Download presentation
Presentation is loading. Please wait.
Published byMoritz Koch Modified over 6 years ago
1
Issues in measuring sensory-motor control performance of human drivers: The case of cognitive load and steering control Johan Engström, Volvo Technology Corporation European Workshop on Advanced Predictive Sensory-motor Control Joudkrante, Lithuania,
2
Vehicle & Load Structures
Outline Multitasking in the vehicle Secondary tasks – visual and cognitive The primary driving task – visual control of steering Effects of secondary tasks on steering control Different effects of visual and cognitive tasks The ”lane keeping improvement” effect of cognitive load Possible explanation in terms of satisficing vs. optimising steering control Testing predictions implied by this hypothesis Vehicle & Load Structures
3
Multitasking in the vehicle: Driving + secondary tasks
Vehicle & Load Structures
4
Secondary tasks: Visual vs. cognitive distraction
Visual distraction Looking off road E.g. Visual time sharing when tuning the radio Cognitive distraction: Engaging in demanding cognitive (working memory) tasks E.g. Mobile phone conversation Most real-world tasks involve both components… Vehicle & Load Structures
5
The primary driving task: Sensory-motor control in steering
Vehicle & Load Structures
6
The visual control of steering: Optical and retinal flow
Wann and Wilke (2000) Straight driving, looking ahead Straight driving, looking to the left side Retinal flow not equal to optical flow Vehicle & Load Structures
7
Using retinal flow patterns to guide steering: Look where you’re going
resulting heading initial heading Wann and Wilke (2000) Underrsteering Going towards target Oversteering Vehicle & Load Structures
8
Gaze angle can be used as a direct cue for steering through curves (Land, 1998)
Fixate tangent point and adjust steering to keep gaze angle constant Vehicle & Load Structures
9
Vehicle & Load Structures
Combining retinal flow patterns and gaze direction: ”Spring” model (Wann and Wilkie, 2000) Stiffness Angular acceleration Reliance on cues Damping Main point: Foveal vision is essential for accurate steering! Vehicle & Load Structures
10
Effects of secondary tasks on steering control
Vehicle & Load Structures
11
Effects of visual distraction on lateral control
Visual time sharing Gaze angle Looking away Loosing visual input for steering control Heading error builds up Looking back Large steering wheel correction Speed reduction to compensate Steering wheel angle Increased lane position variance Lane position Speed Engström and Markkula (2006) Vehicle & Load Structures
12
What about purely cognitive distraction?
Large number of simulator and real-world driving studies found reduced lane keeping variance during cognitive load (Brookhuis et al., 1991; Östlund et al., 2004; Jamson and Merat, 2005; Engström et al., 2005; Mattes, Föhl and Schindhelm, 2007; Merat and Jamson, 2008). Engström, Johansson and Östlund (2005) Does talking on the mobile phone really improve steering control? SD lane position Cognitive task difficulty Vehicle & Load Structures
13
Other effects of cognitive distraction: Gaze concentration
Victor, Harbluk and Engström (2005) Normal driving Cognitive task Vehicle & Load Structures
14
Vehicle & Load Structures
Other effects of cognitive distraction: Increased steering activity (number of steering reversals > 2 deg. per minute) SW reversals/min Engström et al. (2005) Vehicle & Load Structures
15
Summary: Effects of cognitive distraction related to lateral control
Improved lane keeping (!?) Gaze concentration towards the road centre Increased number of micro steering corrections (<2 deg) How are these effects related? How can they be explained? Vehicle & Load Structures
16
Vehicle & Load Structures
Possible explanation Vehicle & Load Structures
17
Vehicle & Load Structures
Two key distinctions Satisficing vs. Optimising Top-down (endogenous) vs. Bottom-up (exogenous) attention selection Vehicle & Load Structures
18
1. Satisficing vs. optimising
Target value Comfort zone Optimising: Minimising performance error relative to a target state. Satisficing: Maintaining performance within acceptable boundaries. Vehicle & Load Structures
19
Example cost functions of optimising and satisficing in lane keeping
Lane position Lane centre Vehicle & Load Structures
20
Example dynamics of satisficing and optimising
X_dot Satisficing Comfort zone Optimising X Vehicle & Load Structures
21
Vehicle & Load Structures
Simulations Satisficing Optimising Vehicle & Load Structures
22
2. Bottom-up and top-down attention selection
attention bias 2. Bottom-up and top-down attention selection Top-down selection Cognitive task Other visual task Vehicle dynamics Steering Bottom-up selection Vehicle & Load Structures
23
Vehicle & Load Structures
Top-down attention bias Normal driving Steering easy and automated task, bottom-up-driven -> satisficing Top-down selection Other visual task Vehicle dynamics Steering Spare top-down attentional resources used for other visual tasks Bottom-up selection visual time sharing Lane keeping variance Distributed gaze Only intermittent steering Vehicle & Load Structures
24
Vehicle & Load Structures
Top-down attention bias Cognitive load Top-down selection Top-down attention allocated to cognitive task Cognitive task Other visual task No top-down-initiation of other visual tasks Steering Vehicle dynamics Gaze can be fully devoted to steering (attracted bottom-up) Bottom-up selection Reduced lane keeping variance Gaze concentration Vehicle & Load Structures
25
Vehicle & Load Structures
Testable predictions General: Improved lane keeping should only occur if the driver is satisficing in baseline condition Specific predictions: Improved lane keeping should not occur if the steering task is difficult (so that satisficing is not possible) Improved lane keeping effect should not occur if the driver is motivated to optimise lane keeping Support for prediction 1 Cognitive load has been demonstrated to impair performance on tracking tasks (Creem and Proffitt, 2001; Strayer and Drews. 2001). These tasks could be expected to be more difficult and/or less automated than normal driving Prediction 2: Tested experimentally… Vehicle & Load Structures
26
Instruction to optimise steering (baseline)
Top-down attention bias Top-down selection Top-down attention allocated to steering task and cognitive task Other visual task Optimising steering performance Steering Vehicle dynamics Bottom-up selection Reduced lane keeping variance Gaze concentration Increased steering wheel control input Vehicle & Load Structures
27
Testing prediction 2: Experimental design
Simulator study in fixed based simulator (at Saab Automobile, Trollhättan) Cognitive task: Count backwards with 7 48 subjects, split in 4 groups: Incentive for group 1 and 2: Two cinema tickets instead of one if meeting some (unspecified) lane keeping criterion Instruction to keep in the middle of the lane Yes No Cognitive task Group 1 Group 3 Group 2 Group 4 Vehicle & Load Structures
28
Vehicle & Load Structures
Prediction Lane keeping improvement effect of cognitive load should only occur when the driver is not motivated to optimise lane keeping = satisficing Interaction between cognitive load and instruction to optimise Vehicle & Load Structures
29
Preliminary results: Lane keeping (HP-filtered SD Lane Position)
No cognitive task Effect only for non-instructed subjects Due to satisficing in baseline condition Cognitive task Instructed to optimise lane keeping No instruction Vehicle & Load Structures
30
Steering wheel reversal rate
Cognitive task Same effect in both conditions Cognitive load less efficient optimising: more steering – same lane keeping performance No cognitive task No instruction Instructed to optimise lane keeping Vehicle & Load Structures
31
Vehicle & Load Structures
Still to be analysed… Eye movements Speed change Performance on cognitive task Vehicle & Load Structures
32
Vehicle & Load Structures
Discussion Replicated earlier findings for non-instructed drivers: Reduced lane keeping performance Increased steering wheel activity Predicted effect of instructions found -> improved lane keeping only for non-instructed drivers – due to satisficing in baseline condition Cognitive load seems to induce less efficient steering while optimising (more effort in steering, same result on lane keeping) Cognitive task does not really improve steering ability-> the effect rather reflects ”involuntary” improvement from ”sloppy” baseline driving Vehicle & Load Structures
33
Vehicle & Load Structures
General conclusions Caution is needed when interpreting driving performance measurements – do we compare to a baseline with satisficing or optimising performance? In this case, changing instructions and/or driving task difficulty may cancel or perhaps even reverse the effect of cognitive load Implies re-interpretation of many existing studies on the effects of cell phone conversation on driving performance (e.g. Strayer and Drews, 2001) Vehicle & Load Structures
Similar presentations
© 2024 SlidePlayer.com Inc.
All rights reserved.