Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Building a Lexicon Statistical learning & recognizing words.

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Building a Lexicon Statistical learning & recognizing words."— Presentation transcript:

1 Building a Lexicon Statistical learning & recognizing words

2 Overview More on segmentation: the role of statistics Using different cues to segmentation: Transitional probabilities vs. Stress cues Once words are pulled out and remembered, is lexical access mature? What are some steps that occur?

3 Word Segmentation In writing, blank spaces between words, but not in speech Once you know words, you can hear them But how do babies pull them out? Only some in isolation!

4 What have babies learned by 8- 10 months? Phonetic categories (position specific!) Phonotactics (consonant sequences allowed) Stress patterns (English predominantly Sw) Ability to use distributional information –A particular word in different contexts might pop out, e.g. –“the milk”, “drink your milk”, “do you want some milk” Can babies use these to help segment words?

5 Word Segmentation At 7.5 months only segment Sw words Succeed on words like DOCtor and HAMlet But fail on words like guiTAR and deVICE For WS words, seem to pull out only the strong syllable Indeed, if the Strong syllable is always followed by a particular function word, will consider it part of the word E.g. pulled out “TAR is”

6 Transitional Probabilities Why would infants pull out ‘Tar is”

7 Different types of statistics Frequency –the occurrence of an item, high vs low –Sw words are higher in frequency Distributional probabilities –the distribution of occurrences, bimodal vs. monomodal Transitional probabilities –the probability that one item will follow (or be followed by) another

8 Transitional probabilities and segmentation What are transitional probabilities? “ABCABCABC” “ABCBABCBA” 121424214 etc. The problem: Listeningtoenglishwhenyoudon’tknowwhereonew ordendsandthenextbeginsfeelsalittlelikethis. Listening to English when you don’t know where one word ends and the next begins feels a little like this.

9 Transitional probabilities and segmentation A sensitivity to transitional probabilities could be useful: –Lookattheprettybaby. –Whatalittlebaby! –Mybabygirliscrying. –Ilikemybagelwithcreamcheeseandlox. Probability of Ba+by is 3/4 If you’re trying to figure out the “Units” in speech, this might be a useful property

10 Transitional probabilities and segmentation Presented infants with a 2min sequence of syllables with no pauses: Bidakupadotigolabubidakugolabu... Test: golabu (word) vs tigola (partword) 1.0 0.33 Infants looked longer when the partwords were played showing that they discriminated between the two test items. Saffran et al., 1996

11 Transitional probabilities and language learning Infants can use the transition probabilities between segments to pull out candidate words from continuous English speech. At 8 months Maybe segmented words are part of prelexical representations

12 Which comes first? So – we’ve learned that infants can use stress cues to segment words And, they can use transitional probabilities to segment words Heard speech has both cues We never produce monotone utterances like Bidakupadotigolabubidakugolabu

13 It seems, that if we combine Stress cues and predictive transitional probabilities segmentation might be even easier But how could you learn stress cues if you didn’t already know some words? Johnson & Jusczyk (2001) suggest that infants learn English stress frequencies from hearing words in isolation Is this necessary? Not if infants use TPs first.

14 Thiessen & Saffran (Dev. Psych, 2003) tested this question They use the Saffran, et. al., type task, but added Stress cues Compared infants of 6-7 and 8-9 months on their ability to learn S-W vs. W-S words At 6-7 months, if the TP predicted a word, the infants were able to segment it whether it was SW or WS At 8-9 months, they only succeeded on SW words

15 To use the Saffran, et. al. example An infant of 6-7 months could learn either: –bidakupadotiGOlabubidakuGOlabu OR –bidakupadotigoLAbubidakugoLAbu But an infant of 8-9 months showed better learning of: –bidakupadotiGOlabubidakuGOlabu * This has been simplified from the way the study was actually done

16 Infants can use many different types of statistics to learn about words These include frequency, distributional probabilities, and transitional probabilities Different types of statistics might be important at different points in development for helping infants along the way

17 Is learning transitional probabilities specific for learning language? Tone sequences AFB, F#A#D, EGD#, CG#C# AFBF#A#DAFB EGD#CG#C# Test: AFB (“word”) vs. D#CG# (“partword”) Infants of 8 months looked longer when the partwords were played, showing that infants discriminated between the two test items. Saffran et al., 1999

18 How specific is this type of learning for language? Visual sequences Presented 1 object at a time, no pauses, objects were in pairs Kirkham et al. 2001

19 How specific is this type of learning for language? Test:(real pair) vs. (novel pair) Infants as young as 2 months looked longer at the “novel pairs” Kirkham et al. 2001

20 Are humans the only species sensitive to statistics? Birds are sensitive to distributions of vowels: can discriminate vowels when they are presented in a bimodal distribution Kluender et al., 1998 Cotton-top Tamarins (monkeys) heard the continuous nonsense words of Saffran et al., and responded the same way as infants, by looking longer at the part-words Hauser et al., 2001

21 Summary: statistical learning Human infants (and adults) can do it Humans can do it for language, but also for tones and for visual images! Birds and monkeys can do it too. Statistical learning seems to be a very basic learning mechanism that human infants use to help “bootstrap” into language.

22 But is that all word learning is? The “word” learning we have talked about so far this week and last, is really just about learning familiar word forms We need word forms But we need to learn word meanings as well Indeed, a full lexical entry has: –Word form –Word meaning (intension: definition) –Word meaning (extension: other objects it can apply to) –Grammatical class

23 The mature “lexicon” and language processing Your lexicon has all this information about every word you hear So, when you hear a sentence like, –“Get the leash so we can take the dog to the park” You’ll recognize the highly predictable word “dog” at the very beginning of the “d” The semantic context has been set up, you know a noun must come after “the”, and those two facts make you expect “dog”

24 This allows you to synthesize, process more efficiently, and look forward for what might be coming next in the sentence Imagine if I had said: –“Get the leash so we can take the drapes down”

25 Fernald, et. al. (1998) ask if infants lexicons are this mature from the beginning. They find they are not – but that they mature quickly –there are rapid gains in the speed of word recognition between 15 and 24 months of age –At 15 months, infants have to hear the whole word and have a chance to see both objects –By 24 months, they, like adults, are making a guess on the basis of the beginning sounds of the word

26 More on accessing the word form… So, by 24 months, infants need only part of the sound to access the whole word in the lexicon But this is just about accessing word form How about when infants start learning the meaning of words as well? To explore this, we tested infants at the very beginning stages of word meaning – an associative understanding of word-object linkages (not nec the same thing as referential understanding) We asked how easy it is for infants to access the full detail in the lexicon

27 Word-learning: Switch Design (Werker, Cohen, Lloyd, Casasola, & Stager, DevPsych, 1998)

28 Baby in the Switch Task

29 Infants succeed by 14 months Werker, Cohen, Lloyd, Casasola, & Stager, Dev. Psych. 1998

30 Learning Similar Words Stager & Werker, Nature 1997

31 Early Word Learning

32 Results: Similar sounding words Infants of 14-months failed to learn similar words, even though they can discriminate the words This suggested that at the early stages of word learning, when the task is challenging for the infant, they do not have the attentional resources available to attend to the fine phonetic detail in words To confirm this, we tested older infants who are more accomplished word learners (with Fennell, Corcoran, & Stager, Infancy, 2002) To rule out the possibility that perhaps the objects were too much alike, we used less similar objects

33 Testing infants on minimal pair words Habituation Phase Test Phase Same Switch “bih”“dih” “bih” “dih”

34 Infants fail until 17 months Stager & Werker, Nature, 1997 Werker, Fennell, Corcoran, & Stager, Infancy, 2002 -Unless they know the words well (Fennell & Werker, Lang. & Speech, 2003; Swingley & Aslin, 2002) -Or tested in a preference test with both objects as reminders (with Fennell, Swingley, & Yoshida) -And see Julia Wales & George Hollich (CDS, 2003)

35 Infants fail until 17 months Stager & Werker, Nature, 1997 Werker, Fennell, Corcoran, & Stager, Infancy, 2002

36 Minimal pair word learning (Werker, Cohen, et al, 1998; Stager & Werker, 1997; Werker, et. al., 2001)

37 Explanation to date (Stager & Werker, 1997; Fennell & Werker, in press; Werker & Fennell, in press) Word-object linking is initially difficult for infants May pick up detail while listening to words Cannot learn linkage and utilize full phonetic detail The task of learning the associative link is so difficult that without the object there, they can’t remember the precise details of the word form even though it is likely stored in the lexicon As become more accomplished word learners, detail can be used

38 Swingley & Aslin (2001, 2002) do find evidence of use of phonetic detail Where’s the Baby? OR Where’s the Vaby? 2 DVs: Latency to look away from mismatch (14 & 18-22 mos) Total looking time to the match (14 mos)

39 How to explain different results? The two choice looking procedure reveals remind the child of what the link is –Can measure on-line processing via latency –All information available for recognition memory to operate in overall LT to match

40 Experiment 2: Combined Method (w/Dan Swingley, Katie Yoshida & Chris Fennell) Teach infants 2 new minimal pair words via habituation design Test using side by side S&A design 2 DVs –latency to look toward match –Total looking time to match

41 Habituation Phase “bih” “dih” “bih” Test Phase (8 trials)

42 When tested this way, when both the object and the word are presented, the infant has more cues to remind them, and is better able to access the full detail in the lexicon

43 To think about…. Infants can use a variety of statistics to help them learn about words –Frequency –Transitional probabilities –Distributional probabilities Indeed, at the earliest stages of word learning, associative statistics are helpful Do you think statistical & associative learning can explain language?

44 Problems for statistics…. How do infants know what to calculate their statistics across? If they can detect transitional probabilities in tones and visual images as well as syllables, why don’t they ever connect syllables to tones? How do they know to listen for syllables to begin with? NGG might suggest that just as the child is a filter for motherese, the child’s language learning biases act as a filter for which statistical regularities to pay attention to, and when


Download ppt "Building a Lexicon Statistical learning & recognizing words."

Similar presentations


Ads by Google