester Genetics.
Lasting Epigenetic Influence of Early-Life Adversity on the BDNF Gene
HUMAN REPRODUCTION BIOLOGY 269
GENETICS A Conceptual Approach
Behavioral Ecology (Part 2)
THE WORST INVASIVE ANTS IN THE PACIFIC
THE WORST INVASIVE ANTS IN THE PACIFIC
Polyandry.
Animal Behaviors.
RNAi Related Mechanisms Affect Both Transcriptional and Posttranscriptional Transgene Silencing in Drosophila  Manika Pal-Bhadra, Utpal Bhadra, James.
The Evolution of Populations
Genetics.
Animal Signals and Communication
Formation of Chromosomal Domains by Loop Extrusion
The Mouse Spo11 Gene Is Required for Meiotic Chromosome Synapsis
Volume 19, Issue 5, Pages (November 1997)
Anne L. Sapiro, Patricia Deng, Rui Zhang, Jin Billy Li  Cell Reports 
Genetics Vocabulary Gene – a location on DNA that codes for a trait; located on both sets of chromosomes Allele – the specific gene that comes either from.
Heredity The passing of characteristics from parent to offspring.
Volume 27, Issue 24, Pages e3 (December 2017)
Unit 4: Principles of Ecology
GENETICS A Conceptual Approach
Volume 16, Issue 6, Pages (August 2016)
Chapter 7 Beyond alleles: Quantitative Genetics
Parental Care.
Maternal Care and DNA Methylation
Heat map of additive effects for PCs QTL
Modes of selection.
Chapter 12 Mendel’s Genetics
IL-10R-deficient macrophages secrete IL-23, inducing IL-22 secretion by ILC3 and TH17 cells. IL-10R-deficient macrophages secrete IL-23, inducing IL-22.
Chapter 25 Animal Interactions
Mice with Ric-8b-deficient olfactory epithelium show impaired olfactory guided behavior. Mice with Ric-8b-deficient olfactory epithelium show impaired.
Volume 9, Issue 3, Pages (November 2014)
The expression of miR-9 and miR-140-5p in Area X is regulated during vocal development. The expression of miR-9 and miR-140-5p in Area X is regulated during.
Presentation transcript:

Chapter 4: Molecular Genetics and Development

FIGURE 4. 1. Genes, termite workers, and queens FIGURE 4.1. Genes, termite workers, and queens. (A) Queen Cryptotermes secundus (bottom left) with male (top right) and workers. (B) Butting among workers in colonies with queen and without queens. (C) and (D) Butting before and after treatment of the queen with Neofem2 small interfering RNA (siRNA) and control siRNA. (Photo credit: Korb, J., et al (2009). Science, 324, 758–758)

FIGURE 4. 1a. Genes, termite workers, and queens FIGURE 4.1a. Genes, termite workers, and queens. (A) Queen Cryptotermes secundus (bottom left) with male (top right) and workers. (B) Butting among workers in colonies with queen and without queens. (C) and (D) Butting before and after treatment of the queen with Neofem2 small interfering RNA (siRNA) and control siRNA. (Photo credit: Korb, J., et al (2009). Science, 324, 758–758)

FIGURE 4. 1b. Genes, termite workers, and queens FIGURE 4.1b. Genes, termite workers, and queens. (A) Queen Cryptotermes secundus (bottom left) with male (top right) and workers. (B) Butting among workers in colonies with queen and without queens. (C) and (D) Butting before and after treatment of the queen with Neofem2 small interfering RNA (siRNA) and control siRNA. (Photo credit: Korb, J., et al (2009). Science, 324, 758–758)

FIGURE 4. 1c. Genes, termite workers, and queens FIGURE 4.1c. Genes, termite workers, and queens. (A) Queen Cryptotermes secundus (bottom left) with male (top right) and workers. (B) Butting among workers in colonies with queen and without queens. (C) and (D) Butting before and after treatment of the queen with Neofem2 small interfering RNA (siRNA) and control siRNA. (Photo credit: Korb, J., et al (2009). Science, 324, 758–758)

FIGURE 4. 1d. Genes, termite workers, and queens FIGURE 4.1d. Genes, termite workers, and queens. (A) Queen Cryptotermes secundus (bottom left) with male (top right) and workers. (B) Butting among workers in colonies with queen and without queens. (C) and (D) Butting before and after treatment of the queen with Neofem2 small interfering RNA (siRNA) and control siRNA. (Photo credit: Korb, J., et al (2009). Science, 324, 758–758)

FIGURE 4. 2a. Satellite and independent ruff males FIGURE 4.2a. Satellite and independent ruff males. Some of the differences in the mating behavior of (A) satellite males and (B) independent males are controlled by a single gene with two alleles labeled S and s. (Photo credits: BS Thurner Hof).

FIGURE 4. 2b. Satellite and independent ruff males FIGURE 4.2b. Satellite and independent ruff males. Some of the differences in the mating behavior of (A) satellite males and (B) independent males are controlled by a single gene with two alleles labeled S and s. (Photo credits: BS Thurner Hof).

FIGURE 4. 3. Quantitative Trait Loci Mapping FIGURE 4.3. Quantitative Trait Loci Mapping. Quantitative Trait Loci (QTL) mapping allows researchers to find the general region of the genome in which quantitative trait loci reside by using marker loci that are easily assayed, but unrelated to the trait in question, in order to identify the approximate locations of the unknown alleles that do affect the trait of interest. (From Bergstrom and Dugatkin, 2012)

FIGURE 4. 3. Quantitative Trait Loci Mapping FIGURE 4.3. Quantitative Trait Loci Mapping. Quantitative Trait Loci (QTL) mapping allows researchers to find the general region of the genome in which quantitative trait loci reside by using marker loci that are easily assayed, but unrelated to the trait in question, in order to identify the approximate locations of the unknown alleles that do affect the trait of interest. (From Bergstrom and Dugatkin, 2012)

FIGURE 4. 4. QTLs for mouse behavior FIGURE 4.4. QTLs for mouse behavior. Chromosomal distribution of QTLs linked to anxiety in mice. (From Wills-Owen and Flint, 2006)

FIGURE 4. 4. QTLs for mouse behavior FIGURE 4.4. QTLs for mouse behavior. Chromosomal distribution of QTLs linked to anxiety in mice. (From Wills-Owen and Flint, 2006)

FIGURE 4. 5. Foraging, age, and mRNA FIGURE 4.5. Foraging, age, and mRNA. (A) Foraging bees have significantly higher levels of per mRNA than younger, nonforaging bees. This difference could be due to age, behavior caste (forager versus nonforager), or both. (B) Some individual bees developed into precocious foragers that began searching for food much earlier than usual. When ten-day-old precocious foragers were compared with normal foragers (twenty-two-dayold foragers), Toma and his colleagues found no statistical differences in per mRNA levels. (From Toma et al., 2000)

FIGURE 4. 5a. Foraging, age, and mRNA FIGURE 4.5a. Foraging, age, and mRNA. (A) Foraging bees have significantly higher levels of per mRNA than younger, nonforaging bees. This difference could be due to age, behavior caste (forager versus nonforager), or both. (B) Some individual bees developed into precocious foragers that began searching for food much earlier than usual. When ten-day-old precocious foragers were compared with normal foragers (twenty-two-dayold foragers), Toma and his colleagues found no statistical differences in per mRNA levels. (From Toma et al., 2000)

FIGURE 4. 5b. Foraging, age, and mRNA FIGURE 4.5b. Foraging, age, and mRNA. (A) Foraging bees have significantly higher levels of per mRNA than younger, nonforaging bees. This difference could be due to age, behavior caste (forager versus nonforager), or both. (B) Some individual bees developed into precocious foragers that began searching for food much earlier than usual. When ten-day-old precocious foragers were compared with normal foragers (twenty-two-dayold foragers), Toma and his colleagues found no statistical differences in per mRNA levels. (From Toma et al., 2000)

FIGURE 4. 6. Pollen foragers, nectar foragers, and manganese FIGURE 4.6. Pollen foragers, nectar foragers, and manganese. Pollen foragers have more manganese in their heads than both nectar foragers and nurses. (From Ben-Shahar et al., 2004)

FIGURE 4. 7. A small change goes a long way FIGURE 4.7. A small change goes a long way. A single amino acid change is all that separates the coding for ultraviolet versus violet perception in the zebra finch. (Photo credit: Ann and Rob Simpson)

FIGURE 4. 8. FOXP2 and song learning FIGURE 4.8. FOXP2 and song learning. When the FOXP2 gene was knocked out in young zebra finches, their ability to learn other finches’ songs was diminished. (From Haesler et al., 2007)

FIGURE 4. 8. FOXP2 and song learning FIGURE 4.8. FOXP2 and song learning. When the FOXP2 gene was knocked out in young zebra finches, their ability to learn other finches’ songs was diminished. (From Haesler et al., 2007)

FIGURE 4. 9. ZENK and exposure to song FIGURE 4.9. ZENK and exposure to song. (A) Induction of zenk mRNA in the forebrain of a male zebra finch that has been exposed to zebra finch song for forty-five minutes. (B) Same area in male that was not exposed to song. (From Mello et al., 1992, p. 6819; courtesy Claudio Mello)

FIGURE 4. 10. Zenk levels and habituation FIGURE 4.10. Zenk levels and habituation. (A) Induced zenk mRNA levels decreased with increased exposure to the same song in male zebra finches. (B) Induced zenk mRNA levels increased when a male zebra finch was exposed to a new song. (Adapted from Mello et al., 1995)

FIGURE 4. 10. Zenk levels and habituation FIGURE 4.10. Zenk levels and habituation. (A) Induced zenk mRNA levels decreased with increased exposure to the same song in male zebra finches. (B) Induced zenk mRNA levels increased when a male zebra finch was exposed to a new song. (Adapted from Mello et al., 1995)

FIGURE 4. 10a. Zenk levels and habituation FIGURE 4.10a. Zenk levels and habituation. (A) Induced zenk mRNA levels decreased with increased exposure to the same song in male zebra finches. (B) Induced zenk mRNA levels increased when a male zebra finch was exposed to a new song. (Adapted from Mello et al., 1995)

FIGURE 4. 10b. Zenk levels and habituation FIGURE 4.10b. Zenk levels and habituation. (A) Induced zenk mRNA levels decreased with increased exposure to the same song in male zebra finches. (B) Induced zenk mRNA levels increased when a male zebra finch was exposed to a new song. (Adapted from Mello et al., 1995)

FIGURE 4. 10b. Zenk levels and habituation FIGURE 4.10b. Zenk levels and habituation. (A) Induced zenk mRNA levels decreased with increased exposure to the same song in male zebra finches. (B) Induced zenk mRNA levels increased when a male zebra finch was exposed to a new song. (Adapted from Mello et al., 1995)

FIGURE 4. 11. Gene expression levels change as songbirds sing FIGURE 4.11. Gene expression levels change as songbirds sing. Expression of some genes increases as zebra finches sing, while expression of others decreases. (From Warren et al., 2010)

FIGURE 4. 11. Gene expression levels change as songbirds sing FIGURE 4.11. Gene expression levels change as songbirds sing. Expression of some genes increases as zebra finches sing, while expression of others decreases. (From Warren et al., 2010)

FIGURE 4. 12a. Avpr1a length and behavior FIGURE 4.12a. Avpr1a length and behavior. (A) A schematic of the long and short avpr1a alleles and the expression of vasopressin receptors in the brain of prairie voles (differences in expression can be seen at the two red arrows). (B) Both male care of offspring and the strength of partner preference were greater in males who were homozygous for the longer avpr1a allele. (From Donaldson and Young, 2008; Hammock and Young, 2005)

FIGURE 4. 12b. Avpr1a length and behavior FIGURE 4.12b. Avpr1a length and behavior. (A) A schematic of the long and short avpr1a alleles and the expression of vasopressin receptors in the brain of prairie voles (differences in expression can be seen at the two red arrows). (B) Both male care of offspring and the strength of partner preference were greater in males who were homozygous for the longer avpr1a allele. (From Donaldson and Young, 2008; Hammock and Young, 2005)

FIGURE 4. 13. Dispersal strategies in Erigone altra FIGURE 4.13. Dispersal strategies in Erigone altra. These spiders use temperature as a development cue for when to use risky (ballooning) versus less risky (rappeling) dispersal strategies. (Photo credit: TDP Invertebrate Surveyors and Consultants, Trevor Pendleton)

FIGURE 4. 14. Temperature, learning, and egg laying FIGURE 4.14. Temperature, learning, and egg laying. Exposure to cold temperature (4° C for three weeks for larvae in group 1, 4° C for twelve weeks for larvae in group 2) during development had significant effects on a female wasp’s ability as an adult to discriminate between hosts of different quality. Larvae in group 3, which were continuously raised at normal temperatures of 24° C, did not experience these effects. Exposure to cold temperature also had a strong negative effect on the speed at which females learned to avoid already parasitized hosts, as they had to choose to lay their eggs in patches in which none of the hosts had been parasitized (left), half had been parasitized (center), or all had been parasitized (right). Orange circles indicate a parasitized host. (Based on van Baaren et al., 2005)

FIGURE 4.15. Amount of licking and grooming differed in one- versus two-parent nests. (A) In prairie voles, pups in the biparental (BP) treatment received more licking and grooming than pups in the single-mother (SM) treatment. (B) After they matured, females that were raised by a male and female (BP-reared) displayed more licking/grooming and pup care than females raised by only a female (SM-reared). These females also spent less time away from their pups than females raised by only a female. (From Ahem and Young, 2009)

FIGURE 4.15a. Amount of licking and grooming differed in one- versus two-parent nests. (A) In prairie voles, pups in the biparental (BP) treatment received more licking and grooming than pups in the single-mother (SM) treatment. (B) After they matured, females that were raised by a male and female (BP-reared) displayed more licking/grooming and pup care than females raised by only a female (SM-reared). These females also spent less time away from their pups than females raised by only a female. (From Ahem and Young, 2009)

FIGURE 4.15b. Amount of licking and grooming differed in one- versus two-parent nests. (A) In prairie voles, pups in the biparental (BP) treatment received more licking and grooming than pups in the single-mother (SM) treatment. (B) After they matured, females that were raised by a male and female (BP-reared) displayed more licking/grooming and pup care than females raised by only a female (SM-reared). These females also spent less time away from their pups than females raised by only a female. (From Ahem and Young, 2009)

FIGURE 4. 16. Nest building and experience in oldfield mice FIGURE 4.16. Nest building and experience in oldfield mice. The proportion of females that did not start building nests was lower in experienced females (green) versus inexperienced females (orange). Experienced females began to build nests sooner and built superior nests than inexperienced. (From Margulis et al., 2005)