Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Cartography and Chronometry

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Cartography and Chronometry"— Presentation transcript:

1 Cartography and Chronometry
fMRI Graduate Course October 9, 2002

2 Why do you need to know? Spatial resolution Temporal resolution
Tradeoffs between coverage and spatial resolution Influences viability of preprocessing steps Temporal resolution Tradeoffs between number of slices and TR Needed resolution depends upon design Non-linearity of the hemodynamic response Limits experimental designs Affects subsequent analyses Reduces power

3 Spatial Resolution

4 What spatial resolution do we want?
Hemispheric Lateralization studies Selective attention studies Systems / lobic Relation to lesion data Centimeter Identification of active regions Millimeter Topographic mapping (e.g., motor, vision) Sub-millimeter Ocular Dominance Columns Cortical Layers

5 What determines Spatial Resolution?
Voxel Size In-plane Resolution Slice thickness Spatial noise Head motion Artifacts Spatial blurring Smoothing (within subject) Coregistration (within subject) Normalization (within subject) Averaging (across subjects)

6 K – Space Revisited A B FOV: 10cm, Pixel Size: 2 cm FOV: 10 cm, Pixel Size: 1 cm To increase spatial resolution we need to sample at higher spatial frequencies.

7 How large are functional voxels?
= ~.08cm3  5.0mm   3.75mm   3.75mm  Within a typical brain (~1300cm3), there may be about 20,000 functional voxels.

8 How large are anatomical voxels?
= ~.004cm3  5.0mm   .9375mm   .9375mm  Within a typical brain (~1300cm3), there may be about 300,000+ anatomical voxels.

9 Costs of Increased Spatial Resolution
Acquisition Time In-plane Higher resolution takes more time to fill K-space (resolution ~ size of K-space) #Slices/second Sample rates for 64*64 images Early Duke fMRI: 2-4 sl/s GE EPI: 12 sl/s Duke Spiral (1.5T): 14 sl/s Duke Inverse Spiral (4.0T): 21 sl/s Reduced signal per voxel What is our dependent measure?

10 Effects of Stimulus Duration on Spatial Extent of Activity

11 Example: Ocular Dominance
Goodyear & Menon, 2001

12 4sec  10sec  Goodyear & Menon, 2001

13 Example: Visual System
100ms 500ms 1500 ms

14 T2* Blurring Signal decays over time needed for collection of an image
For standard resolution images, this is not a critical issue However, for high-resolution (in-plane) images, the time to acquire an image may be a significant fraction of T2* Under these conditions, multi-shot imaging may be necessary.

15 Temporal Resolution

16 What temporal resolution do we want?
10,000ms: Change in arousal or emotional state 1000ms: Decisions, recall from memory ms: Response time 250ms: Reaction time 10-100ms: Difference between response times Initial visual processing 10ms: Neuronal activity in one area

17 Basic Sampling Theory Nyquist Sampling Theorem
To be able to identify changes at frequency X, one must sample the data at 2X. For example, if your task causes brain changes at 1 Hz (every second), you must take two images per second.

18

19 Aliasing Mismapping of high frequencies (above the Nyquist limit) to lower frequencies Results from insufficient sampling Potential problem for designs with long TRs and fast stimulus changes

20 Frequency Analyses t < -1.96 t < +1.96 McCarthy et al., 1996

21 Phase Analyses Design Task frequency: Left/right alternating flashes
6.4s for each Task frequency: 1 / 12.8 = 0.078 McCarthy et al., 1996

22 Why do we want to measure differences in timing within a brain region?
Determine relative ordering of activity Make inferences about connectivity Anatomical Functional Relate activity timing to other measures Stimulus presentation Reaction time Relative amplitude

23 Timing Differences across Regions
Presented left hemifield before right hemifield (0-1000ms delays) fMRI vs RT (LH) Plot of LH signal as function of RH signal fMRI vs. Stimulus Menon et al., 1998

24 Activation maps Relative onset time differences Menon et al., 1998

25 V1 FFG We also investigated the latency of the hemodynamic response across brain regions. We found, in both elderly and young adults, that the hemodynamic response in primary visual cortex anticipates that in fusiform cortex by about 300 ms. This result has since been replicated using face stimuli. Huettel et al., 2001

26 Subject 1 Subject 2 5.5s 4.0s Secondary Visual Cortex (FFG)
Primary Visual Cortex (V1) These figures demonstrate this effect on an individual voxel level. The overlaid color maps are not traditional significance maps; they are maps of latency to hemodynamic peak, with earliest responses in blue and latest responses in yellow. As can be seen, on the left image, activation in the fusiform gyri generally has a much later peak than that in calcarine cortex, shown at right. Huettel et al., 2001

27 Linearity of the Hemodynamic Response

28 Linear Systems Scaling Superposition
The ratio of inputs determines the ratio of outputs Example: if Input1 is twice as large as Input2, Output1 will be twice as large as Output2 Superposition The response to a sum of inputs is equivalent to the sum of the response to individual inputs Example: Output1+2+3 = Output1+Output2+Output3

29 Possible Sources of Nonlinearity
Stimulus time course  neural activity Activity not uniform across stimulus (for any stimulus) Neural activity  Vascular changes Different activity durations may lead to different blood flow or oxygen extraction Minimum bolus size? Minimum activity necessary to trigger? Vascular changes  BOLD measurement Saturation of BOLD response necessitates nonlinearity Vascular measures combining to generate BOLD have different time courses From Buxton, 2001

30 Effects of Stimulus Duration
Short stimulus durations evoke BOLD responses Amplitude of BOLD response often depends on duration Stimuli < 100ms evoke measurable BOLD responses Form of response changes with duration Latency to peak increases with increasing duration Onset of rise does not change with duration Rate of rise increases with duration Key issue: deconfounding duration of stimulus with duration of neuronal activity

31 Boynton et al., 1996 Linear model for HDR
Varied contrast of checkerboard bars as well as their interval (B) and duration (C).

32 Boynton, et al, 1996

33 Boynton, et al, 1996

34 Differences in Nonlinearity across Brain Regions
Birn, et al, 2001

35 SMA vs. M1 Birn, et al, 2001

36 Caveat: Stimulus Duration ≠ Neuronal Activity Duration

37 fMRI Hemodynamic Response
1500ms 500ms 100ms Calcarine Sulci Fusiform Gyri

38 Calcarine 1500ms 500ms * Fusiform 100ms

39 Refractory Periods Definition: a change in the responsiveness to an event based upon the presence or absence of a similar preceding event Neuronal refractory period Vascular refractory period

40 Dale & Buckner, 1997 Responses to consecutive presentations of a stimulus add in a “roughly linear” fashion Subtle departures from linearity are evident

41 Intra-Pair Interval (IPI)
Inter-Trial Interval (16-20 seconds) 6 sec IPI 4 sec IPI 2 sec IPI 1 sec IPI Single-Stimulus Our basic design was derived from electrophysiological studies of refractory periods. We presented either a single short duration visual checkerboard, or a pair of checkerboards separated by an intra-pair interval of either 1, 2, 4, or 6 seconds. A long inter-trial interval ensured that the hemodynamic response returned to baseline before the onset of the next trial. Our hypothesis was that the second stimulus in the pair would have relatively little effect upon the composite waveform at short intervals, like 1 or 2 seconds, but would have a large effect at long intervals. That is, the hemodynamic response would be relatively linearly additive at long-intervals, but non-linear at short intervals. 500 ms duration Huettel & McCarthy, 2000

42 Methods and Analysis 16 male subjects (mean age: 27y) GE 1.5T scanner
CAMRD Gradient-echo EPI TR : 1 sec TE : 50 msec Resolution: * * 7 mm Analysis Voxel-based analyses Waveforms derived from active voxels within anatomical ROI The study was conducted at 1.5T in the center for advanced magnetic resonance development at Duke. We took two echo-planar slices chosen to bracket the calcarine sulcus in each subject, and sampled those slices with repetition time of 1 sec. In each subject we identified a functional ROI consisting of contiguous active voxels in calcarine cortex. Huettel & McCarthy, 2000

43 Hemodynamic Responses to Closely Spaced Stimuli
These graphs show the time courses of fMRI activation in calcarine cortex. The yellow line that is repeated in each graph shows the response to a single stimulus. The colored lines show the response to pairs of stimuli. Readily apparent is the contribution of the second stimulus above that of the single stimulus condition. To determine how large of a hemodynamic response was evoked by the second stimulus, we took the residual area between the two curves (the additive effect of the second stimulus), and we time-locked that difference to the onset of the second stimulus. Huettel & McCarthy, 2000

44 Refractory Effects in the fMRI Hemodynamic Response
Signal Change over Baseline(%) The independent contribution of the second stimulus is shown on this plot. The yellow line shows the response to a single stimulus. Readily apparent are the significant refractory effects. At 1 second intervals, the response to the second stimulus is attenuated in amplitude by about 45% and is increased in latency by about a second. Both amplitude and latency values recover to near single-stimulus values by about six seconds. Time since onset of second stimulus (sec) Huettel & McCarthy, 2000

45 Refractory Effects across Visual Regions
HDRs to 1st and 2nd stimuli in a pair (calcarine cortex) Relative amplitude of 2nd stimulus in pair across regions

46 6 sec IPI 1 sec IPI Single-Stimulus Intra-Pair Interval (IPI)
Inter-Trial Interval (16-20 seconds) 6 sec IPI 1 sec IPI Single-Stimulus

47 L R Figure 2 Single 6s IPI 1s IPI Mean HDRs
Mean HDRs L Time since stimulus onset (sec) Signal Change over baseline (%) Single 6s IPI 1s IPI R

48 Refractory Effect Summary
Duration HDR evoked by a long-duration stimulus is less than predicted by convolution of short-duration stimuli Present for durations < ~6s Interstimulus interval HDR evoked by a stimulus is reduced by a preceding similar stimulus Present for intervals < ~6s Differences across brain regions Some regions show considerable departures from linearity May result from differences in processing Source of non-linearity not well understood Neuronal effects comprise at least part of the overall effect Vascular differences may also contribute

49 Using refractory effects to study cognition: fMRI Adaptation Studies

50 Neuronal Adaptation Grill-Spector & Malach, 2001
Several neuronal populations vs. homogeneous population Adaptation If neurons are insensitive to the feature being varied, then their activity will adapt. Viewpoint Sensitive Viewpoint Invariant

51 Lateral Occipital Posterior Fusiform

52 Overall Summary Spatial resolution Temporal resolution
Advantages (of increasing) Smaller voxels allow distinction among areas Disadvantages Require more slices, thus longer TR Reduces signal per voxel Temporal resolution Improves sampling of hemodynamic response Reduces # of slices per TR May not be necessary for some designs Non-linearity of hemodynamic response Advantages (of phenomenon for design) May be used to study adaptation Reduces power of short interval designs Must be accounted for in analyses


Download ppt "Cartography and Chronometry"

Similar presentations


Ads by Google