Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Analyzing Intervention Fidelity and Achieved Relative Strength

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Analyzing Intervention Fidelity and Achieved Relative Strength"— Presentation transcript:

1 Analyzing Intervention Fidelity and Achieved Relative Strength
David S. Cordray Vanderbilt University NCER/IES RCT Training Institute,2010

2 Perspectives on Interventionion Fidelity: When is Analysis Needed?
Monitoring implementation, retraining, maximize fidelity to the intervention model. Program-specific models (within Tx, only) Interpreting results from ITT analyses; Cause-Effect congruity; Lack of Cause-Effect congruity. Analysis of variation in delivery/receipt of intervention components. Compliance-based analyses (TOT, LATE) Mediation – instrumental variables approach

3 Program Specific Models
Analyses conducted within the intervention, only RAOutputs  Outcomes No variance, no covariance Component-wise assessment of fidelity Configural judgment about implementation fidelity

4 TTX’ TTx Variability TC Variability Review: Concepts and Definitions
Positive Infidelity .45 .40 .35 .30 .25 .20 .15 .10 .05 .00 Intervention Exposure 100 90 85 80 75 70 65 60 55 50 Infidelity “Infidelity” tC t tx True Fidelity Intervention Differentiation Achieved Relative Strength =.15 Variability TC Variability Tx Contamination Augmentation of C Intervention Exposure Treatment Strength Outcome Review: Concepts and Definitions

5 Congruity of Cause-Effect
Analysis Type I Congruity of Cause-Effect

6 Common Cause-Effect Scenarios
The Cause The Effect Low High Low/Low = Cause-Effect Congruity Low/High = ???? High/Low = Dampening Process ???? High/High = Cause-Effect Congruity

7 Cause-Effect Congruity: High/High Example
Fantuzzo, King & Heller (1992) studied the effects of reciprocal peer tutoring on mathematics and school adjustment. 2 X 2 factorial design crossing levels of structured peer tutoring and group reward 45 min. 2-3 per week; sessions Fidelity assessments: Observations (via checklist) of students and staff, rated the adherence of group members to scripted features of each condition; 50% random checks of sessions Mid-year, knowledge tests to index the level of understanding of students about the intervention components in each of the four conditions.

8 Fantuzzo et al. Continued
Fidelity results: Adherence (via observations): 90-100% across conditions, 95% overall Student understanding (via 15 item test): 82% SD=11% (range %); ANOVA=ns Reward+structure condition: 84% Control: 86% Effects on mathematics computation: ES= ( )/1.71 = 1.58 Congruity=High/High; no additional analyses needed

9 Exposure and Achieved Relative Strength
Fantuzzo et al. example is: Relatively rare; Incorporates intervention differentiation, yielding fidelity indices for all conditions. More commonly, intervention exposure is assessed: Yielding scales of the degree to which individuals experience the intervention components in both conditions The achieved relative strength index is used for establishing the differences between conditions on causal components

10 Average ARS Index Clustering Adjustment Group Difference
Sample Size Adjustment Where, = mean for group 1 (tTx ) = mean for group 2 (tC) ST = pooled within groups standard deviation nTx = treatment sample size nC = control sample size n = average cluster size p = Intra-class correlation (ICC) N = total sample size 10 10

11 A Partial Example of the Meaning of ARSI
Randomized Group Assignment Professional Development Differentiated Instruction Improved Student Outcomes

12 Very Large Group Difference, Limited Overlap Between Conditions
Intervention Control 70 60 50 40 0 1 00 40 7 9 30 20 10 00 Hours of Professional Development 70 60 50 40 0 1 00 40 7 9 30 20 10 00 Mean=61.8 SD=6.14 ARSI: = ( )/6.61 =5.08 U3= 99% Hours of Professional Development Mean= 28.2 SD=7.04

13 Cohen’s U3 Index: Very Large Group Separation
Intervention Mean Control Mean U3=99th Percentile ARSI=5.08 50th percentile

14 Small Group Differences, Substantial Overlap
Control Intervention 70 60 50 40 0 1 00 70 60 50 10 6 8 00 Hours of Professional Development 70 60 50 40 0 1 00 70 60 50 10 6 8 00 Mean=30.8 SD=6.14 ARSI: = ( )/6.61 =0.39 U3= 66% Hours of Professional Development Mean= 28.2 SD=7.04

15 Main points…. Analysis of intervention fidelity and achieve relative strength is a natural counterpart to estimating ESs in ITT studies. They provide an interpretive framework for explaining outcome effects. When ES and ARSI are discordant, serve as the basis for additional analysis. Next section focuses on analysis of variation

16 Linking Variation in Treatment Receipt/Delivery to Outcomes
Analysis II Linking Variation in Treatment Receipt/Delivery to Outcomes

17 Variation in Treatment Receipt/Delivery Within Groups: Regression Models Using Fidelity/ARS Indicators Rather than relying on the 0,1 coding of groups, fidelity indicators replace the group variable. New question being answered. Value of fidelity indices will depend on their strength of the relationship with the outcome; The greater the group difference, on average, the less informative fidelity indicators will be; and High predictability requires reliable indices

18 Using Group, Fidelity Indicators, or Both: A Simple Example
Fidelity Indicator= Hours of Professional Development Randomized Group Assignment Outcome= Differentiated Instruction Improved Student Outcomes

19 The “Value Added” of Implementation Fidelity/ARS Data
Group Separation U3 Predicting Level of Differentiated Instruction R2Group R2Hours Pro Development Small 0.39 0.01 0.293* (0.28) Large 2.36 0.215* 0.437* (0.22) Very Large 5.08 0.401* 0.549* (0.15)

20 Intent-to-treat (ITT) vs. Fidelity/ARS Index: An Example
Justice, Mashburn, Pence, & Wiggins (2008) examined: Language-Focused Curriculum (LFC) in 14 classes; Classes randomly assigned to LFC and control; Core component of LFC is the use of language stimulation techniques (e.g., open questions, recasts, models); and Outcome  Growth in expressive language examined (fall to spring)

21 Justice et al. Continued
Implementation fidelity assessed: 3 times using 2 hour observation (45 item check list) 50 min. video sample; and 40 weekly lesson plans. Fidelity score = weighted sum of frequency of the use of 7 language stimulation techniques (range 0-21); Fidelity = score/21; averaged over observations Results: LST teachers average fidelity = 0.57 (range ) Control teachers average fidelity = 0.32 (range ) ANOVA F=11.83, p = .005; d = ARSI = 1.71

22 Justice et al. Continued
Level 1 0,1 Group Level 2  ITT Fidelity Score Level 2  TOT Both coefficients were not significant and near zero.

23 Justice, et al. (2008): Some Important Issues
Few teachers exhibited high levels of LST use (core component of LFC) Fidelity overall = 0.45 Justice et al. argue, the large group difference (ARSI=1.71 for fidelity = 0.57 vs. 0.32) may not have been sufficient because the dosage (0.57) was so far below what is needed to affect language development. Other possibilities include: Reliability of the scaling? Use of average when trend in observations showed improvement? Coverage of central constructs? Functional form of fidelity-outcome linkage?

24 Analysis III: Compliance-based Models

25 ITT and Treatment on the Treated (TOT)
Average Treatment Effect (ATE) estimate assume full compliance. Intent to treat (ITT) estimate ignores compliance. Common to have “no-shows” (for whatever reasons). What is the effect of treatment on those receiving treatment (TOT) – the “shows”? Bloom 1984 – proposed an adjustment to the ITT estimate, applies to non-compliance of “no-shows” only: ITT/pT Where PT is the percent Tx recipients

26 TOT continued Assumes treatment receipt/delivery can be meaningfully dichotomized and there is experimentally induced receipt or non-receipt of treatment. Works well when no shows experience no treatment effect, Assumes randomization does not influence the outcome unless an individual receives the intended treatment. This is plausible when treatments are voluntary, substantial, and without a close substitute. In mandatory treatments assumption is less plausible, randomization can affect behavior and outcomes,

27 Local Average Treatment Effect (LATE)
LATE (Local Average Treatment Effect): adjust ITT estimate by T and C treatment receipt rates (compliers in T and defiers in C) ITT is adjusted as: ITT/(pc-pd) LATE can approximate causal estimates, if a series of assumptions hold. Model can be expressed as Z  D  Y (or random assignment treatment receipt  outcome)

28 Some Key LATE Assumptions
Defiers do not exist or the average program effect is the same for compliers and defiers (monotonicity assumption). The effect of Z (assignment) on Y (outcome) is thru variable D (Tx receipt) or the exclusion restriction (all other paths between Z and Y are excluded). Strong covariation between Z and D Randomization makes the instrument and predicted D uncorrelated with error (e). SUTVA (stable unit treatment value assumption)

29 Analysis Type 4: Mediation Model

30 Mediation Models Random Assignment (Z) Treatment Receipt (D) Outcome
(Y) This is an extension of LATE and is assumption laden

31 Key Points and Issues Fidelity assessment serves two roles:
Average causal difference between conditions; and Using fidelity measures to assess the effects of variation in implementation on outcomes. Degree of fidelity and Achieved Relative Strength provide fuller picture of the results Modeling fidelity depends on: Level of implementation fidelity Outcome variance Variability in implementation fidelity/ARS l Most applications, fidelity is just another Level 2 or 3 variable. Uncertainty and the need for alternative specifications: Measure of fidelity Index of achieved relative strength Fidelity-outcome model specification (linear, non-linear) Adaptation-fidelity tension

32 Questions and Discussion


Download ppt "Analyzing Intervention Fidelity and Achieved Relative Strength"

Similar presentations


Ads by Google