Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

1st Level Analysis Design Matrix, Contrasts & Inference

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "1st Level Analysis Design Matrix, Contrasts & Inference"— Presentation transcript:

1 1st Level Analysis Design Matrix, Contrasts & Inference
Cat Sebastian and Nathalie Fontaine University College London

2 Outline What is ‘1st level analysis’? Design matrix
What are we testing for? What do all the black lines mean? What do we need to include? Contrasts What are they for? t and F contrasts Inferences How do we do that in SPM5? A B C D [ ]

3 Then create a design matrix which describes the design of your study
Then create a design matrix which describes the design of your study. Then you estimate how much your parameters of interest (betas for each regressor) explain the BOLD response for each voxel, and threshold the resulting estimates to find out where your parameters explain significant amounts of variance.

4 What is 1st level analysis?
1st level analysis: activation is averaged across scans within a subject 2nd level analysis: activation is averaged across subjects (groups can be compared) What question are we asking?: Which voxels in the brain show a pattern of activation over conditions that is consistent with our hypothesis?

5 The Design Matrix More on this Not so much on this Time
These are basis functions, which enable your model to better approximate the haemodynamic response function, and they will be discussed next week. We’ll be talking about the variables that you want to specify that are of interest in your experiment, and how you test for differences under your different conditions using contrasts Not so much on this

6 Y is the BOLD signal at various time points at a
The GLM in fMRI Y = X x β ε Observed data: Y is the BOLD signal at various time points at a single voxel Design matrix: Several components which explain the observed data, i.e. the BOLD time series for the voxel Parameters: The contribution of each component of the design matrix to the value of Y (aim to minimise error) Error: Difference between the observed data, Y, and that predicted by the model, Xβ. b1 b2 Time Fitting X to Y gives you one  (parameter estimate) for each column of X, a μ and e. Betas provide information about fit of regressor X to data, Y, in each voxel. If a beta value is large, this suggest that the X variable associated with it is contributing more to the variation in Y. The least square estimates are the parameter estimates which minimize the residual sum of squares. = +

7 What is Y? Y is a matrix of BOLD signals
Each column represents a single voxel sampled at successive time points. Y Time Intensity

8 What is X (design matrix)?
The design matrix is simply a mathematical description of your experiment E.g.: ‘visual stimulus on = 1’ ‘visual stimulus off = 0’ It should contain ‘regressors of interest’, i.e. variables you have experimentally manipulated, and ‘regressors of no interest’ – head movement, block effects. Why? To minimise the error term, you want to model as much of Y as possible using variables specified in X

9 What should the model look like?
E.g. of a regressor of no interest Regressors of interest Baseline Motion Usually 6 motion regressors: 3 translations, 3 rotations X =

10 Regressors of interest
There are different ways to specify variables, e.g. Conditions: 'dummy' codes identify different levels of experimental factor e.g. integers 0 or 1: 'off' or 'on' Covariates: parametric modulation of independent variable e.g. task-difficulty 1 to 6 on off off on

11 Block vs. event related designs
Event-related – go back retrospectively and see where the events of interest occurred for each regressor. This is just one condition – in your design matrix, you’d have lots of columns next to each other with a different pattern of lines

12 Modelling the baseline
This a column of ‘ones’ modelling the constant, or mean signal (the signal is not zero even without any stimuli or task) SPM will model this automatically Two event-related conditions Baseline often used as a reference (not the same as baseline fixation)

13 From design to a design matrix: an example
Imaging a 2x3 factorial design with factors Modality (Auditory, Visual) and Condition (Concrete, Abstract, Proper) You can model it like this…but is it the best way? V A C1 C2 C3 C1: Concrete nouns Visual C2: Abstract nouns C3: Proper nouns C1: Concrete nouns C2: Abstract nouns Auditory C3: Proper nouns

14 What can we test with this design matrix?
We can test for main effects: - Visual > Auditory? - Concrete > Abstract? But we can’t test for interactions or simple main effects: Visual/concrete > Visual/Abstract? etc V A C1 C2 C3 The design is not orthogonal…

15 An orthogonal design matrix
C1 C2 C3 C1 C1 C2 C2 C3 C3 V A V A V A V A Just like in SPSS, you need to cross your variables in order to model interactions SPM will do this for you automatically if you have a factorial design – just input the factors and the number of levels

16 Ways to improve your model: modelling haemodynamics
The brain does not just switch on and off. Reshape (convolve) regressors to resemble HRF HRF basic function More on this next week! Original HRF Convolved

17 To return to the GLM… ´ = + b1 b2
Y = X x β ε b1 b2 = + Time Remember each regressor is not necessarily one variable. In a factorial design, there is one regressor for each crossed variable We calculate beta values for each regressor in the design matrix We can then perform contrasts to see which regressors make a significant contribution to the model

18 Interim summary: design matrix
We want X to model as much of Y as possible, making the error term small – therefore model everything! This will ensure that the beta values associated with your regressors of interest are as accurate as possible Make sure you specify a new regressor for each crossed variable of interest (orthogonality) Additional complications (basis functions and correlated regressors) will be covered next week Contrasts can then be performed...over to Nathalie

19 Outline What is ‘1st level analysis’? Design matrix
What are we testing for? What do all the black lines mean? What factors do we need to include? Contrasts What are they for? t and F contrasts Inferences How do we do that in SPM5? A B C D [ ]

20 What are they for? General Linear Model (GLM) characterises relationships between our experimental manipulations and the observed data Multiple effects all within the same design matrix Thus, to focus on a particular characteristic, condition, or regressor we use contrasts

21 What are they for? A contrast is used by SPM to test hypotheses about the effects defined in the design matrix, using t-tests and F-tests Contrast specification and the interpretation of the results are entirely dependent on the model specification which in turn depends on the design of the experiment

22 Some general remarks Clear hypothesis / question
Clear design to answer the research question The contrasts and inferences made are dependent on choice of experimental design Most of the problems concerning contrast specification come from poor design specification Poor design: Unclear about what the objective is Try to answer too many questions in a single model We need to think about how the experiment is going to be modelled and which comparisons we wish to make BEFORE acquiring the data

23 Contrasts E.g.: Contrasts with conditions:
The conditions that we are interested in can take on a positive value, such as 1 The conditions that we want to subtract from these conditions of interest can take on a negative value, such as -1

24 Contrasts Contrast 1: Language minus Control: 1 0 0 -1
Condition 1: Language task Condition 2: Memory task Condition 3: Motor task Condition 4: Control Contrast 1: Language minus Control: Contrast 2: Motor minus Memory: Contrast 3: Control minus Motor: Contrast 4: (Language + Memory) minus Control: This contrast will measure areas of the brain that have significantly increased activity in the average of the language and memory conditions, compared with the control condition – another way of looking at this contrast is the sum of the individual condition contrasts of and

25 Contrasts - Factorial design
SIMPLE MAIN EFFECT A – B Simple main effect of motion (vs. no motion) in the context of low load [ ] MAIN EFFECT (A + B) – (C + D) The main effect of low load (vs. high load) irrelevant of motion  Main effect of load [ ] INTERACTION (A - B) – (C - D) The interaction effect of motion (vs. no motion) greater under low (vs. high) load [ ] MOTION NO MOTION A B C D LOW LOAD HIGH A B C D A B C D A B C D

26 Contrasts t-test: is there a significant increase or is there a significant decrease in a specific contrast (between conditions) – directional F-test: is there a significant difference between conditions in the contrast – non-directional

27 Example Two event-related conditions
The subjects press a button with either their left or right hand depending on a visual instruction (involving some attention) We are interested in finding the brain regions that respond more to left than right motor movement

28 t-contrasts t-contrasts are directional Left Right Mean
To find the brain regions corresponding more to left than right motor responses we use the contrast: T = [1 -1 0] Left Right Mean So we create this design matrix, with left presses, right presses, and mean activation

29 contrast of estimated parameters
t-contrasts A one dimensional contrast t = contrast of estimated parameters variance estimate s2c’(X’X)+c c’b So, for a contrast in our model of : t = (ß1x1 + ß2x-1 + ß3x0) Estimated variance

30 Brain activation: Left motor responses
This shows activation of the contralateral motor cortex, ipsilateral cerebellum, etc.

31 F-contrasts F-contrasts are non-directional Left Right Mean
To test for the overall difference (positive or negative) from the left and right responses we use: [ ; ] Left Right Mean

32 F-test To test a hypothesis about general effects, independent of the direction of the contrast A collection of t-contrasts that you want to test together F = Error variance estimate Additional variance accounted for by tested effects So you are testing for significance of the first or second parameter estimates

33 Brain activation Areas involved in the overall difference (positive or negative) from the left and right responses (non-directional)

34 Test Design and contrast SPM(t) or SPM(F) t-test F-test [1 -1 0]
[ ; ] F-test

35 Inferences about subjects and populations
Inference about the effect in relation to: The within-subject variability (1st level analysis) The between subject variability (2nd level analysis) This distinction relates directly to the difference between fixed and random-effect analyses Inferences based on fixed effects analyses are about the particular subject(s) studied Random-effects analyses are usually more conservative but allow the inference to be generalized to the population from which the subjects were selected For a given group of subjects, there is a fundamental distinction between saying that the response is significant relative to the precision with which that response in measured and saying that it is significant in relation to the intersubject variability. This distinction relates directly to the difference between fixed and random-effect analyses. More on this in few weeks!

36 One voxel = One test (t, F)
amplitude General Linear Model fitting statistical image time Statistical image (SPM) Temporal series fMRI voxel time course From Poline (2005)

37 Choosing a statistical threshold
Important consideration in neuroimaging = the tremendous number of statistical tests computed for each comparison E.g.: if 100,000 voxels are tested at a probability threshold of 5%, we should expect: 5000 voxels will incorrectly appear as significant activations = Apparent activations by chance; FALSE POSITIVE

38 Choosing a statistical threshold
Uncorrected threshold of p < .001 Familywise Error (FWE) Bonferroni correction E.g.: .05/100,000 = False Discovery Rate (FDR) Adjusts the criterion used based on the amount of signal present in the data Reduce the number of comparisons E.g.: Instead of examining the entire brain, examine just a small region IMPORTANCE of taking into account the multiple comparisons across voxels BUT also the multiple comparisons across contrasts (i.e., the number of contrasts tested) Bonferroni: this correction will result in not detecting some “real” activations (Bianca de Haan & Rorden)

39 How do we do that in SPM5?

40 Summary Contrasts are statistical (t or F) tests of specific hypotheses t-contrast looks for a significant increase or decrease in a specific contrast (directional) F-contrast looks for a significant difference between conditions in the contrast (non-directional) Importance of having a clear design Inferences about subjects (1st level) and populations (2nd level) Importance of considering the multiple comparisons

41 References Human Brain Function 2, in particular Chapter 8 by Poline, Kherif, & Penny ( Introduction: Experimental design and statistical parametric mapping, by Friston Linear Models and Contrasts, PowerPoint presentation by Poline (April, 2005), SPM short course at Yale Previous years’ slides CBU Imaging Wiki ( ( SPM5 Manual, The FIL Methods Group (2007) An introduction to functional MRI by de Haan & Rorden

42 Thank you!


Download ppt "1st Level Analysis Design Matrix, Contrasts & Inference"

Similar presentations


Ads by Google