From: The effect of perceived surface orientation on perceived surface albedo in binocularly viewed scenes Journal of Vision. 2003;3(8):2. doi:10.1167/3.8.2.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Point A Point B Point C Point D Point E
Advertisements

From: Disparity sensitivity in man and owl: Psychophysical evidence for equivalent perception of shape-from-stereo Journal of Vision. 2010;10(1):10. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: System gamma as a function of image- and monitor-dynamic range
From: Focus information is used to interpret binocular images
From: Estimating 3D tilt from local image cues in natural scenes
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2003;3(5):3. doi: /3.5.3 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Vectors and the Geometry
From: Why the long face? The importance of vertical image structure for biological “barcodes” underlying face recognition Journal of Vision. 2014;14(8):25.
Copyright © Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.
Figure Legend: From: Visual detection of symmetry of 3D shapes
From: Contribution of the crystalline lens gradient refractive index to the accommodation amplitude in non-human primates: In vitro studies Journal of.
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(2):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Illumination discrimination in real and simulated scenes
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(16):13. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Survey of Heliostat Concepts for Cost Reduction
Figure Legend: From: Key characteristics of specular stereo
Figure Legend: From: Adaptation to blurred and sharpened video
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(12):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Noise masking of White's illusion exposes the weakness of current spatial filtering models of lightness perception Journal of Vision. 2015;15(14):1.
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Key characteristics of specular stereo
Figure Legend: From: Color constancy and hue scaling
Figure Legend: From: Comparing integration rules in visual search
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):22. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Slant from texture and disparity cues: Optimal cue combination
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):1. doi: /9.5.1 Figure Legend:
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Dynamics of oculomotor direction discrimination
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Inhibition of saccade and vergence eye movements in 3D space
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
From: Size-induced distortions in perceptual maps of visual space
From: Variability of eye movements when viewing dynamic natural scenes
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(10):5. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(15):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Figure Legend: From: Spatial structure of contextual modulation
Copyright © Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.
Figure Legend: From: The fine structure of multifocal ERG topographies
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(12):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Copyright © Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.
From: Optimal disparity estimation in natural stereo images
Vectors and the Geometry
Plotting Points Guided Notes
Presentation transcript:

From: The effect of perceived surface orientation on perceived surface albedo in binocularly viewed scenes Journal of Vision. 2003;3(8):2. doi:10.1167/3.8.2 Figure Legend: Coordinate systems. A. Cartesian and spherical coordinates. The origin of the coordinate system was the center of the test surface. Cartesian coordinates are specified as (x, y, z) vectors. The z-axis lies along the line of sight from the observer to the origin. The y-axis is vertical, and the x-axis horizontal, both in the fronto-parallel plane containing the origin. The positive side of the x-axis is to the observer’s right, of the y-axis is upward, and of the z-axis is toward the observer. The spherical coordinate system (Ψ,ϕ,r) is the angle between the z-axis and the projection of any vector into the xz-plane. The angle ϕ is the angle between a vector and the xz-plane. The radius r is measured from the origin in centimeters. N is the unit normal to the surface; P is a vector in the direction of the punctate source. B. Spherical coordinates and the test surface. The viewpoint is from above, along the y-axis, which is shown with the symbol ⊙, pointing out of the page. The central cube is shown in outline. The possible orientations of the test patch (ΨT = {s−50°, −40°, −30°, 0°, 30°, 40°, 50°}) are shown as well as the direction to the projection of the punctate light source onto the xz-plane. The origin of the coordinate system is always in the middle of the test patch, the observer is always at (x, y, z)=(0, 0, 70 cm), which is (Ψ, ϕ, r) = (0°, 0°, 70 cm) in spherical coordinates, and the punctate light source is always at (x, y, z)= (−55 cm, 40 cm, 140 cm), which is (Ψ,ϕ,r)=(−21.44°, 14.89°, 155.64 cm) in spherical coordinates. To maintain the center of the test patch at the origin, the central cube is displaced slightly from trial to trial depending on the orientation of the test patch being displayed. Date of download: 12/22/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.