Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs"— Presentation transcript:

1 Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs
Event-related fMRI Rik Henson With thanks to: Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs

2 Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response
3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

3 Advantages of Event-related fMRI
1. Randomised trial order c.f. confounds of blocked designs (Johnson et al 1997)

4 Blocked Randomised Data Model O = Old Words N = New Words O1 O2 O3 N1

5 Advantages of Event-related fMRI
1. Randomised trial order c.f. confounds of blocked designs (Johnson et al 1997) 2. Post hoc / subjective classification of trials e.g, according to subsequent memory (Wagner et al 1998)

6 Event-Related ~4s R F R = Words Later Remembered
F = Words Later Forgotten Event-Related ~4s Data Model

7 Advantages of Event-related fMRI
1. Randomised trial order c.f. confounds of blocked designs (Johnson et al 1997) 2. Post hoc / subjective classification of trials e.g, according to subsequent memory (Wagner et al 1998) 3. Some events can only be indicated by subject (in time) e.g, spontaneous perceptual changes (Kleinschmidt et al 1998)

8

9 Advantages of Event-related fMRI
1. Randomised trial order c.f. confounds of blocked designs (Johnson et al 1997) 2. Post hoc / subjective classification of trials e.g, according to subsequent memory (Wagner et al 1998) 3. Some events can only be indicated by subject (in time) e.g, spontaneous perceptual changes (Kleinschmidt et al 1998) 4. Some trials cannot be blocked e.g, “oddball” designs (Clark et al., 2000)

10 “Oddball” Time

11 Advantages of Event-related fMRI
1. Randomised trial order c.f. confounds of blocked designs (Johnson et al 1997) 2. Post hoc / subjective classification of trials e.g, according to subsequent memory (Wagner et al 1998) 3. Some events can only be indicated by subject (in time) e.g, spontaneous perceptual changes (Kleinschmidt et al 1998) 4. Some trials cannot be blocked e.g, “oddball” designs (Clark et al., 2000) 5. More accurate models even for blocked designs? e.g, “state-item” interactions (Chawla et al, 1999)

12 Blocked Design “Epoch” model “Event” model
Data “Epoch” model Model O1 O2 O3 N1 N2 N3 N1 N2 N3 “Event” model O1 O2 O3

13 Epoch vs Events Sustained epoch Series of events =>
New in SPM2 Epoch vs Events Epochs are periods of sustained stimulation (e.g, box-car functions) Events are impulses (delta-functions) In SPM99, epochs and events are distinct (eg, in choice of basis functions) In SPM2, all conditions are specified in terms of their 1) onsets and 2) durations… … events simply have zero duration Near-identical regressors can be created by 1) sustained epochs, 2) rapid series of events (SOAs<~3s) i.e, designs can be blocked or intermixed … models can be epoch or event-related Boxcar function Sustained epoch Series of events Delta functions Convolved with HRF =>

14 Epoch vs Events Rate = 1/4s Rate = 1/2s Though blocks of trials can be modelled as boxcars or runs of events… … interpretation of the parameter estimates differs… Consider an experiment presenting words at different rates in different blocks: An “epoch” model will estimate parameter that increases with rate, because the parameter reflects response per block An “event” model may estimate parameter that decreases with rate, because the parameter reflects response per word b=3 b=5 b=11 b=9

15 (Disadvantages of Intermixed Designs)
1. Less efficient for detecting effects than are blocked designs (see later…) 2. Some psychological processes may be better blocked (eg task-switching, attentional instructions)

16 Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response
3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

17 BOLD Impulse Response Function of blood oxygenation, flow, volume (Buxton et al, 1998) Peak (max. oxygenation) 4-6s poststimulus; baseline after 20-30s Initial undershoot can be observed (Malonek & Grinvald, 1996) Similar across V1, A1, S1… … but differences across: other regions (Schacter et al 1997) individuals (Aguirre et al, 1998) Brief Stimulus Undershoot Initial Peak

18 BOLD Impulse Response Early event-related fMRI studies used a long Stimulus Onset Asynchrony (SOA) to allow BOLD response to return to baseline However, if the BOLD response is explicitly modelled, overlap between successive responses at short SOAs can be accommodated… … particularly if responses are assumed to superpose linearly Short SOAs are more sensitive… Brief Stimulus Undershoot Initial Peak

19 Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response
3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

20 General Linear (Convolution) Model
GLM for a single voxel: y(t) = u(t)  h(t) + (t) u(t) = neural causes (stimulus train) u(t) =   (t - nT) h(t) = hemodynamic (BOLD) response h(t) =  ßi fi (t) fi(t) = temporal basis functions y(t) =   ßi fi (t - nT) + (t) y = X ß ε T 2T 3T ... u(t) h(t)= ßi fi (t) convolution sampled each scan Design Matrix

21 General Linear Model (in SPM)
Auditory words every 20s SPM{F} time {secs} Gamma functions ƒi() of peristimulus time  (Orthogonalised) Sampled every TR = 1.7s Design matrix, X [x(t)ƒ1() | x(t)ƒ2() |...]

22 A word about down-sampling
T=16, TR=2s Scan 1 o T0=16 o T0=9 x2 x3

23 Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response
3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

24 Temporal Basis Functions
Fourier Set Windowed sines & cosines Any shape (up to frequency limit) Inference via F-test

25 Temporal Basis Functions
Finite Impulse Response Mini “timebins” (selective averaging) Any shape (up to bin-width) Inference via F-test

26 Temporal Basis Functions
Fourier Set Windowed sines & cosines Any shape (up to frequency limit) Inference via F-test Gamma Functions Bounded, asymmetrical (like BOLD) Set of different lags

27 Temporal Basis Functions
Fourier Set Windowed sines & cosines Any shape (up to frequency limit) Inference via F-test Gamma Functions Bounded, asymmetrical (like BOLD) Set of different lags “Informed” Basis Set Best guess of canonical BOLD response Variability captured by Taylor expansion “Magnitude” inferences via t-test…?

28 Temporal Basis Functions

29 Temporal Basis Functions
“Informed” Basis Set (Friston et al. 1998) Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions) Canonical

30 Temporal Basis Functions
“Informed” Basis Set (Friston et al. 1998) Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions) plus Multivariate Taylor expansion in: time (Temporal Derivative) Canonical Temporal

31 Temporal Basis Functions
“Informed” Basis Set (Friston et al. 1998) Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions) plus Multivariate Taylor expansion in: time (Temporal Derivative) width (Dispersion Derivative) Canonical Temporal Dispersion

32 Temporal Basis Functions
“Informed” Basis Set (Friston et al. 1998) Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions) plus Multivariate Taylor expansion in: time (Temporal Derivative) width (Dispersion Derivative) “Magnitude” inferences via t-test on canonical parameters (providing canonical is a good fit…more later) Canonical Temporal Dispersion

33 Temporal Basis Functions
“Informed” Basis Set (Friston et al. 1998) Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions) plus Multivariate Taylor expansion in: time (Temporal Derivative) width (Dispersion Derivative) “Magnitude” inferences via t-test on canonical parameters (providing canonical is a good fit…more later) “Latency” inferences via tests on ratio of derivative : canonical parameters (more later…) Canonical Temporal Dispersion

34 (Other Approaches) Long Stimulus Onset Asychrony (SOA)
Can ignore overlap between responses (Cohen et al 1997) … but long SOAs are less sensitive Fully counterbalanced designs Assume response overlap cancels (Saykin et al 1999) Include fixation trials to “selectively average” response even at short SOA (Dale & Buckner, 1997) … but unbalanced when events defined by subject Define HRF from pilot scan on each subject May capture intersubject variability (Zarahn et al, 1997) … but not interregional variability Numerical fitting of highly parametrised response functions Separate estimate of magnitude, latency, duration (Kruggel et al 1999) … but computationally expensive for every voxel

35 Temporal Basis Sets: Which One?
In this example (rapid motor response to faces, Henson et al, 2001)… Canonical + Temporal + Dispersion + FIR …canonical + temporal + dispersion derivatives appear sufficient …may not be for more complex trials (eg stimulus-delay-response) …but then such trials better modelled with separate neural components (ie activity no longer delta function) + constrained HRF (Zarahn, 1999)

36 Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response
3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

37 Timing Issues : Practical
TR=4s Scans Typical TR for 48 slice EPI at 3mm spacing is ~ 4s

38 Timing Issues : Practical
TR=4s Scans Typical TR for 48 slice EPI at 3mm spacing is ~ 4s Sampling at [0,4,8,12…] post- stimulus may miss peak signal Stimulus (synchronous) SOA=8s Sampling rate=4s

39 Timing Issues : Practical
TR=4s Scans Typical TR for 48 slice EPI at 3mm spacing is ~ 4s Sampling at [0,4,8,12…] post- stimulus may miss peak signal Higher effective sampling by: 1. Asynchrony eg SOA=1.5TR Stimulus (asynchronous) SOA=6s Sampling rate=2s

40 Timing Issues : Practical
TR=4s Scans Typical TR for 48 slice EPI at 3mm spacing is ~ 4s Sampling at [0,4,8,12…] post- stimulus may miss peak signal Higher effective sampling by: 1. Asynchrony eg SOA=1.5TR Random Jitter eg SOA=(2±0.5)TR Stimulus (random jitter) Sampling rate=2s

41 Timing Issues : Practical
TR=4s Scans Typical TR for 48 slice EPI at 3mm spacing is ~ 4s Sampling at [0,4,8,12…] post- stimulus may miss peak signal Higher effective sampling by: 1. Asynchrony eg SOA=1.5TR Random Jitter eg SOA=(2±0.5)TR Better response characterisation (Miezin et al, 2000) Stimulus (random jitter) Sampling rate=2s

42 Timing Issues : Practical
…but “Slice-timing Problem” (Henson et al, 1999) Slices acquired at different times, yet model is the same for all slices

43 Timing Issues : Practical
Top Slice Bottom Slice …but “Slice-timing Problem” (Henson et al, 1999) Slices acquired at different times, yet model is the same for all slices => different results (using canonical HRF) for different reference slices TR=3s SPM{t} SPM{t}

44 Timing Issues : Practical
Top Slice Bottom Slice …but “Slice-timing Problem” (Henson et al, 1999) Slices acquired at different times, yet model is the same for all slices => different results (using canonical HRF) for different reference slices Solutions: 1. Temporal interpolation of data … but less good for longer TRs TR=3s SPM{t} SPM{t} Interpolated SPM{t}

45 Timing Issues : Practical
Top Slice Bottom Slice …but “Slice-timing Problem” (Henson et al, 1999) Slices acquired at different times, yet model is the same for all slices => different results (using canonical HRF) for different reference slices Solutions: 1. Temporal interpolation of data … but less good for longer TRs 2. More general basis set (e.g., with temporal derivatives) … but inferences via F-test TR=3s SPM{t} SPM{t} Interpolated SPM{t} Derivative SPM{F}

46 Timing Issues : Latency
Assume the real response, r(t), is a scaled (by ) version of the canonical, f(t), but delayed by a small amount dt: r(t) =  f(t+dt) ~  f(t) +  f ´(t) dt st-order Taylor If the fitted response, R(t), is modelled by the canonical + temporal derivative: R(t) = ß1 f(t) + ß2 f ´(t) GLM fit Then canonical and derivative parameter estimates, ß1 and ß2, are such that :  = ß1 dt = ß2 / ß1 (Henson et al, 2002) (Liao et al, 2002) ie, Latency can be approximated by the ratio of derivative-to-canonical parameter estimates (within limits of first-order approximation, +/-1s)

47 Timing Issues : Latency
Delayed Responses (green/ yellow) Canonical Canonical Derivative Basis Functions ß1 ß2 Parameter Estimates ß2 /ß1 Actual latency, dt, vs. ß2 / ß1 Face repetition reduces latency as well as magnitude of fusiform response

48 Timing Issues : Latency
Neural BOLD A. Decreased B. Advanced C. Shortened (same integrated) D. Shortened (same maximum) A. Smaller Peak B. Earlier Onset C. Earlier Peak D. Smaller Peak and earlier Peak

49 Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response
3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

50 Fixed SOA = 16s  = Not particularly efficient… Stimulus (“Neural”)
HRF Predicted Data = Not particularly efficient…

51 Fixed SOA = 4s  = Very Inefficient… Stimulus (“Neural”) HRF
Predicted Data = Very Inefficient…

52 Randomised, SOAmin= 4s  = More Efficient… Stimulus (“Neural”) HRF
Predicted Data = More Efficient…

53 Blocked, SOAmin= 4s  = Even more Efficient… Stimulus (“Neural”) HRF
Predicted Data = Even more Efficient…

54 Blocked, epoch = 20s Stimulus (“Neural”) HRF Predicted Data = = Blocked-epoch (with small SOA) and Time-Freq equivalences

55 Sinusoidal modulation, f = 1/33s
Stimulus (“Neural”) HRF Predicted Data = = The most efficient design of all!

56  =  = Blocked (80s), SOAmin=4s, highpass filter = 1/120s
Stimulus (“Neural”) HRF Predicted Data “Effective HRF” (after highpass filtering) (Josephs & Henson, 1999) = = Don’t have long (>60s) blocks!

57 Randomised, SOAmin=4s, highpass filter = 1/120s
Stimulus (“Neural”) HRF Predicted Data = = (Randomised design spreads power over frequencies)

58 Design Efficiency T = cTb / var(cTb)
Events (A-B) T = cTb / var(cTb) Var(cTb) = sqrt(2cT(XTX)-1c) (i.i.d) For max. T, want min. contrast variability (Friston et al, 1999) If assume that noise variance (2) is unaffected by changes in X… …then want maximal efficiency, e: e(c,X) = { cT (XTX)-1 c }-1 = maximal bandpassed signal energy (Josephs & Henson, 1999)

59 Efficiency - Single Event-type
Design parametrised by: SOAmin Minimum SOA

60 Efficiency - Single Event-type
Design parametrised by: SOAmin Minimum SOA p(t) Probability of event at each SOAmin

61 Efficiency - Single Event-type
Design parametrised by: SOAmin Minimum SOA p(t) Probability of event at each SOAmin Deterministic p(t)=1 iff t=nT

62 Efficiency - Single Event-type
Design parametrised by: SOAmin Minimum SOA p(t) Probability of event at each SOAmin Deterministic p(t)=1 iff t=nSOAmin Stationary stochastic p(t)=constant<1

63 Efficiency - Single Event-type
Design parametrised by: SOAmin Minimum SOA p(t) Probability of event at each SOAmin Deterministic p(t)=1 iff t=nT Stationary stochastic p(t)=constant Dynamic stochastic p(t) varies (eg blocked) Blocked designs most efficient! (with small SOAmin)

64 Efficiency - Multiple Event-types
Design parametrised by: SOAmin Minimum SOA pi(h) Probability of event-type i given history h of last m events With n event-types pi(h) is a nm  n Transition Matrix Example: Randomised AB A B A B => ABBBABAABABAAA... 4s smoothing; 1/60s highpass filtering Differential Effect (A-B) Common Effect (A+B)

65 Efficiency - Multiple Event-types
Example: Alternating AB A B A 0 1 B 1 0 => ABABABABABAB... 4s smoothing; 1/60s highpass filtering Permuted (A-B) Alternating (A-B) Example: Permuted AB A B AA AB BA BB => ABBAABABABBA...

66 Efficiency - Multiple Event-types
Example: Null events A B A B => AB-BAA--B---ABB... Efficient for differential and main effects at short SOA Equivalent to stochastic SOA (Null Event like third unmodelled event-type) Selective averaging of data (Dale & Buckner 1997) 4s smoothing; 1/60s highpass filtering Null Events (A-B) Null Events (A+B)

67 Efficiency - Conclusions
Optimal design for one contrast may not be optimal for another Blocked designs generally most efficient with short SOAs (but earlier restrictions and problems of interpretation…) With randomised designs, optimal SOA for differential effect (A-B) is minimal SOA (assuming no saturation), whereas optimal SOA for main effect (A+B) is 16-20s Inclusion of null events improves efficiency for main effect at short SOAs (at cost of efficiency for differential effects) If order constrained, intermediate SOAs (5-20s) can be optimal; If SOA constrained, pseudorandomised designs can be optimal (but may introduce context-sensitivity)

68 Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response
3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

69 Nonlinear Model [u(t)]
Volterra series - a general nonlinear input-output model y(t) = 1[u(t)] + 2[u(t)] n[u(t)] n[u(t)] = ....  hn(t1,..., tn)u(t - t1) .... u(t - tn)dt dtn [u(t)] response y(t) input u(t) Stimulus function kernels (h) estimate

70 Nonlinear Model Friston et al (1997) kernel coefficients - h SPM{F}
SPM{F} testing H0: kernel coefficients, h = 0

71 Nonlinear Model Friston et al (1997) kernel coefficients - h SPM{F}
SPM{F} testing H0: kernel coefficients, h = 0 Significant nonlinearities at SOAs 0-10s: (e.g., underadditivity from 0-5s)

72 Underadditivity at short SOAs
Nonlinear Effects Underadditivity at short SOAs Linear Prediction Volterra Prediction

73 Underadditivity at short SOAs
Nonlinear Effects Underadditivity at short SOAs Linear Prediction Volterra Prediction

74 Underadditivity at short SOAs
Nonlinear Effects Underadditivity at short SOAs Linear Prediction Implications for Efficiency Volterra Prediction

75 Overview 1. Advantages of efMRI 2. BOLD impulse response
3. General Linear Model 4. Temporal Basis Functions 5. Timing Issues 6. Design Optimisation 7. Nonlinear Models 8. Example Applications

76 Example 1: Intermixed Trials (Henson et al 2000)
Short SOA, fully randomised, with 1/3 null events Faces presented for 0.5s against chequerboard baseline, SOA=(2 ± 0.5)s, TR=1.4s Factorial event-types: 1. Famous/Nonfamous (F/N) 2. 1st/2nd Presentation (1/2)

77 Lag=3 . . . Famous Nonfamous (Target)

78 Example 1: Intermixed Trials (Henson et al 2000)
Short SOA, fully randomised, with 1/3 null events Faces presented for 0.5s against chequerboard baseline, SOA=(2 ± 0.5)s, TR=1.4s Factorial event-types: 1. Famous/Nonfamous (F/N) 2. 1st/2nd Presentation (1/2) Interaction (F1-F2)-(N1-N2) masked by main effect (F+N) Right fusiform interaction of repetition priming and familiarity

79 Example 2: Post hoc classification (Henson et al 1999)
SPM{t} R-K SPM{t} K-R Subjects indicate whether studied (Old) words: i) evoke recollection of prior occurrence (R) ii) feeling of familiarity without recollection (K) iii) no memory (N) Random Effects analysis on canonical parameter estimate for event-types Fixed SOA of 8s => sensitive to differential but not main effect (de/activations arbitrary)

80 Example 3: Subject-defined events (Portas et al 1999)
Subjects respond when “pop-out” of 3D percept from 2D stereogram

81

82 Example 3: Subject-defined events (Portas et al 1999)
Subjects respond when “pop-out” of 3D percept from 2D stereogram Popout response also produces tone Control event is response to tone during 3D percept Temporo-occipital differential activation Pop-out Control

83 Example 4: Oddball Paradigm (Strange et al, 2000)
16 same-category words every 3 secs, plus … … 1 perceptual, 1 semantic, and 1 emotional oddball

84 Perceptual Oddball Semantic Oddball Emotional Oddball ~3s WHEAT BARLEY
CORN Perceptual Oddball BARLEY OATS PLUG Semantic Oddball RYE RAPE Emotional Oddball HOPS ~3s

85 Example 4: Oddball Paradigm (Strange et al, 2000)
Right Prefrontal Cortex 16 same-category words every 3 secs, plus … … 1 perceptual, 1 semantic, and 1 emotional oddball 3 nonoddballs randomly matched as controls Conjunction of oddball vs. control contrast images: generic deviance detector Parameter Estimates Controls Oddballs

86 Example 5: Item/State effects (Chawla et al 1999)
Visual stimulus = dots periodically changing in colour or motion Epochs of attention to: 1) motion, or 2) colour Events are target stimuli differing in motion or colour Randomised, long SOAs between events (targets) to decorrelate epoch and event-related covariates Attention modulates BOTH: 1) baseline activity (state-effect, additive) 2) evoked response (item-effect, multiplicative)

87 Example 5: Item/State effects (Chawla et al 1999)
V Motion change under attention to motion (red) or color (blue) Item Effect (Evoked) State Effect (Baseline) V Color change under attention to motion (red) or color (blue)

88

89 Efficiency – Detection vs Estimation
“Detection power” vs “Estimation efficiency” (Liu et al, 2001) Detect response, or characterise shape of response? Maximal detection power in blocked designs; Maximal estimation efficiency in randomised designs => simply corresponds to choice of basis functions: detection = canonical HRF estimation = FIR

90 Design Efficiency HRF can be viewed as a filter (Josephs & Henson, 1999) Want to maximise the signal passed by this filter Dominant frequency of canonical HRF is ~0.04 Hz So most efficient design is a sinusoidal modulation of neural activity with period ~24s (eg, boxcar with 12s on/ 12s off)

91 Mixed Designs Recent interest in both transient (“item- or event-related”) and sustained (“state- or epoch-related”) fMRI activity What is the best design to estimate both…?

92 A bit more formally… “Efficiency”
Efficiency, e (see earlier): e(c,X) = { cT (XTX)-1 c }-1 XTX represents covariance of regressors in design matrix High covariance increases elements of (XTX)-1 So, when correlation between regressors, efficiency reduced

93 Item effect only… Efficiency = 565 (Item Effect)
Blocks = 40s, Fixed SOA = 4s Design Matrix (X) Efficiency = 565 (Item Effect) OK…

94 Item and State effects Efficiency = 16 (Item Effect)
Blocks = 40s, Fixed SOA = 4s Design Matrix (X) Efficiency = 16 (Item Effect) Correlation = .97 Not good…

95 Item and State effects Efficiency = 54 (Item Effect)
Blocks = 40s, Randomised SOAmin= 2s Design Matrix (X) Efficiency = 54 (Item Effect) Correlation = .78 Better!

96 Mixed Designs (Chawla et al 1999)
Visual stimulus = dots periodically changing in colour or motion Epochs of attention to: 1) motion, or 2) colour Events are target stimuli differing in motion or colour Randomised, long SOAs between events (targets) to decorrelate epoch and event-related covariates Attention modulates BOTH: 1) baseline activity (state-effect, additive) 2) evoked response (item-effect, multiplicative)

97 Mixed Designs (Chawla et al 1999)
V Motion change under attention to motion (red) or color (blue) Item Effect (Evoked) State Effect (Baseline) V Color change under attention to motion (red) or color (blue)

98 BOLD Response Latency (Iterative)
Height Peak Delay Onset Delay Numerical fitting of explicitly parameterised canonical HRF (Henson et al, 2001) Distinguishes between Onset and Peak latency… …unlike temporal derivative… …and which may be important for interpreting neural changes (see previous slide) Distribution of parameters tested nonparametrically (Wilcoxon’s T over subjects)

99 BOLD Response Latency (Iterative)
240ms Peak Delay wT(11)=14, p<.05 0.34% Height Change wT(11)=5, p<.001 No difference in Onset Delay, wT(11)=35 D. Shortened (same maximum) Neural D. Smaller Peak and earlier Peak BOLD Most parsimonious account is that repetition reduces duration of neural activity…

100 BOLD Response Latency (Iterative)
Height Peak Delay Onset Delay Dispersion Different fits across subjects Four-parameter HRF, nonparametric Random Effects (SNPM99) Advantages of iterative vs linear: Height “independent” of shape Canonical “height” confounded by latency (e.g, different shapes across subjects); no slice-timing error 2. Distinction of onset/peak latency Allowing better neural inferences? Disadvantages of iterative: 1. Unreasonable fits (onset/peak tension) Priors on parameter distributions? (Bayesian estimation) 2. Local minima, failure of convergence? 3. CPU time (~3 days for above) FIR used to deconvolve data, before nonlinear fitting over PST Height p<.05 (cor) 1-2 SNPM

101 Temporal Basis Sets: Inferences
How can inferences be made in hierarchical models (eg, “Random Effects” analyses over, for example, subjects)? 1. Univariate T-tests on canonical parameter alone? may miss significant experimental variability canonical parameter estimate not appropriate index of “magnitude” if real responses are non-canonical (see later) 2. Univariate F-tests on parameters from multiple basis functions? need appropriate corrections for nonsphericity (Glaser et al, 2001) 3. Multivariate tests (eg Wilks Lambda, Henson et al, 2000) not powerful unless ~10 times as many subjects as parameters

102  =  =  = r() s(t) u(t) u(t) h() x(t) u(t) h() x(t) PST (s)
Time (s) Time (s) u(t) h() x(t) = Time (s) PST (s) Time (s) u(t) h() x(t) = Time (s) PST (s) Time (s)

103 A B C Peak Dispersion Undershoot Initial Dip

104 SOA=2s SOA=16s A+B Ran A-B Alt A-B


Download ppt "Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs"

Similar presentations


Ads by Google