Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology"— Presentation transcript:

1 Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology
Statistics Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology

2 Statistics 2 General kinds of Statistics Descriptive statistics
Used to describe, simplify, & organize data sets Describing distributions of scores Inferential statistics Used to test claims about the population, based on data gathered from samples Takes sampling error into account, are the results above and beyond what you’d expect by random chance Statistics

3 Describing Distributions
Visual descriptions - A picture of the distribution is usually helpful Gives a good sense of the properties of the distribution Many different ways to display distribution Graphs Continuous variable: histogram, line graph Categorical variable: pie chart, bar chart Tables Frequency distribution table Table of condition means and standard deviations Numerical descriptions of distributions Describing Distributions

4 Be careful using a line graph for categorical variables
People might interpolate: The line implies that there are responses between Smith and Doe, but there are not A word of caution

5 Describing Distributions
Properties of a distribution Shape Symmetric v. asymmetric (skew) Unimodal v. multimodal Center Where most of the data in the distribution are Mean, Median, Mode Spread (variability) How similar/dissimilar are the scores in the distribution? Standard deviation (variance), Range Describing Distributions

6 Properties of distributions: Shape
Symmetric The two sides line up Asymmetric (skewed) The two sides do not line up Properties of distributions: Shape

7 Properties of distributions: Shape
Unimodal (one mode) Major mode Minor mode Multimodal Bimodal examples Properties of distributions: Shape

8 Properties of distributions: Center
There are three main measures of center Purpose: to use a single score to represent the distribution as a whole Mean (M): the arithmetic average Add up all of the scores and divide by the total number Most used measure of center Median (Mdn): the middle score in terms of location The score that cuts off the top 50% of the from the bottom 50% Good for skewed distributions (e.g. net worth) Mode: the most frequent score Good for nominal scales (e.g. eye color) A must for multi-modal distributions Properties of distributions: Center

9 The Mean The most commonly used measure of center
The arithmetic average Computing the mean Divide by the total number in the population The formula for the population mean is (a parameter): Add up all of the X’s The formula for the sample mean is (a statistic): Divide by the total number in the sample The Mean

10 Spread (Variability) How similar are the scores? Low variability
The scores are fairly similar High variability The scores are fairly dissimilar mean mean Spread (Variability)

11 Spread (Variability) How similar are the scores?
Range: the maximum value - minimum value Only takes two scores from the distribution into account Influenced by extreme values (outliers) Standard deviation (SD): (essentially) the average amount that the scores in the distribution deviate from the mean Takes all of the scores into account Also influenced by extreme values (but not as much as the range) Variance: standard deviation squared Spread (Variability)

12 The standard deviation is the most popular and most important measure of variability.
The standard deviation measures how far off all of the individuals in the distribution are from a standard, where that standard is the mean of the distribution. Essentially, the average of the deviations. μ Standard deviation

13 An Example: Computing the Mean
Our population 2, 4, 6, 8 μ An Example: Computing the Mean

14 An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)
Step 1: To get a measure of the deviation we need to subtract the population mean from every individual in our distribution. Our population 2, 4, 6, 8 μ -3 X – μ = deviation scores 2 - 5 = -3 An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)

15 An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)
Step 1: To get a measure of the deviation we need to subtract the population mean from every individual in our distribution. Our population 2, 4, 6, 8 μ -1 X – μ = deviation scores 2 - 5 = -3 4 - 5 = -1 An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)

16 An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)
Step 1: To get a measure of the deviation we need to subtract the population mean from every individual in our distribution. Our population 2, 4, 6, 8 μ 1 X – μ = deviation scores 2 - 5 = -3 6 - 5 = +1 4 - 5 = -1 An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)

17 An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)
Step 1: To get a measure of the deviation we need to subtract the population mean from every individual in our distribution. Our population 2, 4, 6, 8 μ 3 X – μ = deviation scores 2 - 5 = -3 6 - 5 = +1 Notice that if you add up all of the deviations they must equal 0. 4 - 5 = -1 8 - 5 = +3 An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)

18 An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)
Step 2: So what we have to do is get rid of the negative signs. We do this by squaring the deviations and then taking the square root of the sum of the squared deviations (SS). SS = Σ(X - μ)2 2 - 5 = -3 4 - 5 = -1 6 - 5 = +1 8 - 5 = +3 X – μ = deviation scores = (-3)2 + (-1)2 + (+1)2 + (+3)2 = = 20 An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)

19 An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)
Step 3: ComputeVariance (which is simply the average of the squared deviations (SS)) So to get the mean, we need to divide by the number of individuals in the population. variance = σ2 = SS/N An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)

20 An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)
Step 4: Compute Standard Deviation To get this we need to take the square root of the population variance. standard deviation = σ = An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)

21 An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)
To review: Step 1: Compute deviation scores Step 2: Compute the SS Step 3: Determine the variance Take the average of the squared deviations Divide the SS by the N Step 4: Determine the standard deviation Take the square root of the variance An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (population)

22 An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (sample)
To review: Step 1: Compute deviation scores Step 2: Compute the SS Step 3: Determine the variance Take the average of the squared deviations Divide the SS by the N-1 Step 4: Determine the standard deviation Take the square root of the variance This is done because samples are biased to be less variable than the population. This “correction factor” will increase the sample’s SD (making it a better estimate of the population’s SD) An Example: Computing Standard Deviation (sample)

23 Statistics Why do we use them? Descriptive statistics
Used to describe, simplify, & organize data sets Describing distributions of scores Inferential statistics Used to test claims about the population, based on data gathered from samples Takes sampling error into account, are the results above and beyond what you’d expect by random chance Statistics

24 Inferential Statistics
Purpose: To make claims about populations based on data collected from samples What’s the big deal? Example Experiment: Group A - gets treatment to improve memory Group B - gets no treatment (control) After treatment period test both groups for memory Results: Group A’s average memory score is 80% Group B’s is 76% Is the 4% difference a “real” difference (statistically significant) or is it just sampling error? Inferential Statistics

25 Testing Hypotheses Step 1: State your hypotheses
Step 2: Set your decision criteria Step 3: Collect your data from your sample(s) Step 4: Compute your test statistics Step 5: Make a decision about your null hypothesis “Reject H0” “Fail to reject H0” Testing Hypotheses

26 Testing Hypotheses Step 1: State your hypotheses
This is the hypothesis that you are testing Null hypothesis (H0) Alternative hypothesis(ses) “There are no differences (effects)” Generally, “not all groups are equal” You aren’t out to prove the alternative hypothesis (although it feels like this is what you want to do) If you reject the null hypothesis, then you’re left with support for the alternative(s) (NOT proof!) Testing Hypotheses

27 Testing Hypotheses Step 1: State your hypotheses
In our memory example experiment Null H0: mean of Group A = mean of Group B Alternative HA: mean of Group A ≠ mean of Group B (Or more precisely: Group A > Group B) It seems like our theory is that the treatment should improve memory. That’s the alternative hypothesis. That’s NOT the one the we’ll test with inferential statistics. Instead, we test the H0 Testing Hypotheses

28 Testing Hypotheses Step 1: State your hypotheses
Step 2: Set your decision criteria Your alpha level will be your guide for when to: “reject the null hypothesis” “fail to reject the null hypothesis” This could be correct conclusion or the incorrect conclusion Two different ways to go wrong Type I error: saying that there is a difference when there really isn’t one (probability of making this error is “alpha level”) Type II error: saying that there is not a difference when there really is one Testing Hypotheses

29 Error types Real world (‘truth’) H0 is correct H0 is wrong
Type I error Reject H0 Experimenter’s conclusions Fail to Reject H0 Type II error Error types

30 Error types: Courtroom analogy
Real world (‘truth’) Defendant is innocent Defendant is guilty Type I error Find guilty Jury’s decision Type II error Find not guilty Error types: Courtroom analogy

31 Type I error: concluding that there is an effect (a difference between groups) when there really isn’t. Sometimes called “significance level” We try to minimize this (keep it low) Pick a low level of alpha Psychology: 0.05 and 0.01 most common Type II error: concluding that there isn’t an effect, when there really is. Related to the Statistical Power of a test How likely are you able to detect a difference if it is there Error types

32 Testing Hypotheses Step 1: State your hypotheses
Step 2: Set your decision criteria Step 3: Collect your data from your sample(s) Step 4: Compute your test statistics Descriptive statistics (means, standard deviations, etc.) Inferential statistics (t-tests, ANOVAs, etc.) Step 5: Make a decision about your null hypothesis Reject H0 “statistically significant differences” Fail to reject H0 “not statistically significant differences” Testing Hypotheses

33 Statistical significance
“Statistically significant differences” When you “reject your null hypothesis” Essentially this means that the observed difference is above what you’d expect by chance “Chance” is determined by estimating how much sampling error there is Factors affecting “chance” Sample size Population variability Statistical significance

34 (Pop mean - sample mean)
Population mean Population Distribution x Sampling error (Pop mean - sample mean) n = 1 Sampling error

35 (Pop mean - sample mean)
Population mean Population Distribution Sample mean x x Sampling error (Pop mean - sample mean) n = 2 Sampling error

36 (Pop mean - sample mean)
Generally, as the sample size increases, the sampling error decreases Population mean Population Distribution Sample mean x Sampling error (Pop mean - sample mean) n = 10 Sampling error

37 Typically the narrower the population distribution, the narrower the range of possible samples, and the smaller the “chance” Large population variability Small population variability Sampling error

38 Sampling error Population Distribution of sample means
These two factors combine to impact the distribution of sample means. The distribution of sample means is a distribution of all possible sample means of a particular sample size that can be drawn from the population Population Distribution of sample means XC Samples of size = n XA XD Avg. Sampling error XB “chance” Sampling error

39 Significance “A statistically significant difference” means:
the researcher is concluding that there is a difference above and beyond chance with the probability of making a type I error at 5% (assuming an alpha level = 0.05) Note “statistical significance” is not the same thing as theoretical significance. Only means that there is a statistical difference Doesn’t mean that it is an important difference Significance

40 Non-Significance Failing to reject the null hypothesis
Generally, not interested in “accepting the null hypothesis” (remember we can’t prove things only disprove them) Usually check to see if you made a Type II error (failed to detect a difference that is really there) Check the statistical power of your test Sample size is too small Effects that you’re looking for are really small Check your controls, maybe too much variability Non-Significance

41 From last time XA XB About populations Example Experiment:
Group A - gets treatment to improve memory Group B - gets no treatment (control) After treatment period test both groups for memory Results: Group A’s average memory score is 80% Group B’s is 76% H0: μA = μB H0: there is no difference between Grp A and Grp B Real world (‘truth’) H0 is correct H0 is wrong Experimenter’s conclusions Reject H0 Fail to Reject H0 Type I error Type II error Is the 4% difference a “real” difference (statistically significant) or is it just sampling error? Two sample distributions XA XB 76% 80% From last time

42 “Generic” statistical test
Tests the question: Are there differences between groups due to a treatment? H0 is true (no treatment effect) Real world (‘truth’) H0 is correct H0 is wrong Experimenter’s conclusions Reject H0 Fail to Reject H0 Type I error Type II error Two possibilities in the “real world” One population Two sample distributions XA XB 76% 80% “Generic” statistical test

43 “Generic” statistical test
Tests the question: Are there differences between groups due to a treatment? Real world (‘truth’) H0 is correct H0 is wrong Experimenter’s conclusions Reject H0 Fail to Reject H0 Type I error Type II error Two possibilities in the “real world” H0 is true (no treatment effect) H0 is false (is a treatment effect) Two populations XA XB XB XA 76% 80% 76% 80% People who get the treatment change, they form a new population (the “treatment population) “Generic” statistical test

44 “Generic” statistical test
XB XA ER: Random sampling error ID: Individual differences (if between subjects factor) TR: The effect of a treatment Why might the samples be different? (What is the source of the variability between groups)? “Generic” statistical test

45 “Generic” statistical test
XB XA ER: Random sampling error ID: Individual differences (if between subjects factor) TR: The effect of a treatment The generic test statistic - is a ratio of sources of variability Observed difference TR + ID + ER ID + ER Computed test statistic = = Difference from chance “Generic” statistical test

46 Sampling error Population “chance” Distribution of sample means
The distribution of sample means is a distribution of all possible sample means of a particular sample size that can be drawn from the population Population Distribution of sample means XC Samples of size = n XA XD Avg. Sampling error XB “chance” Sampling error

47 “Generic” statistical test
The generic test statistic distribution To reject the H0, you want a computed test statistics that is large reflecting a large Treatment Effect (TR) What’s large enough? The alpha level gives us the decision criterion TR + ID + ER ID + ER Distribution of the test statistic Test statistic Distribution of sample means -level determines where these boundaries go “Generic” statistical test

48 “Generic” statistical test
The generic test statistic distribution To reject the H0, you want a computed test statistics that is large reflecting a large Treatment Effect (TR) What’s large enough? The alpha level gives us the decision criterion Distribution of the test statistic Reject H0 Fail to reject H0 “Generic” statistical test

49 “Generic” statistical test
The generic test statistic distribution To reject the H0, you want a computed test statistics that is large reflecting a large Treatment Effect (TR) What’s large enough? The alpha level gives us the decision criterion Distribution of the test statistic Reject H0 “One tailed test”: sometimes you know to expect a particular difference (e.g., “improve memory performance”) Fail to reject H0 “Generic” statistical test

50 “Generic” statistical test
Things that affect the computed test statistic Size of the treatment effect The bigger the effect, the bigger the computed test statistic Difference expected by chance (sample error) Sample size Variability in the population “Generic” statistical test

51 Significance “A statistically significant difference” means:
the researcher is concluding that there is a difference above and beyond chance with the probability of making a type I error at 5% (assuming an alpha level = 0.05) Note “statistical significance” is not the same thing as theoretical significance. Only means that there is a statistical difference Doesn’t mean that it is an important difference Significance

52 Non-Significance Failing to reject the null hypothesis
Generally, not interested in “accepting the null hypothesis” (remember we can’t prove things only disprove them) Usually check to see if you made a Type II error (failed to detect a difference that is really there) Check the statistical power of your test Sample size is too small Effects that you’re looking for are really small Check your controls, maybe too much variability Non-Significance

53 Some inferential statistical tests
1 factor with two groups T-tests Between groups: 2-independent samples Within groups: Repeated measures samples (matched, related) 1 factor with more than two groups Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) (either between groups or repeated measures) Multi-factorial Factorial ANOVA Some inferential statistical tests

54 T-test Design Formula: Observed difference X1 - X2 T =
2 separate experimental conditions Degrees of freedom Based on the size of the sample and the kind of t-test Formula: Observed difference T = X X2 Diff by chance Based on sample error Computation differs for between and within t-tests T-test

55 T-test Reporting your results
The observed difference between conditions Kind of t-test Computed T-statistic Degrees of freedom for the test The “p-value” of the test “The mean of the treatment group was 12 points higher than the control group. An independent samples t-test yielded a significant difference, t(24) = 5.67, p < 0.05.” “The mean score of the post-test was 12 points higher than the pre-test. A repeated measures t-test demonstrated that this difference was significant significant, t(12) = 5.67, p < 0.05.” T-test

56 Analysis of Variance XB XA XC Designs Test statistic is an F-ratio
More than two groups 1 Factor ANOVA, Factorial ANOVA Both Within and Between Groups Factors Test statistic is an F-ratio Degrees of freedom Several to keep track of The number of them depends on the design Analysis of Variance

57 Analysis of Variance More than two groups F-ratio = XB XA XC
Now we can’t just compute a simple difference score since there are more than one difference So we use variance instead of simply the difference Variance is essentially an average difference Observed variance Variance from chance F-ratio = Analysis of Variance

58 1 factor ANOVA 1 Factor, with more than two levels XB XA XC
Now we can’t just compute a simple difference score since there are more than one difference A - B, B - C, & A - C 1 factor ANOVA

59 1 factor ANOVA The ANOVA tests this one!! XA = XB = XC XA ≠ XB ≠ XC
Null hypothesis: H0: all the groups are equal The ANOVA tests this one!! XA = XB = XC Do further tests to pick between these Alternative hypotheses HA: not all the groups are equal XA ≠ XB ≠ XC XA ≠ XB = XC XA = XB ≠ XC XA = XC ≠ XB 1 factor ANOVA

60 1 factor ANOVA Planned contrasts and post-hoc tests:
- Further tests used to rule out the different Alternative hypotheses XA ≠ XB ≠ XC Test 1: A ≠ B XA = XB ≠ XC Test 2: A ≠ C XA ≠ XB = XC Test 3: B = C XA = XC ≠ XB 1 factor ANOVA

61 1 factor ANOVA Reporting your results The observed differences
Kind of test Computed F-ratio Degrees of freedom for the test The “p-value” of the test Any post-hoc or planned comparison results “The mean score of Group A was 12, Group B was 25, and Group C was 27. A 1-way ANOVA was conducted and the results yielded a significant difference, F(2,25) = 5.67, p < Post hoc tests revealed that the differences between groups A and B and A and C were statistically reliable (respectively t(1) = 5.67, p < 0.05 & t(1) = 6.02, p <0.05). Groups B and C did not differ significantly from one another” 1 factor ANOVA

62 We covered much of this in our experimental design lecture
More than one factor Factors may be within or between Overall design may be entirely within, entirely between, or mixed Many F-ratios may be computed An F-ratio is computed to test the main effect of each factor An F-ratio is computed to test each of the potential interactions between the factors Factorial ANOVAs

63 Factorial ANOVAs Reporting your results The observed differences
Because there may be a lot of these, may present them in a table instead of directly in the text Kind of design e.g. “2 x 2 completely between factorial design” Computed F-ratios May see separate paragraphs for each factor, and for interactions Degrees of freedom for the test Each F-ratio will have its own set of df’s The “p-value” of the test May want to just say “all tests were tested with an alpha level of 0.05” Any post-hoc or planned comparison results Typically only the theoretically interesting comparisons are presented Factorial ANOVAs

64 Non-Experimental designs
Sometimes you just can’t perform a fully controlled experiment Because of the issue of interest Limited resources (not enough subjects, observations are too costly, etc). Surveys Correlational Quasi-Experiments Developmental designs Small-N designs This does NOT imply that they are bad designs Just remember the advantages and disadvantages of each Non-Experimental designs

65 Quasi-experiments What are they? General types
Almost “true” experiments, but with an inherent confounding variable General types An event occurs that the experimenter doesn’t manipulate Something not under the experimenter’s control (e.g., flashbulb memories for traumatic events) Interested in subject variables high vs. low IQ, males vs. females Time is used as a variable Quasi-experiments

66 Quasi-experiments Program evaluation
Research on programs that is implemented to achieve some positive effect on a group of individuals. e.g., does abstinence from sex program work in schools Steps in program evaluation Needs assessment - is there a problem? Program theory assessment - does program address the needs? Process evaluation - does it reach the target population? Is it being run correctly? Outcome evaluation - are the intended outcomes being realized? Efficiency assessment- was it “worth” it? The the benefits worth the costs? Quasi-experiments

67 Quasi-experiments Nonequivalent control group designs
with pretest and posttest (most common) (think back to the second control lecture) participants Experimental group Control Measure Non-Random Assignment Independent Variable Dependent Variable But remember that the results may be compromised because of the nonequivalent control group (review threats to internal validity) Quasi-experiments

68 Quasi-experiments Advantages Disadvantages
Allows applied research when experiments not possible Threats to internal validity can be assessed (sometimes) Disadvantages Threats to internal validity may exist Designs are more complex than traditional experiments Statistical analysis can be difficult Most statistical analyses assume randomness Quasi-experiments

69 Non-Experimental designs
Sometimes you just can’t perform a fully controlled experiment Because of the issue of interest Limited resources (not enough subjects, observations are too costly, etc). Surveys Correlational Quasi-Experiments Developmental designs Small-N designs This does NOT imply that they are bad designs Just remember the advantages and disadvantages of each Non-Experimental designs

70 Developmental designs
Used to study changes in behavior that occur as a function of age changes Age typically serves as a quasi-independent variable Three major types Cross-sectional Longitudinal Cohort-sequential Developmental designs

71 Developmental designs
Cross-sectional design Groups are pre-defined on the basis of a pre-existing variable Study groups of individuals of different ages at the same time Use age to assign participants to group Age is subject variable treated as a between-subjects variable Age 4 Age 7 Age 11 Developmental designs

72 Developmental designs
Cross-sectional design Advantages: Can gather data about different groups (i.e., ages) at the same time Participants are not required to commit for an extended period of time Developmental designs

73 Developmental designs
Cross-sectional design Disavantages: Individuals are not followed over time Cohort (or generation) effect: individuals of different ages may be inherently different due to factors in the environment Are 5 year old different from 15 year olds just because of age, or can factors present in their environment contribute to the differences? Imagine a 15yr old saying “back when I was 5 I didn’t have a Wii, my own cell phone, or a netbook” Does not reveal development of any particular individuals Cannot infer causality due to lack of control Developmental designs

74 Developmental designs
Longitudinal design Follow the same individual or group over time Age is treated as a within-subjects variable Rather than comparing groups, the same individuals are compared to themselves at different times Changes in dependent variable likely to reflect changes due to aging process Changes in performance are compared on an individual basis and overall Age 11 time Age 20 Age 15 Developmental designs

75 Longitudinal Designs Example Wisconsin Longitudinal Study (WLS)
Began in 1957 and is still on-going (50 years) 10,317 men and women who graduated from Wisconsin high schools in 1957 Originally studied plans for college after graduation Now it can be used as a test of aging and maturation Longitudinal Designs

76 Developmental designs
Longitudinal design Advantages: Can see developmental changes clearly Can measure differences within individuals Avoid some cohort effects (participants are all from same generation, so changes are more likely to be due to aging) Developmental designs

77 Developmental designs
Longitudinal design Disadvantages Can be very time-consuming Can have cross-generational effects: Conclusions based on members of one generation may not apply to other generations Numerous threats to internal validity: Attrition/mortality History Practice effects Improved performance over multiple tests may be due to practice taking the test Cannot determine causality Developmental designs

78 Developmental designs
Cohort-sequential design Measure groups of participants as they age Example: measure a group of 5 year olds, then the same group 10 years later, as well as another group of 5 year olds Age is both between and within subjects variable Combines elements of cross-sectional and longitudinal designs Addresses some of the concerns raised by other designs For example, allows to evaluate the contribution of cohort effects Developmental designs

79 Developmental designs
Cohort-sequential design Time of measurement Cross-sectional component 1975 1985 1995 Age 5 Age 15 Age 25 Cohort A 1970s Age 5 Age 15 Cohort B 1980s Age 5 Cohort C 1990s Longitudinal component Developmental designs

80 Developmental designs
Cohort-sequential design Advantages: Get more information Can track developmental changes to individuals Can compare different ages at a single time Can measure generation effect Less time-consuming than longitudinal (maybe) Disadvantages: Still time-consuming Need lots of groups of participants Still cannot make causal claims Developmental designs

81 Small N designs What are they?
Historically, these were the typical kind of design used until 1920’s when there was a shift to using larger sample sizes Even today, in some sub-areas, using small N designs is common place (e.g., psychophysics, clinical settings, expertise, etc.) Small N designs

82 Small N designs One or a few participants
Data are typically not analyzed statistically; rather rely on visual interpretation of the data Observations begin in the absence of treatment (BASELINE) Then treatment is implemented and changes in frequency, magnitude, or intensity of behavior are recorded Small N designs

83 Small N designs Baseline experiments – the basic idea is to show:
when the IV occurs, you get the effect when the IV doesn’t occur, you don’t get the effect (reversibility) Before introducing treatment (IV), baseline needs to be stable Measure level and trend Small N designs

84 Small N designs Level – how frequent (how intense) is behavior?
Are all the data points high or low? Trend – does behavior seem to increase (or decrease) Are data points “flat” or on a slope? Small N designs

85 ABA design ABA design (baseline, treatment, baseline)
The reversibility is necessary, otherwise something else may have caused the effect other than the IV (e.g., history, maturation, etc.) ABA design

86 Small N designs Advantages
Focus on individual performance, not fooled by group averaging effects Focus is on big effects (small effects typically can’t be seen without using large groups) Avoid some ethical problems – e.g., with non-treatments Allows to look at unusual (and rare) types of subjects (e.g., case studies of amnesics, experts vs. novices) Often used to supplement large N studies, with more observations on fewer subjects Small N designs

87 Small N designs Disadvantages
Effects may be small relative to variability of situation so NEED more observation Some effects are by definition between subjects Treatment leads to a lasting change, so you don’t get reversals Difficult to determine how generalizable the effects are Small N designs

88 Some researchers have argued that Small N designs are the best way to go.
The goal of psychology is to describe behavior of an individual Looking at data collapsed over groups “looks” in the wrong place Need to look at the data at the level of the individual Small N designs


Download ppt "Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology"

Similar presentations


Ads by Google