From: Estimating 3D tilt from local image cues in natural scenes

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: Human perception of subresolution fineness of dense textures based on image intensity statistics Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):8. doi: /
Advertisements

Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: A unified Bayesian observer analysis for set size and cueing effects on perceptual decisions and saccades Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):27. doi: /
From: System gamma as a function of image- and monitor-dynamic range
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(7):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2003;3(5):3. doi: /3.5.3 Figure Legend:
Date of download: 10/23/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
From: Comparison of a MEMS-Based Handheld OCT Scanner With a Commercial Desktop OCT System for Retinal Evaluation Trans. Vis. Sci. Tech ;3(4):3.
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Bayesian inference for psychometric functions
Date of download: 10/26/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Does print size matter for reading
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(2):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Date of download: 11/1/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
From: Spatial attention alleviates temporal crowding, but neither temporal nor spatial uncertainty are necessary for the emergence of temporal crowding.
From: Illumination discrimination in real and simulated scenes
From: The gender-specific face aftereffect is based in retinotopic not spatiotopic coordinates across several natural image transformations Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: An escape from crowding
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(14):32. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Noise masking of White's illusion exposes the weakness of current spatial filtering models of lightness perception Journal of Vision. 2015;15(14):1.
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Figure Legend: From: Color constancy and hue scaling
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):22. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Optimum spatiotemporal receptive fields for vision in dim light
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(6):4. doi: /4.6.4 Figure Legend:
From: Surface color perception and equivalent illumination models
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Figure Legend: From: Gating of remote effects on lightness
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: A new theoretical approach to improving face recognition in disorders of central vision: Face caricaturing Journal of Vision. 2014;14(2):12. doi: /
From: The effect of perceived surface orientation on perceived surface albedo in binocularly viewed scenes Journal of Vision. 2003;3(8):2. doi: /3.8.2.
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(1):17. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Variability of eye movements when viewing dynamic natural scenes
From: The perceptual basis of common photographic practice
Date of download: 12/27/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: What's color got to do with it
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(15):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
From: Shape, motion, and optical cues to stiffness of elastic objects
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Figure Legend: From: Measuring visual clutter
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Spatial structure of contextual modulation
Figure Legend: From: The fine structure of multifocal ERG topographies
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
Figure Legend: From: Eidolons: Novel stimuli for vision research
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
From: Optimal disparity estimation in natural stereo images
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
Presentation transcript:

From: Estimating 3D tilt from local image cues in natural scenes Journal of Vision. 2016;16(13):2. doi:10.1167/16.13.2 Figure Legend: Registered range and camera images. (A) Camera and laser range scanner mounted on portable four-axis robotic gantry: (A) natural scene, (B) Nikon D700 DSLR camera, (C) Riegl VZ-400 3D range scanner, (D) custom robotic gantry. (B) A 200 × 200 pixel patch of a camera image of stone structure mapped onto the range image. The registration is generally within plus or minus one pixel. Note that the shadows in the camera image coincide with the mortared seams in the stone structure. Date of download: 10/19/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.