Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Genetic Meta-Analysis and Mendelian Randomization George Davey Smith MRC Centre for Causal Analyses in Translational Epidemiology, University of Bristol.

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Genetic Meta-Analysis and Mendelian Randomization George Davey Smith MRC Centre for Causal Analyses in Translational Epidemiology, University of Bristol."— Presentation transcript:

1 Genetic Meta-Analysis and Mendelian Randomization George Davey Smith MRC Centre for Causal Analyses in Translational Epidemiology, University of Bristol

2 RCT vs Observational Meta- Analysis: fundamental difference in assumptions In meta-analysis of observational studies confounding, residual confounding and bias: –May introduce heterogeneity –May lead to misleading (albeit very precise) estimates

3 Relative risk (95% confidence interval) 0.10.20.512510 Trial (Year) Barber (1967) Reynolds (1972) Wilhelmsson (1974) Ahlmark (1974) Multicentre International (1975) Yusuf (1979) Andersen (1979) Rehnqvist (1980) Baber (1980) Wilcox Atenolol (1980) Wilcox Propanolol (1980) Hjalmarson (1981) Norwegian Multicentre (1981) Hansteen (1982) Julian (1982) BHAT (1982) Taylor (1982) Manger Cats (1983) Rehnqvist (1983) Australian-Swedish (1983) Mazur (1984) EIS (1984) Salathia (1985) Roque (1987) LIT 91987) Kaul (1988) Boissel (1990) Schwartz low risk (1992) Schwartz high risk (1992) SSSD (1993) Darasz (1995) Basu (1997) Aronow (1997) Overall (95% CI) 0.80 (0.74 - 0.86) Mortality results from 33 trials of beta-blockers in secondary prevention after myocardial infarction Adapted from Freemantle et al BMJ 1999

4 Results from 29 studies examining the association between intact foreskin and the risk of HIV infection in men Adapted from Van Howe Int J STD AIDS 1999

5 Vitamin E supplement use and risk of Coronary Heart Disease Stampfer et al NEJM 1993; 328: 144-9; Rimm et al NEJM 1993; 328: 1450-6; Eidelman et al Arch Intern Med 2004; 164:1552-6 1.0

6 Genetic meta-analysis, while of observational data, may be analogous to RCT meta-analysis NOT conventional observational meta-analysis

7 Clustered environments and randomised genes (93 phenotypes, 23 SNPs) Phenotype / phenotype 4278 pairwise associations Phenotype / genotype 2139 pairwise combinations Genotype / genotype 253 pairwise combinations 43% significant at p<0.01 20 significant at p<0.01 vs 21 expected 4 / 253 significant at p<0.01 vs 3 expected Davey Smith et al. PLoS Medicine 2007 in press

8 WTCCC: blood donors versus 1958 birth cohort controls

9 A leading epidemiologist speaks … Forget what you learnt at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine …. just get as many cases as possible and a bunch of controls from wherever you can.. Paul McKeigue, Nov 2002

10 Or the polite version … This approach allows geneticists to focus on collecting large numbers of cases and controls at low cost, without the strict population-based sampling protocols that are required to minimize selection bias in case-control studies of environmental exposures Am J Human Genetics 2003;72:1492-1504

11 If not confounding or selection bias, why have genetic association studies such a poor history of replication?

12 Are genetic association studies replicable? Hirschhorn et al reviewed 166 putative associations for which there were 3 or more published studies and found that only 6 had been consistently replicated (defined as achieving statistically significant findings in 75% or more of published studies) Hirschhorn JN et al. Genetics in Medicine 2002;4:45-61

13 Reasons for inconsistent genotype – disease associations True variation Variation of allelic association between subpopulations Effect modification by other genetic or environmental factors that vary between populations Spurious variation Misclassification of phenotype Confounding by population structure Lack of power Chance Publication bias Colhoun et al, Lancet 2003;361:865-72

14 True variation in genotype and health outcome between populations Disease-causing allele is in LD with a different allele at the marker locus in different groups Allelic heterogeneity (different variants within the same gene) between ethnic groups More likely when disease-causing variant is rare or has been subject to selection pressure The association is modified by other genetic or environmental factors that vary between the groups studied Effect modification by genes unlikely to account for failure to replicate studies in similar populations. Modification by environmental factors more likely, especially when absolute risk of disease varies

15 Differential misclassification of genotypesAvoided by appropriate laboratory procedures Differential misclassification of outcome: possible if genotype is known when outcome is classified Unlikely, because outcome is usually confirmed in advance of genotyping Biases vary between studies

16 Population is divided into strata that vary by disease risk and by allele frequencies at the marker locus Unlikely to be a serious problem in most studies: when confounding is a problem, it can be controlled in study design by restriction or use of family-based controls, or in the analysis by quantifying and controlling for substructure Confounding by population substructure

17 Case mix heterogeneity in an apparently homogenous outcome between populations studied: for instance in a study of stroke, mix of haemorrhagic and thrombotic subtypes may vary between populations Unlikely to be an explanation for failure to replicate studies in similar populations with similar case sampling strategies Case-mix heterogeneity

18 Failure to consider that the initial effect size reported is an inflation of the true effect size Replication studies should be powered to detect effect sizes that are smaller than the initial effect size reported, especially when the initial study had low power Absence of power leading to false-negative results and failure to replicate

19 The Beavis effect If the location of a variant and its phenotypic effect size are estimated from the same data sets, the effect size will be over-estimated, in many cases substantially. Statistical significance and the estimated magnitude of the parameter are highly correlated. H Göring et al. Am J Hum Genetics 2001;69:1357-69

20 Multiple testing and publication bias: multiple loci are assessed in each study, many statistical tests are done, and multiple studies are undertaken but only positive results are reported Perhaps the most likely reason for failure to replicate? False positive results by chance in initial positive studies

21 What is being associated in genetic association studies? Estimates of 15M SNPs in human genome (rare allele frequency >1% in at least one population) Large number of outcomes (diseases and subcategories of particular disease outcomes) Large number of potential subgroups Multiple possible genetic contrasts

22 Polymorphism really is associated with disease Polymorphism is not associated with disease Total Result of experiment Association declared to exist 8045125 Association not declared to exist 20855 875 Total1009001000 What percentage of associations that are studied actually exist? … 1 in 10? (at 80% power, 5% significance level) Oakes 1986; Davey Smith 1998; Sterne & Davey Smith 2001

23 Power of study (% of time we reject null hypothesis if it is false) P=0.05P=0.01P=0.001 2069.231.04.3 5047.415.31.8 8036.010.11.1 Percentage of significant results that are false positives if 10% of studied associations actually exist Sterne & Davey Smith BMJ 2001;322:226-231

24 Power of study (% of time we reject null hypothesis if it is false) P=0.05P=0.01P=0.001 2096.183.233.1 5090.866.416.5 8086.155.311.0 Percentage of significant results that are false positives if 1% of studied associations actually exist Sterne & Davey Smith. BMJ 2001;322:226-231

25 P values often misinterpreted in both genetic and conventional epidemiology Low prior probability major issue in genetic epidemiology; meaningless (but real) associations a major issue in conventional epidemiology

26 Why has replication proved to be so difficult? LOW STATISTICAL POWER A consistent feature of almost all analyses Fundamental to many of the explanations or the approach needed to correct for them If we need 5,000 cases to test for a given aetiological effect with a power of 80%, and with a critical p-value of 0.0001, how much power would there be for a study with 500 cases?

27 Why has replication proved to be so difficult? LOW STATISTICAL POWER!! A key feature of almost all proffered explanations, and/or of the approach needed to correct for them If we need 5,000 cases to test for a given aetiological effect with a power of 80%, and with a critical p-value of 0.0001, how much power would there be for a study with 500 cases? 0.008

28 Deducing true numerical ratios requires the greatest possible number of individual values; and the greater the number of these the more effectively will mere chance be eliminated. Gregor Mendel 1865/6

29 Association of GNB3 and Hypertension Bagos et al, J Hypertens March 2007 34 Studies Cases = 14,094 Controls = 17,760 Total = 21,654

30 ¿ | α β γ | A B C | a b c | ?

31 Are genetic associations studies replicable: take two? Joel Hirschhorns group selected 25 of the 166 genetic associations that they had studied and performed formal meta-analysis, claiming that 8 of these (one third) were robust. One third claim widely welcomed! Lohmueller KE et al. Nature Genetics 2003;33:177-182

32 Replicable Studies ABCC8, type 2 diabetes2.28 (1.27-4.10) COL1A1, fracture1.59 (1.36-1.86) CTLA4, type 1 diabetes1.27 (1.17-1.37) DRD3, schizophrenia1.12 (1.02-1.23) GSTM1, head/neck cancer1.20 (1.09-1.33) HTR2A, schizophrenia1.07 (1.01-1.14) PPARG, type 2 diabetes1.22 (1.08-1.37) SLC2A1, type 2 diabetes1.76 (1.35-2.31)

33 Are genetic associations studies replicable: take two? Low hanging fruit and a best-case scenario. Effect size estimates not so widely welcomed..

34 Science, June 1, 2007 All Studies Combined 14,585 cases 17,968 controls 1.17 1.12 1.13 1.20 1.12 1.14 1.12 1.37 1.14 TCF7

35 Nature, June 7, 2007 Distribution of ORs for 70 Common Disease Variants Odds Ratio %

36 for exposures with small effect sizes it is very difficult to exclude confounding and bias in conventional epidemiology, and level of statistical significance does not help statistical deviation from the null more important in genetic epidemiology

37 Mendel on Mendelian randomization the behaviour of each pair of differentiating characteristics in hybrid union is independent of the other differences between the two original plants, and, further, the hybrid produces just so many kinds of egg and pollen cells as there are possible constant combination forms (Sometimes called Mendels second law – the law of independent assortment) Gregor Mendel, 1865. Mendel in 1862

38 Mendelian randomization Genotypes can proxy for some modifiable environmental factors, and there should be no confounding of genotype by behavioural, socioeconomic or physiological factors (excepting those influenced by alleles at closely proximate loci or due to population stratification), no bias due to reverse causation, and lifetime exposure patterns can be captured

39

40 Mendelian randomisation and RCTs RANDOMISATION METHOD RANDOMISED CONTROLLED TRIAL CONFOUNDERS EQUAL BETWEEN GROUPS MENDELIAN RANDOMISATION RANDOM SEGREGATION OF ALLELES CONFOUNDERS EQUAL BETWEEN GROUPS EXPOSED: FUNCTIONAL ALLELLES EXPOSED: INTERVENTION CONTROL: NULL ALLELLES CONTROL: NO INTERVENTION OUTCOMES COMPARED BETWEEN GROUPS


Download ppt "Genetic Meta-Analysis and Mendelian Randomization George Davey Smith MRC Centre for Causal Analyses in Translational Epidemiology, University of Bristol."

Similar presentations


Ads by Google