The Practice of Statistics, 5th Edition Starnes, Tabor, Yates, Moore Bedford Freeman Worth Publishers CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies 4.2Experiments.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
Advertisements

+ Experiments How to Experiment Well: The RandomizedComparative Experiment The remedy for confounding is to perform a comparative experiment in which some.
The Practice of Statistics – For AP* STARNES, YATES, MOORE
DESIGNING EXPERIMENTS
+ Chapter 4 Designing Studies 4.1Samples and Surveys 4.2Experiments 4.3Using Studies Wisely.
Warm-Up Grab a sheet and review!. Section 4.2 Experiments Some of these things are going to look familiar, but remember…we are talking about different.
+ The Practice of Statistics, 4 th edition – For AP* STARNES, YATES, MOORE Chapter 4: Designing Studies Section 4.2 Experiments.
The Practice of Statistics, 5th Edition Starnes, Tabor, Yates, Moore Bedford Freeman Worth Publishers CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies 4.2Experiments.
 Get out your homework and materials for notes!  Take-home quiz due!
Lecture PowerPoint Slides Basic Practice of Statistics 7 th Edition.
CHAPTER 9: Producing Data: Experiments. Chapter 9 Concepts 2  Observation vs. Experiment  Subjects, Factors, Treatments  How to Experiment Badly 
CHAPTER 9: Producing Data Experiments ESSENTIAL STATISTICS Second Edition David S. Moore, William I. Notz, and Michael A. Fligner Lecture Presentation.
Lecture PowerPoint Slides Basic Practice of Statistics 7 th Edition.
Producing Data (C11-13 BVD) C13: Experiments and Observational Studies.
The Practice of Statistics, 5th Edition Starnes, Tabor, Yates, Moore Bedford Freeman Worth Publishers CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies 4.2Experiments.
+ The Practice of Statistics, 4 th edition – For AP* STARNES, YATES, MOORE Chapter 4: Designing Studies Section 4.2 Experiments.
The Practice of Statistics, 5th Edition Starnes, Tabor, Yates, Moore Bedford Freeman Worth Publishers CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies 4.2Experiments.
+ Experiments Experiments: What Can Go Wrong? The logic of a randomized comparative experiment dependson our ability to treat all the subjects the same.
The Practice of Statistics, 5th Edition Starnes, Tabor, Yates, Moore Bedford Freeman Worth Publishers CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies 4.2Experiments.
+ Experiments Observational Study versus Experiment In contrast to observational studies, experiments don’t just observe individuals or ask them questions.
Producing Data: Experiments BPS - 5th Ed. Chapter 9 1.
CHAPTER 9: Producing Data Experiments ESSENTIAL STATISTICS Second Edition David S. Moore, William I. Notz, and Michael A. Fligner Lecture Presentation.
CHAPTER 9: Producing Data Experiments ESSENTIAL STATISTICS Second Edition David S. Moore, William I. Notz, and Michael A. Fligner Lecture Presentation.
Experiments Textbook 4.2. Observational Study vs. Experiment Observational Studies observes individuals and measures variables of interest, but does not.
 Get out your homework and materials for notes!  If you have your parent letter, supplies, or AP contract, please put them on my desk.
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
Take-home quiz due! Get out materials for notes!
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
4.2 Experiments.
Chapter 3 Producing Data
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
CHAPTER 9: Producing Data— Experiments
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
Statistical Reasoning December 8, 2015 Chapter 6.2
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
10/22/ A Observational Studies and Experiments.
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
10/28/ B Experimental Design.
Chapter 4: Designing Studies
If you have your parent letter, please turn in at my desk (scissors on my desk). Get out your homework and materials for notes!
Presentation transcript:

The Practice of Statistics, 5th Edition Starnes, Tabor, Yates, Moore Bedford Freeman Worth Publishers CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies 4.2Experiments

Learning Objectives After this section, you should be able to: The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition2 DISTINGUISH between an observational study and an experiment. EXPLAIN the concept of confounding. IDENTIFY the experimental units, explanatory and response variables, and treatments in an experiment. EXPLAIN the purpose of comparison, random assignment, control, and replication in an experiment. DESCRIBE a completely randomized design for an experiment. DESCRIBE the placebo effect and the purpose of blinding in an experiment. INTERPRET the meaning of statistically significant in the context of an experiment. EXPLAIN the purpose of blocking in an experiment. DESCRIBE a randomized block design or a matched pairs design for an experiment. Experiments

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition3 Observational Study vs. Experiment An observational study observes individuals and measures variables of interest but does not attempt to influence the responses. An experiment deliberately imposes some treatment on individuals to measure their responses. An observational study observes individuals and measures variables of interest but does not attempt to influence the responses. An experiment deliberately imposes some treatment on individuals to measure their responses. When our goal is to understand cause and effect, experiments are the only source of fully convincing data. The distinction between observational study and experiment is one of the most important in statistics.

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition4 See example on p.235

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition5 Observational Study vs. Experiment Observational studies of the effect of an explanatory variable on a response variable often fail because of confounding between the explanatory variable and one or more other variables. Well-designed experiments take steps to prevent confounding. Confounding occurs when two variables are associated in such a way that their effects on a response variable cannot be distinguished from each other. To explain confounding you should explain how the variable you chose is associated with the explanatory variable and also how it affects the response variable.

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition6 AP Exam Tip on p.236 –“Diet is a confounding variable because people with bad diets are more likely to have a heart attack” Need to explain why diet is also associated with the choice to take hormones. Would need to say something about why a better diet might be more common in the group of women who choose to take hormones. AP common error on p.236 –For the hormone example you would need to explain not only that wealthier people tend to go to the doctor more often and to be healthier overall, but also that wealthier people are more likely to get hormone replacement. Thus, it could actually be the overall healthiness of wealthy women that caused the reduction in heart attacks, not any hormones they may have taken. CYU on p.237

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition7 The Language of Experiments An experiment is a statistical study in which we actually do something (a treatment) to people, animals, or objects (the experimental units) to observe the response. Here is the basic vocabulary of experiments. A specific condition applied to the individuals in an experiment is called a treatment. If an experiment has several explanatory variables, a treatment is a combination of specific values of these variables. The experimental units are the smallest collection of individuals to which treatments are applied. When the units are human beings, they often are called subjects. A specific condition applied to the individuals in an experiment is called a treatment. If an experiment has several explanatory variables, a treatment is a combination of specific values of these variables. The experimental units are the smallest collection of individuals to which treatments are applied. When the units are human beings, they often are called subjects. Often times the explanatory variables in an experiment are called factors. If an experiment has several factors, the combinations of each level of each factor form the treatments.

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition8 See example on p http:// –Describes a multifactor experiment to determine if money can buy hapiness. –There are 2 factors, amount of money & how it is spent.

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition9 How to Experiment Badly Many laboratory experiments use a design like the one in the online SAT course example on p.240: Experimental Units Treatment Measure Response In the lab environment, simple designs often work well. Field experiments and experiments with animals or people deal with more variable conditions. Outside the lab, badly designed experiments often yield worthless results because of confounding.

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition10 How to Experiment Well The remedy for confounding is to perform a comparative experiment in which some units receive one treatment and similar units receive another. Most well designed experiments compare two or more treatments. Comparison alone isn’t enough, if the treatments are given to groups that differ greatly, bias will result. The solution to the problem of bias is random assignment. In an experiment, random assignment means that experimental units are assigned to treatments using a chance process. See example on p.241

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition11 Principles of Experimental Design The basic principles for designing experiments are as follows: 1. Comparison. Use a design that compares two or more treatments. 2. Random assignment. Use chance to assign experimental units to treatments. Doing so helps create roughly equivalent groups of experimental units by balancing the effects of other variables among the treatment groups. 3. Control. Keep other variables that might affect the response the same for all groups. 4. Replication. Use enough experimental units in each group so that any differences in the effects of the treatments can be distinguished from chance differences between the groups. Principles of Experimental Design

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition12 See example on p.243

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition13 Completely Randomized Design In a completely randomized design, the treatments are assigned to all the experimental units completely by chance. Some experiments may include a control group that receives an inactive treatment or an existing baseline treatment. In a completely randomized design, the treatments are assigned to all the experimental units completely by chance. Some experiments may include a control group that receives an inactive treatment or an existing baseline treatment. Experimental Units Random Assignment Group 1 Group 2 Treatment 1 Treatment 2 Compare Results

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition14 See example on p.245 Read exam tip on p.246 CYU on p.247

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition15 Experiments: What Can Go Wrong? The response to a dummy treatment is called the placebo effect. In a double-blind experiment, neither the subjects nor those who interact with them and measure the response variable know which treatment a subject received. The response to a dummy treatment is called the placebo effect. In a double-blind experiment, neither the subjects nor those who interact with them and measure the response variable know which treatment a subject received. The logic of a randomized comparative experiment depends on our ability to treat all the subjects the same in every way except for the actual treatments being compared. Good experiments, therefore, require careful attention to details to ensure that all subjects really are treated identically.

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition16 See example on p placebo-effect/ placebo-effect/

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition17 Inference for Experiments An observed effect so large that it would rarely occur by chance is called statistically significant. A statistically significant association in data from a well-designed experiment does imply causation. An observed effect so large that it would rarely occur by chance is called statistically significant. A statistically significant association in data from a well-designed experiment does imply causation. In an experiment, researchers usually hope to see a difference in the responses so large that it is unlikely to happen just because of chance variation. We can use the laws of probability, which describe chance behavior, to learn whether the treatment effects are larger than we would expect to see if only chance were operating. If they are, we call them statistically significant.

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition18 Blocking When a population consists of groups of individuals that are “similar within but different between,” a stratified random sample gives a better estimate than a simple random sample. This same logic applies in experiments. A block is a group of experimental units that are known before the experiment to be similar in some way that is expected to affect the response to the treatments. In a randomized block design, the random assignment of experimental units to treatments is carried out separately within each block. A block is a group of experimental units that are known before the experiment to be similar in some way that is expected to affect the response to the treatments. In a randomized block design, the random assignment of experimental units to treatments is carried out separately within each block.

The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition19 Matched Pairs Design A common type of randomized block design for comparing two treatments is a matched pairs design. The idea is to create blocks by matching pairs of similar experimental units. A matched pairs design is a randomized blocked experiment in which each block consists of a matching pair of similar experimental units. Chance is used to determine which unit in each pair gets each treatment. Sometimes, a “pair” in a matched-pairs design consists of a single unit that receives both treatments. Since the order of the treatments can influence the response, chance is used to determine with treatment is applied first for each unit. A matched pairs design is a randomized blocked experiment in which each block consists of a matching pair of similar experimental units. Chance is used to determine which unit in each pair gets each treatment. Sometimes, a “pair” in a matched-pairs design consists of a single unit that receives both treatments. Since the order of the treatments can influence the response, chance is used to determine with treatment is applied first for each unit.

Section Summary In this section, we learned how to… The Practice of Statistics, 5 th Edition20 DISTINGUISH between an observational study and an experiment. EXPLAIN the concept of confounding. IDENTIFY the experimental units, explanatory and response variables, and treatments in an experiment. EXPLAIN the purpose of comparison, random assignment, control, and replication in an experiment. DESCRIBE a completely randomized design for an experiment. DESCRIBE the placebo effect and the purpose of blinding in an experiment. INTERPRET the meaning of statistically significant in the context of an experiment. EXPLAIN the purpose of blocking in an experiment. DESCRIBE a randomized block design or a matched pairs design for an experiment. Experiments