Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

LING 581: Advanced Computational Linguistics Lecture Notes January 12th.

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "LING 581: Advanced Computational Linguistics Lecture Notes January 12th."— Presentation transcript:

1 LING 581: Advanced Computational Linguistics Lecture Notes January 12th

2 Course Webpage for lecture slides –http://dingo.sbs.arizona.edu/~sandiway/ling581- 12/http://dingo.sbs.arizona.edu/~sandiway/ling581- 12/ Meeting information

3 Course Objectives Follow-on course to LING/C SC/PSYC 438/538 Computational Linguistics: –continue with J&M: 25 chapters, a lot of material not covered in 438/538 And gain project experience –dealing with natural language software packages –Installation, input data formatting –operation –project exercises –useful “real-world” computational experience –abilities gained will be of value to employers

4 Computational Facilities Advise using your own laptop/desktop –we can also make use of this computer lab but you don’t have installation rights on these computers Platforms Windows is possible but you really should run some variant of Unix… (your task #1 for this week) –Linux (separate bootable partition or via virtualization software) de facto standard for advanced/research software https://www.virtualbox.org/ (free!)https://www.virtualbox.org/ –Cygwin on Windows http://www.cygwin.com/ Linux-like environment for Windows making it possible to port software running on POSIX systems (such as Linux, BSD, and Unix systems) to Windows. –MacOS X Not quite Linux, some porting issues, especially with C programs, can use Virtual Box

5 Grading Completion of all homework task and course project will result in a satisfactory grade (A)

6 Homework Task 1: Install Tregex Runs in java

7 Last semester 438/538: Language models

8 Language Models and N-grams given a word sequence –w 1 w 2 w 3... w n chain rule –how to compute the probability of a sequence of words –p(w 1 w 2 ) = p(w 1 ) p(w 2 |w 1 ) –p(w 1 w 2 w 3 ) = p(w 1 ) p(w 2 |w 1 ) p(w 3 |w 1 w 2 ) –... –p(w 1 w 2 w 3...w n ) = p(w 1 ) p(w 2 |w 1 ) p(w 3 |w 1 w 2 )... p(w n |w 1...w n-2 w n-1 ) note –It’s not easy to collect (meaningful) statistics on p(w n |w n-1 w n-2...w 1 ) for all possible word sequences

9 Language Models and N-grams Given a word sequence –w 1 w 2 w 3... w n Bigram approximation –just look at the previous word only (not all the proceedings words) –Markov Assumption: finite length history –1st order Markov Model –p(w 1 w 2 w 3...w n ) = p(w 1 ) p(w 2 |w 1 ) p(w 3 |w 1 w 2 )...p(w n |w 1...w n-3 w n-2 w n-1 ) –p(w 1 w 2 w 3...w n )  p(w 1 ) p(w 2 |w 1 ) p(w 3 |w 2 )...p(w n |w n-1 ) note –p(w n |w n-1 ) is a lot easier to collect data for (and thus estimate well) than p(w n |w 1...w n-2 w n-1 )

10 Language Models and N-grams Trigram approximation –2nd order Markov Model –just look at the preceding two words only –p(w 1 w 2 w 3 w 4...w n ) = p(w 1 ) p(w 2 |w 1 ) p(w 3 |w 1 w 2 ) p(w 4 |w 1 w 2 w 3 )...p(w n |w 1...w n-3 w n-2 w n-1 ) –p(w 1 w 2 w 3...w n )  p(w 1 ) p(w 2 |w 1 ) p(w 3 |w 1 w 2 )p(w 4 |w 2 w 3 )...p(w n |w n-2 w n-1 ) note –p(w n |w n-2 w n-1 ) is a lot easier to estimate well than p(w n |w 1...w n-2 w n-1 ) but harder than p(w n |w n-1 )

11 Language Models and N-grams estimating from corpora –how to compute bigram probabilities –p(w n |w n-1 ) = f(w n-1 w n )/f(w n-1 w)w is any word –Since f(w n-1 w) = f(w n-1 ) f(w n-1 ) = unigram frequency for w n-1 –p(w n |w n-1 ) = f(w n-1 w n )/f(w n-1 )relative frequency Note: –The technique of estimating (true) probabilities using a relative frequency measure over a training corpus is known as maximum likelihood estimation (MLE)

12 Motivation for smoothing Smoothing: avoid zero probability estimates Consider what happens when any individual probability component is zero? –Arithmetic multiplication law: 0×X = 0 –very brittle! even in a very large corpus, many possible n-grams over vocabulary space will have zero frequency –particularly so for larger n-grams p(w 1 w 2 w 3...w n )  p(w 1 ) p(w 2 |w 1 ) p(w 3 |w 2 )...p(w n |w n-1 )

13 Language Models and N-grams Example: unigram frequencies w n-1 w n bigram frequencies bigram probabilities sparse matrix zeros render probabilities unusable (we’ll need to add fudge factors - i.e. do smoothing) w n-1 wnwn

14 Smoothing and N-grams sparse dataset means zeros are a problem –Zero probabilities are a problem p(w 1 w 2 w 3...w n )  p(w 1 ) p(w 2 |w 1 ) p(w 3 |w 2 )...p(w n |w n-1 ) bigram model one zero and the whole product is zero –Zero frequencies are a problem p(w n |w n-1 ) = f(w n-1 w n )/f(w n-1 )relative frequency bigram f(w n-1 w n ) doesn’t exist in dataset smoothing –refers to ways of assigning zero probability n-grams a non-zero value

15 Smoothing and N-grams Add-One Smoothing (4.5.1 Laplace Smoothing) –add 1 to all frequency counts –simple and no more zeros (but there are better methods) unigram –p(w) = f(w)/N(before Add-One) N = size of corpus –p(w) = (f(w)+1)/(N+V)(with Add-One) –f*(w) = (f(w)+1)*N/(N+V)(with Add-One) V = number of distinct words in corpus N/(N+V) normalization factor adjusting for the effective increase in the corpus size caused by Add-One bigram –p(w n |w n-1 ) = f(w n-1 w n )/f(w n-1 )(before Add-One) –p(w n |w n-1 ) = (f(w n-1 w n )+1)/(f(w n-1 )+V)(after Add-One) –f*(w n-1 w n ) = (f(w n-1 w n )+1)* f(w n-1 ) /(f(w n-1 )+V)(after Add-One) must rescale so that total probability mass stays at 1

16 Smoothing and N-grams Add-One Smoothing –add 1 to all frequency counts bigram –p(w n |w n-1 ) = (f(w n-1 w n )+1)/(f(w n-1 )+V) –(f(w n-1 w n )+1)* f(w n-1 ) /(f(w n-1 )+V) frequencies Remarks: perturbation problem add-one causes large changes in some frequencies due to relative size of V (1616) want to: 786  338 = figure 6.8 = figure 6.4

17 Smoothing and N-grams Add-One Smoothing –add 1 to all frequency counts bigram –p(w n |w n-1 ) = (f(w n-1 w n )+1)/(f(w n-1 )+V) –(f(w n-1 w n )+1)* f(w n-1 ) /(f(w n-1 )+V) Probabilities Remarks: perturbation problem similar changes in probabilities = figure 6.5 = figure 6.7

18 Smoothing and N-grams let’s illustrate the problem –take the bigram case: –w n-1 w n –p(w n |w n-1 ) = f(w n-1 w n )/f(w n-1 ) –suppose there are cases –w n-1 w 0 1 that don’t occur in the corpus probability mass f(w n-1 ) f(w n-1 w n ) f(w n-1 w 0 1 )=0 f(w n-1 w 0 m )=0...

19 Smoothing and N-grams add-one –“give everyone 1” probability mass f(w n-1 ) f(w n-1 w n )+1 f(w n-1 w 0 1 )=1 f(w n-1 w 0 m )=1...

20 Smoothing and N-grams add-one –“give everyone 1” probability mass f(w n-1 ) f(w n-1 w n )+1 f(w n-1 w 0 1 )=1 f(w n-1 w 0 m )=1... V = |{w i )| redistribution of probability mass –p(w n |w n-1 ) = (f(w n- 1 w n )+1)/(f(w n-1 )+V)

21 Smoothing and N-grams Excel spreadsheet available –addone.xls

22 Smoothing and N-grams Good-Turing Discounting (4.5.2) –N c = number of things (= n-grams) that occur c times in the corpus –N = total number of things seen –Formula: smoothed c for N c given by c* = (c+1)N c+1 /N c –Idea: use frequency of things seen once to estimate frequency of things we haven’t seen yet –estimate N 0 in terms of N 1 … –Formula: P*(things with zero freq) = N 1 /N –smaller impact than Add-One Textbook Example: –Fishing in lake with 8 species –Bass, carp, catfish, eel, perch, salmon, trout, whitefish –Sample data: –10 carp, 3 perch, 2 whitefish, 1 trout, 1 salmon, 1 eel –P(new fish unseen) = 3/18 –P(next fish=trout) = 1/18 (but, we have reassigned probability mass…) –C*(trout) = 2*N 2 /N 1 =2(1/3)=0.67 (discounted from 1)

23 Language Models and N-grams N-gram models –they’re technically easy to compute (in the sense that lots of training data are available) –but just how good are these n-gram language models? –and what can they show us about language?

24 Language Models and N-grams approximating Shakespeare –generate random sentences using n-grams –train on complete Works of Shakespeare Unigram (pick random, unconnected words) Bigram

25 Language Models and N-grams Approximating Shakespeare (section 6.2) –generate random sentences using n-grams –train on complete Works of Shakespeare Trigram Quadrigram Remarks: dataset size problem training set is small 884,647 words 29,066 different words 29,066 2 = 844,832,356 possible bigrams for the random sentence generator, this means very limited choices for possible continuations, which means program can’t be very innovative for higher n

26 Language Models and N-grams Aside: http://hemispheresmagazine.com/contests/2004/intro.htm

27 Language Models and N-grams N-gram models + smoothing –one consequence of smoothing is that –every possible concatentation or sequence of words has a non-zero probability

28 Colorless green ideas examples –(1) colorless green ideas sleep furiously –(2) furiously sleep ideas green colorless Chomsky (1957): –... It is fair to assume that neither sentence (1) nor (2) (nor indeed any part of these sentences) has ever occurred in an English discourse. Hence, in any statistical model for grammaticalness, these sentences will be ruled out on identical grounds as equally `remote' from English. Yet (1), though nonsensical, is grammatical, while (2) is not. idea –(1) is syntactically valid, (2) is word salad Statistical Experiment (Pereira 2002)

29 Colorless green ideas examples –(1) colorless green ideas sleep furiously –(2) furiously sleep ideas green colorless Statistical Experiment (Pereira 2002) wiwi w i-1 bigram language model

30 example –colorless green ideas sleep furiously First hit Interesting things to Google

31 example –colorless green ideas sleep furiously first hit –compositional semantics – a green idea is, according to well established usage of the word "green" is one that is an idea that is new and untried. –again, a colorless idea is one without vividness, dull and unexciting. –so it follows that a colorless green idea is a new, untried idea that is without vividness, dull and unexciting. –to sleep is, among other things, is to be in a state of dormancy or inactivity, or in a state of unconsciousness. –to sleep furiously may seem a puzzling turn of phrase but one reflects that the mind in sleep often indeed moves furiously with ideas and images flickering in and out.

32 Interesting things to Google example –colorless green ideas sleep furiously another hit: (a story) –"So this is our ranking system," said Chomsky. "As you can see, the highest rank is yellow." –"And the new ideas?" –"The green ones? Oh, the green ones don't get a color until they've had some seasoning. These ones, anyway, are still too angry. Even when they're asleep, they're furious. We've had to kick them out of the dormitories - they're just unmanageable." –"So where are they?" –"Look," said Chomsky, and pointed out of the window. There below, on the lawn, the colorless green ideas slept, furiously.


Download ppt "LING 581: Advanced Computational Linguistics Lecture Notes January 12th."

Similar presentations


Ads by Google