Lecture Outline nHeuristics and Social Inference nRepresentative Heuristic nBase Rate Fallacy nStereotypes as Base Rates nDilution Effect nOther Cognitive.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
C81COG: Cognitive Psychology 1 PROBABILISTIC REASONING Dr. Alastair D. Smith Room B22 – School of Psychology
Advertisements

Do you think Research in psychology is Important? Why or why not?
Testing Theories: Three Reasons Why Data Might not Match the Theory.
Lecture Outline Heuristics Heuristics and Social Influence Types of heuristics Stereotypes as base rates Dilution Effect Other cognitive errors.
1 Intuitive Irrationality: Reasons for Unreason. 2 Epistemology Branch of philosophy focused on how people acquire knowledge about the world Descriptive.
© POSbase 2005 The Conjunction Fallacy Please read the following scenario: (by Tversky & Kahneman, 1983)Tversky & Kahneman, 1983 Linda is 31 years old,
Fallacies in Probability Judgment Yuval Shahar M.D., Ph.D. Judgment and Decision Making in Information Systems.
Judgment and Decisions
Thinking, Deciding and Problem Solving
Reasoning What is the difference between deductive and inductive reasoning? What are heuristics, and how do we use them? How do we reason about categories?
Judgment in Managerial Decision Making 8e Chapter 3 Common Biases
Solved the Maze? Start at phil’s house. At first, you can only make right turns through the maze. Each time you cross the red zigzag sign (under Carl’s.
Decision-making II judging the likelihood of events.
Heuristics and Biases. Normative Model Bayes rule tells you how you should reason with probabilities – it is a normative model But do people reason like.
Decision-making II judging the likelihood of events.
Reasoning with Uncertainty. Often, we want to reason from observable information to unobservable information We want to calculate how our prior beliefs.
Inductive Reasoning Bayes Rule. Urn problem (1) A B A die throw determines from which urn to select balls. For outcomes 1,2, and 3, balls are picked from.
Decision Making and Reasoning
Cognitive Processes PSY 334 Chapter 10 – Reasoning & Decision-Making August 21, 2003.
Cognitive - reasoning.ppt © 2001 Laura Snodgrass, Ph.D.1 Reasoning and Decision Making Five general strategies Reasoning and Logic Two hypotheses –inherently.
Copyright 2010 McGraw-Hill Companies
Heuristics & Biases. Bayes Rule Prior Beliefs Evidence Posterior Probability.
Decision Making. Test Yourself: Decision Making and the Availability Heuristic 1) Which is a more likely cause of death in the United States: being killed.
Copyright © 2000 by Harcourt, Inc. All rights reserved. What is Perception? Perception: The process of recognizing and understanding others By understanding.
Rank how likely is it that…
Good thinking or gut feeling
Understanding Probability and Long-Term Expectations Example from Student - Kalyani Thampi I have an example of "chance" that I thought about mentioning.
Inductive Generalizations Induction is the basis for our commonsense beliefs about the world. In the most general sense, inductive reasoning, is that in.
Chapter 10 Thinking.
Assumes that events are governed by some lawful order
Lecture 15 – Decision making 1 Decision making occurs when you have several alternatives and you choose among them. There are two characteristics of good.
Chapter 4 Perceiving Persons.
LESSON TWO ECONOMIC RATIONALITY Subtopic 10 – Statistical Reasoning Created by The North Carolina School of Science and Math forThe North Carolina School.
Judgement Judgement We change our opinion of the likelihood of something in light of new information. Example:  Do you think.
Past research in decision making has shown that when solving certain types of probability estimation problems, groups tend to exacerbate errors commonly.
PSY 323 – Cognition Chapter 13: Judgment, Decisions & Reasoning.
Exercise 2-6: Ecological fallacy. Exercise 2-7: Regression artefact: Lord’s paradox.
1 DECISION MAKING Suppose your patient (from the Brazilian rainforest) has tested positive for a rare but serious disease. Treatment exists but is risky.
Representativeness Heuristic Then: Framing Effects Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 6/2 /2015: Lecture 10-2 This Powerpoint.
Decision Making. Reasoning & Problem Solving A. Two Classes of Reasoning I. Deductive Reasoning II. Inductive Reasoning.
Probability Judgments: Overview I  Heuristics and Basis:  Availabilty heuristic  Representativeness heuristic.  Anchoring and Adjustment.
Lecture Outline Heuristics and Social Inference Representative Heuristic Base Rate Fallacy Stereotypes as Base Rates Dilution Effect Other Cognitive Errors.
Lesson 3 Measurement and Scaling. Case: “What is performance?” brandesign.co.za.
A. Judgment Heuristics Definition: Rule of thumb; quick decision guide When are heuristics used? - When making intuitive judgments about relative likelihoods.
Heuristics and Biases Thomas R. Stewart, Ph.D. Center for Policy Research Rockefeller College of Public Affairs and Policy University at Albany State University.
The Representativeness Heuristic then: Risk Attitude and Framing Effects Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 6/1/2016: Lecture.
Cognitive model of stereotype change: Hewstone & Johnston
Experimental Research
Foundations of Science
Exercise 2-7: Regression artefact: Lord’s paradox
Unit 7 Part II: Cognition
Cognition: Thinking and Language
Inference and Tests of Hypotheses
Chapter 4 Perceiving Persons.
RELIABILITY OF QUANTITATIVE & QUALITATIVE RESEARCH TOOLS
Understanding Results
Skepticism and Empiricism in Psychology
Pavle Valerjev Marin Dujmović
1st: Representativeness Heuristic and Conjunction Errors 2nd: Risk Attitude and Framing Effects Psychology 355:
Inferential statistics,
% Positive/Neutral Attributions
Chapter Eight: Quantitative Methods
Starter Imagine - you did not do as well as you wanted to in a biology test, but your teacher praises you for working hard and trying your best. You feel.
Conceptions and Misconceptions
Correlational Studies
An Interactive Discussion: Contemporary Research on IS Auditors and Automated Controls Dr. Daniel Selby University Of Richmond ISACA VA January 20, 2011.
Decision Making and Reasoning
HEURISTICS.
The Research Process & Surveys, Samples, and Populations
Science of Psychology WHS AP Psychology
Presentation transcript:

Lecture Outline nHeuristics and Social Inference nRepresentative Heuristic nBase Rate Fallacy nStereotypes as Base Rates nDilution Effect nOther Cognitive Errors

Heuristics Definition: Rules or principles that allow people to make social inferences rapidly and with reduced effort nmental shortcuts nrules of thumb

Social Inference Social Inferences: 3 components 1. Determine (ir)relevant information 2. Sample social information 3. Combine and integrate information

History of Cognitive Errors Up to 1960’s nPeople use formal statistical rules to make social inferences Around 1960 nPeople use formal statistical rules, but imperfectly After 1960 nPeople don’t use formal statistical rules at all

Kahneman & Tversky Proposed 3 main ideas: 1. People rely on heuristics to make social inferences 2. Heuristics simplify the process of making social inferences 3. Heuristics sometimes lead to faulty reasoning

Kahneman & Tversky Major shift in thinking: nResearchers began to focus on people’s weaknesses

Representative Heuristic Definition: categorizations made on the basis of similarity between instance and category members If you guessed that Jack was an engineer, you fell prey to the Representative Heuristic!

Is using this heuristic always bad? NO An instance that IS a category member will share features with other category members. Here it works! But……………. Representative Heuristic

Similarity does not ensure category membership Representative Heuristic Category People romantically interested in you Features of Category Members Talk with you when together Laugh at your jokes Features of New Instance Talks with you when together Laughs at your jokes

Relying solely on similarity will often lead to incorrect categorizations

Kahneman & Tversky (1973) Purpose: 1. Show that people use the representative heuristic to make social inferences 2. Show that people fall prey to the “Base Rate Fallacy”

Base Rate Fallacy Definition: when people do not take prior probabilities into account when making social inferences. Example of base rate: n50% of babies are girls n50% are boys If you guessed your chances of having a girl is 65%, you are not using base rates to make your judgment

Kahneman & Tversky (1973) Procedure: 1. Participants given following instructions: A panel of psychologists interviewed and administered personality tests to engineers and lawyers, all successful in their respective fields. On the basis of this information, thumbnail descriptions of the engineers and lawyers have been written written. You will find on your form a description, chosen at random from the 100 available descriptions. Please indicate your probability that the person described is an engineer, on a scale from 0 to 100.

Kahneman & Tversky (1973) Manipulation: Prior probability (base rate) n1/2 participants told of the 100 –70% engineers –30% lawyers n1/2 participants told of the 100 –30% engineers –70% lawyers

Kahneman & Tversky (1973) Description: Jack is a 45-year old man. He is married and has four children. He is generally conservative, careful, and ambitious. He shows no interest in political and social issues and spends most of his free time on his many hobbies which include home carpentry, sailing, and mathematical puzzles. What is the likelihood that Jack is an engineer (0 to 100%).

Kahneman & Tversky (1973) Competing Predictions: 1. People use the representative heuristic to make social inferences nInferences will be based solely on similarity of target to category members nBase rates (70%-30%) will be ignored

Kahneman & Tversky (1973) Competing Predictions: 2. People use formal statistical rules to make social inferences nInferences will be based on similarity of target to category members AND base rates (70%-30%)

Kahneman & Tversky (1973) Results: nParticipants in the 30% condition judged Jack just as likely to be an engineer as participants in the 70% condition. Which prediction does this support? Why?

Kahneman & Tversky (1973) Conclusions: nPeople use the representative heuristic when making social inferences nPeople do not use base rates when making social inferences

Kahneman & Tversky (1973) Question: Might it be that people did not use the base rates (70%-30%) because they did not know they were important?

Kahneman & Tversky (1973) Answer: No. When asked: “Suppose that you are given no information whatsoever about an individual chosen at random from the sample. What is the probability that this man is one of the engineers? Result: People used base rates when given no case information

Kahneman & Tversky (1973) Conclusion #2: People use base rates when no case information is given People do not use base rates when case information IS given

Stereotypes as Base Rates Definition of Stereotypes: Generalized beliefs about the attributes that characterize members of a social group Example: women tend to be passive men tend to be assertive

Stereotypes as Base Rates Kahneman & Tversky’s study showed that base rates only influenced social inferences in the ABSENCE of case information Locksley et al. (1980) wanted to see if the same is true for stereotypes.

Locksley, Borgida, Hepburn, Ortiz (1980) Purpose: Test whether stereotypes act as base rates Stereotype under investigation: Men are more assertive than women

Locksley et al. (1980) Predictions: When case information absent: nsex stereotypes bias judgments of assertiveness When case information present: nsex stereotypes do not bias judgments of assertiveness

Locksley et al. (1980) Procedures: Step 1: Participants read about 6 targets Step 2: Participants rated each target’s assertiveness “How often person behaves assertively in daily life” ( % of the time)

Locksley et al. (1980) Targets: 2 Targets = name only (Susan and Paul)

Locksley et al. (1980) Targets: 2 Targets = name plus case information that was diagnostic of assertiveness See example next slide………...

Locksley et al. (1980) Example: Diagnostic case information The other day Nancy was in a class in which she wanted to make several points about the readings being discussed. But another student was dominating the class discussion so thoroughly that she had to abruptly interrupt this student in order to break into the discussion and express her own views

Locksley et al. (1980) Targets: 2 Targets = name plus case information that was non-diagnostic of assertiveness See example next slide………...

Locksley et al. (1980) Example: Non-diagnostic case information Yesterday Tom went to get his hair cut. He had an early morning appointment because he had classes that day. Since the place where he gets his hair cut is near campus, he had no trouble getting to class on time

Locksley et al. (1980) Judgments of Assertiveness

Locksley et al. (1980) Conclusion: nDiagnostic case information reduces people’s reliance on base rates nNon-diagnostic information does not reduce people’s reliance on base rates

Dilution Effect Locksley’s study is not consistent with the dilution effect Definition: non-diagnostic information weakens the effect of base rates on social inferences

Recap Diagnostic information: information that is relevant to a judgment GPA is diagnostic of success in graduate school

Recap Non-Diagnostic information: information that is irrelevant to a judgment Eating pizza for dinner is non- diagnostic of success in graduate school

Nisbett, Zukier, & Lemley (1981) Purpose: Demonstrate that non- diagnostic information reduces effect of stereotypes on judgments

Nisbett et al. (1981) Pilot Study: nAssessed stereotypes of college majors. nEngineering majors tolerate more electrical shock than music major

Nisbett et al. (1981) Main Study: nStep 1: Read study about pain suppressant nStep 2: Read vignette of two people in pain suppressant study nStep 3: rate how much shock each tolerated in the the study

Nisbett et al. (1981) Participants from Pain Study nEngineering major nMusic major Manipulation nMajor only nMajor plus non-diagnostic information

Nisbett et al. (1981) Prediction Major only: nbig difference in shock tolerance, with engineer tolerating more Major plus non-diagnostic information: nsmall or no difference in shock tolerance

Nisbett et al., 1980 (E rating - M rating) Total Rating Note: The taller the bar, the more stereotypes influenced judgments of shock tolerance

Locksley et al. Vs. Nisbett et al. Opposite results: nLocksley: non-diagnostic information does NOT weaken stereotyping nNisbett: non-diagnostic information DOES weak stereotyping

Locksley et al. Vs. Nisbett et al. What is going on? What may have caused the discrepancy?

Locksley et al. Vs. Nisbett et al. Answer: It may be something about the non-diagnostic information

Locksley et al. Vs. Nisbett et al. Locksley: Non-diagnostic = generally useless nGot a hair cut Nisbett: Non-diagnostic = generally useful nParent’s occupation

Hilton & Fein, 1989 Clearly-irrelevant information nnot diagnostic of particular judgment, and nnot diagnostic of judgments in general

Hilton & Fein, 1989 Pseudo-irrelevant information nnot diagnostic of particular judgment, but ndiagnostic of judgments in general

Hilton & Fein, 1989 Purpose: Test whether this distinction can reconcile discrepant results Pilot Study: Assessed stereotypes of college majors nPre-med majors more competitive than social work majors

Hilton & Fein, 1989 Main Study: Step 1: Participants read about Bill H. Step 2: Participants rated his assertiveness

Hilton & Fein, 1989 Manipulations: 1. College major: lpre-med lsocial work 2. Type of information lclearly irrelevant lpseudo-irrelevant

Hilton & Fein, 1989 Predictions: 1. Clearly-irrelevant information will NOT weaken stereotyping 2. Pseudo-irrelevant information WILL weaken stereotyping

Results Judgments of Competitiveness

Hilton & Fein, 1989 Conclusion: Pseudo-irrelevant information dilutes stereotyping, but clearly-irrelevant information does not This clears up the discrepancy

Summary People use prior probabilities when: nno case information given nor, clearly-irrelevant case information given People do not use prior probabilities when: ndiagnostic case information given nor, pseudo-irrelevant case information given

Other Cognitive Errors and Biases Sample size nFailure to take sample size into account when making social inferences Pop. = 1000N 1 = 900N 2 = 20

Other Cognitive Errors and Biases Regression Observed score = true ability + chance nWhenever scores are influenced by chance observed scores will over- or underestimate one’s true ability

Other Cognitive Errors and Biases Regression: People don’t realize that……. nVery high observed score lower next time nVery low observed score higher next time

Other Cognitive Errors and Biases Conjunction Fallacy nFalse belief that two events have greater chance of co-occurring than either event by itself

Kahneman & Tversky (1983) Conjunction Fallacy Linda is 31 years old, single, outspoken, and very bright. She majored in philosophy. As a student, she was deeply concerned with issues of discrimination and social justice, and also participated in anti--nuclear demonstrations. Which of the following alternatives is more probable? A) Linda is a bank teller B) Linda is a bank teller and active in the feminist movement

Kahneman & Tversky (1983) Conjunction Fallacy Most participants picked B If you picked B, you have fallen prey to the conjunction fallacy It is not possible for two events to be more probable than one of the events by itself

Other Cognitive Errors and Biases Assessing Covariation: Illusory Correlations Kelly’s Co-variation Model nTwo events must co-vary to be seen as cause-effect Steps of detecting Co-variation

Other Cognitive Errors and Biases Illusory correlation nWhen people overestimate how strongly two events are correlated nOccur when one or more steps needed to assess co-variation goes wrong

Other Cognitive Errors and Biases What might go wrong? nBiased Sample People often fail to realize that their sample is biased (Morning vs. Evening People)

Other Cognitive Errors and Biases What might go wrong? nConfirmation biases in hypothesis testing People often seek information that confirms rather than disconfirms their original hypothesis

Redelmeier & Tversky (1996) Common Belief: Arthritis associated with changes in weather nFollowed 18 arthritis patients for 15 months n2 x per month assessed: lpain and joint tenderness lweather nCorrelated pain/tenderness with weather

Redelmeier & Tversky (1996) Results: nCorrelation between pain/tenderness and weather near ZERO!!! nPatients saw correlation that did not exist nWhy? Confirmation biases in hypothesis testing………..

Redelmeier & Tversky (1996) Confirmation biases in hypothesis testing nPaid special attention to those times when great pain coincided with weather changes nDid not notice when great pain did not coincide with weather changes

Other Cognitive Errors and Biases Availability Heuristic nTendency for people to make judgments of frequency on basis of how easily examples come to mind. (Alcohol and drug use by college students)

Other Cognitive Errors and Biases Availability Heuristic nWorks when frequency correlated with ease of coming up with examples nBut, sometimes frequency not correlated with ease of coming up with examples

Tversky & Kahneman (1973) Availability Heuristic nAsked participants: “Is letter R more likely to be the 1st or 3rd letter in English words? nMost said R more probable as 1st letter nReality: R appears much more often as the 3rd letter, but easier to think of words where R is 1st letter