From: Surface color perception and equivalent illumination models

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Date of download: 9/18/2016 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © All rights reserved. From: What determines the relationship.
Advertisements

From: What limits performance in the amblyopic visual system: Seeing signals in noise with an amblyopic brain Journal of Vision. 2008;8(4):1. doi: /8.4.1.
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(13):22. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(8):11. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2003;3(5):3. doi: /3.5.3 Figure Legend:
From: Responses of the human visual cortex and LGN to achromatic and chromatic temporal modulations: An fMRI study Journal of Vision. 2010;10(13):13. doi: /
From: Color signals in the primary visual cortex of marmosets
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field
From: Breaking object correspondence across saccades impairs object recognition: The role of color and luminance Journal of Vision. 2016;16(11):1. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(2):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The modern Japanese color lexicon
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The attenuation surface for contrast sensitivity has the form of a witch's hat within the central visual field Journal of Vision. 2012;12(11):23.
From: The biological significance of color constancy: An agent-based model with bees foraging from flowers under varied illumination Journal of Vision.
Figure Legend: From: Key characteristics of specular stereo
From: Illumination assumptions account for individual differences in the perceptual interpretation of a profoundly ambiguous stimulus in the color domain:
From: Visual speed sensitivity in the drum corps color guard
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(8):11. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Nonlinearities in color coding: Compensating color appearance for the eye's spectral sensitivity Journal of Vision. 2006;6(9):12. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: The color lexicon of the Somali language
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Contextual effects on real bicolored glossy surfaces
Figure Legend: From: Color constancy and hue scaling
Figure Legend: From: Color constancy and hue scaling
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(6):4. doi: /4.6.4 Figure Legend:
From: Slant from texture and disparity cues: Optimal cue combination
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(2):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: statistical inference
From: What #theDress reveals about the role of illumination priors in color perception and color constancy Journal of Vision. 2017;17(9):4. doi: /
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(2):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(12):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Trichromatic reconstruction from the interleaved cone mosaic: Bayesian model and the color appearance of small spots Journal of Vision. 2008;8(5):15.
From: Rat performance on visual detection task modeled with divisive normalization and adaptive decision thresholds Journal of Vision. 2011;11(9):1. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2014;14(14):2. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Optimizing wavefront-guided corrections for highly aberrated eyes in the presence of registration uncertainty Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):8. doi: /
From: Optimizing wavefront-guided corrections for highly aberrated eyes in the presence of registration uncertainty Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):8. doi: /
From: Linear–nonlinear models of the red–green chromatic pathway
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Rapid visual categorization of natural scene contexts with equalized amplitude spectrum and increasing phase noise Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):2.
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(6):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(15):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(8):6. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(7):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(8):11. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The absolute threshold of cone vision
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(11):18. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The biological significance of color constancy: An agent-based model with bees foraging from flowers under varied illumination Journal of Vision.
From: StarTrek Illusion—General object constancy phenomenon?
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(3):1. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The color constancy of three-dimensional objects
Presentation transcript:

From: Surface color perception and equivalent illumination models Journal of Vision. 2011;11(5):1. doi:10.1167/11.5.1 Figure Legend: (A) Three scenes studied by Delahunt and Brainard (2004). Subjects viewed renderings of the scenes in stereo and set the chromaticity of a test patch (location is indicated by black rectangle) to appear achromatic. Measurements were made for 17 scenes. Across some scenes, only the illuminant changed (e.g., left to center). Across other scenes, both the illuminant and the reflectance of the back surface changed (e.g., left to right). This latter manipulation eliminated local contrast as a cue to the illuminant change. (B) The measured achromatic locus may be interpreted as the visual system's estimate of the illuminant (Brainard et al., 2006). This is shown as a red dashed line in all three panels. The corresponding illuminant estimated by the Bayesian algorithm is shown as a blue dashed line in each panel. In the left panel, the scene illuminant is shown as a solid black line. This line is replotted in the middle and right panels for comparison. In those panels, the physical illuminant is shown as a solid green line. The Bayesian algorithm predicts the human equivalent illuminants well, both for cases of good constancy (e.g., left to center panel scene change) and for cases of poor constancy (e.g., left to right panel scene change). Date of download: 11/12/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.