Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Contributions of Spatial Skills to Geometry Achievement:

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Contributions of Spatial Skills to Geometry Achievement:"— Presentation transcript:

1 Contributions of Spatial Skills to Geometry Achievement:
Yvonne Kao & John Anderson Carnegie Mellon University

2 …the first stages of removing obstacles in the way of pupils learning geometry are activities designed to improve the pupils’ understanding of their spatial world. If every teacher of geometry took steps to prepare the pupils ‘spatially’ before mathematizing the ideas, he or she would find the later work so much easier to teach on the base of those spatial experiences. And the pupils would, of course, realize that they already had some spatial foundations with which to secure their understanding of geometrical ideas (Bishop, 1986, p. 144).

3 Background Spatial Visualization “The ability to mentally manipulate, rotate, twist, or invert a pictorially presented stimulus object” (McGee, 1979) Correlations between spatial visualization and mathematics achievement generally fall between 0.3 and 0.6 (Battista, 1990) Spatial visualization predicts geometry achievement (Battista, 1990; Battista, Wheatley, & Talsma, 1992; Casey, Nuttall, Pezaris, & Benbow, 1995; Connor & Serbin, 1985)

4 Background Spatial factors that also predict geometry achievement:
Spatial orientation (Connor & Serbin, 1985) …the ability to perceive and recognize multiple perspectives or representations of an object (Tartre, 1990) Flexibility of closure (Hoz, 1981) The ability to “break one gestalt and form another” (Lohman, 1988)

5 But… Problems: Space-math correlations may not be greater than correlations between math and (Friedman, 1995): verbal ability reading comprehension abstract reasoning sports information Studies have generally failed to establish a unique contribution of spatial skills to mathematics achievement that is distinct from general intelligence (Chipman, 2005) The factor-analytic approach is not very useful for teachers (Bishop, 1980)

6 Goal To better define the relationship between spatial skills and geometry achievement so that we can identify targets for instruction. To what extent do spatial skills contribute to geometry achievement, above and beyond other cognitive factors? Do spatial skills matter more for specific geometry subdomains?

7 Design Exploratory study
Cognitive battery from Kit of Factor-Referenced Tests (Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976) Collected data on students’ standardized test scores and course grades

8 Cognitive Battery General Reasoning
Necessary Arithmetic Operations Test

9 Cognitive Battery Verbal Vocabulary Word fluency

10 Cognitive Battery Spatial Visualization Orientation
Flexibility of closure

11 Participants 138 public high school students: Geometry Course N
Females Males Mean Age (SD) % Caucasian Traditional 2 teachers 84 46 38 14.41 (1.76) 96.20 Intermediate 1 teacher 41 20 21 15.18 (0.87) 97.30 TE (Cognitive Tutor) 1 teacher 13 4 9 16.08 (0.86) 92.31

12 Participants: PSSA

13 Results: Cognitive Battery
p < .0005 * p = .077 p = .109 p = .020 * p = .029 * p < .0005 *

14 Correlations w/PSSA Math
Standardized math test scores were significantly correlated with: General Reasoning r(132) = 0.67, p < .0005 Verbal Index r(132) = 0.43, p < .0005 Spatial Index r(132) = 0.39, p < .0005 PSSA Reading r(132) = 0.67, p < .0005  = , t(129) = 6.71, p < .0005 59%  = .383, t(129) = 6.82, p < .0005

15 Correlations w/PSSA Math
Standardized math test scores were significantly correlated with: General Reasoning r(132) = 0.67, p < .0005 Verbal Index r(132) = 0.43, p < .0005 Spatial Index r(132) = 0.39, p < .0005 PSSA Reading r(132) = 0.67, p < .0005  = , t(129) = 9.08, p < .0005 50%  = , t(129) = 3.64, p < .0005

16 Intermediate Geometry
Unit Mean Score (SD) Language of Plane Geometry 74% (21%) Angles 75% (19%) Polygons and Polyhedrons 62% (18%) Introduction to Transformations 76% (18%) Triangles and Inequalities 69% (24%) Congruent Triangles 73% (19%) Parallel Lines 79% (10%) Properties of Quadrilaterals 70% (14%) Perimeter and Area 75% (17%) Similarity 75% (9%) Overall 74% (11%)

17 Intermediate Geometry
Overall test performance in Intermediate Geometry significantly correlated with: General Reasoning Verbal Index r(39) = 0.36, p = .010 Spatial Index r(39) = 0.26, p = .004  = .051, t(37) = 2.85, p = .007 17%

18 Intermediate Geometry
Overall test performance in Intermediate Geometry significantly correlated with: General Reasoning Verbal Index r(39) = 0.36, p = .010 Spatial Index r(39) = 0.26, p = .004 Flexibility of Closure  = .042, t(37) = 2.83, p = .005 19%

19 Intermediate Geometry
Individual unit analysis Unit Mean Score (SD) Language of Plane Geometry 74% (21%) Angles 75% (19%) Polygons and Polyhedrons 62% (18%) Introduction to Transformations 76% (18%) Triangles and Inequalities 69% (24%) Congruent Triangles 73% (19%) Parallel Lines 79% (10%) Properties of Quadrilaterals 70% (14%) Perimeter and Area 75% (17%) Similarity 75% (9%) Overall 74% (11%)

20 Intermediate Geometry
Individual unit analysis General Reasoning and Verbal Index did not significantly correlate with any individual units Spatial Index and Congruent Triangles: r(51) = 0.41, p = .001 Flexibility of Closure:  = .054, t(37) = 2.16, p = %

21 Conclusions Spatial skills do contribute to geometry achievement above and beyond other cognitive factors …especially Flexibility of Closure …especially for Congruent Triangles To be continued…

22 Thank You! Joan Son and the participating teachers Cynthia Peng
This research was supported in part by the Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education, through Grant R305B to Carnegie Mellon University and in part by NSF ROLE grant REC to Anderson.

23


Download ppt "Contributions of Spatial Skills to Geometry Achievement:"

Similar presentations


Ads by Google