From: Human perception of subresolution fineness of dense textures based on image intensity statistics Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):8. doi:10.1167/17.4.8.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: The Müller-Lyer Illusion: Investigation of a Center of Gravity Effect on the Amplitudes of Saccades Journal of Vision. 2010;10(1):11. doi: /
Advertisements

Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):14. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: in visual cortex
From: System gamma as a function of image- and monitor-dynamic range
From: Multimodal Industrial Inspection and Analysis
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
From: Estimating 3D tilt from local image cues in natural scenes
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):3. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2003;3(5):3. doi: /3.5.3 Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(12):7. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Asymmetric interference between the perception of shape and the perception of surface properties Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):13. doi: /
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Date of download: 10/23/2017 Copyright © ASME. All rights reserved.
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(13):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):3. doi: /4.9.3 Figure Legend:
From: What are the units of storage in visual working memory?
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(13):27. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Does print size matter for reading
From: Neural bandwidth of veridical perception across the visual field
From: Subjectively homogeneous noise over written text as a tool to investigate the perceptual mechanisms involved in reading Journal of Vision. 2013;13(11):14.
From: ERP P1-N1 changes associated with Vernier perceptual learning and its location specificity and transfer Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):19. doi: /
From: Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects Journal of Vision. 2011;11(7):1. doi: /
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(2):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: The gender-specific face aftereffect is based in retinotopic not spatiotopic coordinates across several natural image transformations Journal of.
Journal of Vision. 2013;13(4):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Illumination assumptions account for individual differences in the perceptual interpretation of a profoundly ambiguous stimulus in the color domain:
From: Metacontrast masking within and between visual channels: Effects of orientation and spatial frequency contrasts Journal of Vision. 2010;10(6):12.
Journal of Vision. 2012;12(6):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2008;8(3):33. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Key characteristics of specular stereo
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):22. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(5):31. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Optimum spatiotemporal receptive fields for vision in dim light
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene?
Figure Legend: From: Gating of remote effects on lightness
Figure Legend: From: Development of a Pediatric Visual Field Test
From: Perception of light source distance from shading patterns
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(6):4. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2009;9(1):19. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Latitude and longitude vertical disparities
From: The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive
From: Probing visual consciousness: Rivalry between eyes and images
From: Expertise with multisensory events eliminates the effect of biological motion rotation on audiovisual synchrony perception Journal of Vision. 2010;10(5):2.
From: The perceptual basis of common photographic practice
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(6):12. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(15):21. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Perceived surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two light sources differing in chromaticity Journal of Vision. 2004;4(9):1. doi: /4.9.1.
From: Rethinking ADA signage standards for low-vision accessibility
Figure Legend: From: Fixations on low-resolution images
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(10):9. doi: / Figure Legend:
From: Learning to integrate arbitrary signals from vision and touch
Figure Legend: From: The resolution of facial expressions of emotion
Figure Legend: From: Measuring visual clutter
From: Four types of ensemble coding in data visualizations
Journal of Vision. 2016;16(3):28. doi: / Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: The fine structure of multifocal ERG topographies
From: Objects predict fixations better than early saliency
From: Statistics for optimal point prediction in natural images
Figure Legend: From: Eidolons: Novel stimuli for vision research
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Figure Legend: From: Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate
Journal of Vision. 2006;6(5):2. doi: /6.5.2 Figure Legend:
Figure Legend: From: Analysis of human vergence dynamics
Journal of Vision. 2010;10(4):15. doi: / Figure Legend:
Journal of Vision. 2015;15(9):20. doi: / Figure Legend:
Presentation transcript:

From: Human perception of subresolution fineness of dense textures based on image intensity statistics Journal of Vision. 2017;17(4):8. doi:10.1167/17.4.8 Figure Legend: Computer-generated human hair images. Subpixel image rendering (Shinya, Shiraishi, Dobashi, Iwasaki, & Nishita, 2010) makes the hair texture in the right image (hair width: 0.3 pixels) look more natural and finer than that in the left image (hair width: 1 pixel). Date of download: 10/2/2017 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Copyright © 2017. All rights reserved.