Objectives (BPS chapter 9) Producing data: experiments  Experiments  How to experiment badly  Randomized comparative experiments  The logic of randomized.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
DESIGNING EXPERIMENTS
Advertisements

Objectives (BPS chapter 9)
Part 2 CHAPTER 6.1. DESIGNING AN EXPERIMENT The first goal in designing an experiment is to ensure that it will show us the effect of the explanatory.
Chapter 6: Experiments in the Real World
Stats: Modeling the World
Chapter 9 Experiments1 Chapter 9 Producing Data: Experiments.
AP Statistics Chapter 5 Notes.
Producing data: - Design of experiments IPS chapters 3.1 and 3.2 © 2006 W.H. Freeman and Company.
Introduction to the Design of Experiments
Introduction to the Design of Experiments
Chapter 13 Experiments and Observational Studies.
Producing data: experiments BPS chapter 9 © 2006 W. H. Freeman and Company.
Chapter 13: Experiments and Observational Studies
Sullivan – Fundamentals of Statistics – 2 nd Edition – Chapter 1 Section 5 – Slide 1 of 34 Chapter 1 Section 5 The Design of Experiments.
Chapter 13 Notes Observational Studies and Experimental Design
Producing data: experiments BPS chapter 8 © 2006 W. H. Freeman and Company.
Obtaining data Available data are data that were produced in the past for some other purpose but that may help answer a present question inexpensively.
Week 2 Chapter 8. Designing Experiments. Objectives (PSLS Chapter 8) Designing experiments  Experimental terminology  Comparative, randomized experiments.
Section 5.2 Designing Experiments AP Statistics
Warm-up A newspaper article about an opinion poll says that “43% of Americans approve of the president’s overall job performance.” Toward the end of the.
Types of Experimental Designs EQ: How can I make a BETTER experiment?
1 Chapter 3: Experimental Design. 2 Effect of Wine Consumption on Heart Disease Death Rate **Each data point represents a different country.
Section 5.2 EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN. EXPERIMENTAL UNITS, SUBJECTS AND TREATMENTS Experimental Unit – The individuals on which the experiment is being conducted.
LT 4.2 Designing Experiments Thanks to James Jaszczak, American Nicaraguan School.
5.2 Designing Experiments
Designing Experiments Purpose for experiments – to study the response of one variable to the changes in other variables. Experimental Units (Subjects)
Simpson’s paradox An association or comparison that holds for all of several groups can reverse direction when the data are combined (aggregated) to form.
The Practice of Statistics, 5th Edition Starnes, Tabor, Yates, Moore Bedford Freeman Worth Publishers CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies 4.2Experiments.
The Practice of Statistics, 5th Edition Starnes, Tabor, Yates, Moore Bedford Freeman Worth Publishers CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies 4.2Experiments.
Lecture PowerPoint Slides Basic Practice of Statistics 7 th Edition.
AP STATISTICS Section 5.2 Designing Experiments. Objective: To be able to identify and use different experimental design techniques. Experimental Units:
CHAPTER 9: Producing Data: Experiments. Chapter 9 Concepts 2  Observation vs. Experiment  Subjects, Factors, Treatments  How to Experiment Badly 
C HAPTER 5: P RODUCING D ATA DESIGNING EXPERIMENTS.
Section 5.2 Designing Experiments. Observational Study - Observes individuals and measures variables of interest but DOES NOT attempt to influence the.
CHAPTER 9: Producing Data Experiments ESSENTIAL STATISTICS Second Edition David S. Moore, William I. Notz, and Michael A. Fligner Lecture Presentation.
Lecture PowerPoint Slides Basic Practice of Statistics 7 th Edition.
The Practice of Statistics, 5th Edition Starnes, Tabor, Yates, Moore Bedford Freeman Worth Publishers CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies 4.2Experiments.
Chapter 13: Experiments and Observational Studies AP Statistics.
CHAPTER 9 Producing Data: Experiments BPS - 5TH ED.CHAPTER 9 1.
OBSERVATIONAL STUDIES & EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN AP Statistics – Ch 13.
The Practice of Statistics, 5th Edition Starnes, Tabor, Yates, Moore Bedford Freeman Worth Publishers CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies 4.2Experiments.
Chapter 5.2 Designing experiments. Terminology The individuals on which the experiment is done are the experiment units. When the units are human beings.
{ Chapter 6.2 Part 2. Experimental Design Terms Terms: Response variable – measures outcome (dependent, y) Explanatory variable – attempts to explain.
The Practice of Statistics, 5th Edition Starnes, Tabor, Yates, Moore Bedford Freeman Worth Publishers CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies 4.2Experiments.
Producing Data: Experiments BPS - 5th Ed. Chapter 9 1.
CHAPTER 9: Producing Data Experiments ESSENTIAL STATISTICS Second Edition David S. Moore, William I. Notz, and Michael A. Fligner Lecture Presentation.
Chapter 6 Designing Experiments. Section 6.2 Experiments in the Real World.
Silent Do Now (5 minutes) *Before you begin, grab a new weekly sheet and take out your homework!  An opinion poll calls 2000 randomly chosen residential.
CHAPTER 9: Producing Data Experiments ESSENTIAL STATISTICS Second Edition David S. Moore, William I. Notz, and Michael A. Fligner Lecture Presentation.
Section 5.2 Designing Experiments AP Statistics October 27 th, 2014.
Chapter 5: Producing Data 5.2 – Designing Experiments.
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Observational Studies and Experiments
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Producing data: experiments
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Section 5.2 EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN.
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Obtaining data Available data are data that were produced in the past for some other purpose but that may help answer a present question inexpensively.
Designing Experiments
Chapter 5.2 Designing Experiments
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Homework: pg. 357 #34-36, ) Subjects: the students living in the selected dormitory Factor: the rate structure Treatments: paying one flat rate or.
Principles of Experimental Design
CHAPTER 4 Designing Studies
Presentation transcript:

Objectives (BPS chapter 9) Producing data: experiments  Experiments  How to experiment badly  Randomized comparative experiments  The logic of randomized comparative experiments  Cautions about experimentation  Matched pairs and other block designs

Terminology  The individuals in an experiment are the experimental units. If they are human, we call them subjects.  The explanatory variables in an experiment are often called factors.  A treatment is any specific experimental condition applied to the subjects. If an experiment has several factors, a treatment is a combination of specific values of each factor.  The factor may be the administration of a drug.  One group of people may be placed on a diet/exercise program for 6 months (treatment), and their blood pressure (response variable) would be compared with that of people who did not diet or exercise.

 If the experiment involves giving two different doses of a drug, we say that we are testing two levels of the factor.  A response to a treatment is statistically significant if it is larger than you would expect by chance (due to random variation among the subjects). We will learn how to determine this later. In a study of sickle cell anemia, 150 patients were given the drug hydroxyurea, and 150 were given a placebo (dummy pill). The researchers counted the episodes of pain in each subject. Identify: The subjects (patients, all 300) The factors/treatments (hydroxyurea and placebo) And the response variable (episodes of pain)

3 Principles of experimental design 1. Control the effects of lurking variables on the response, most simply by comparing two or more treatments. 2. Randomize—use impersonal chance to assign subjects to treatments. 3. Replicate—use enough subjects in each group to reduce chance variation in the results.

Principles of comparative experiments Experiments are comparative in nature: We compare the response to a treatment versus to:  another treatment  no treatment (a control)  a placebo  or any combination of the above A control is a situation in which no treatment is administered. It serves as a reference mark for an actual treatment (e.g., a group of subjects does not receive any drug or pill of any kind). A placebo is a fake treatment, such as a sugar pill. It is used to test the hypothesis that the response to the treatment is due to the actual treatment and not to how the subject is being taken care of.

Designing “controlled” experiments Fisher found the data from experiments that had been going on for decades to be basically worthless because of poor experimental design.  Fertilizer had been applied to a field one year and not in another in order to compare the yield of grain produced in the two years. BUT  It may have rained more, or been sunnier, in different years.  The seeds used may have differed between years as well.  Or fertilizer was applied to one field and not to a nearby field in the same year. BUT  The fields might have different soil, water, drainage, and history of previous use.  Too many factors affecting the results were “uncontrolled.” Sir Ronald Fisher—The “father of statistics” He was sent to Rothamsted Agricultural Station in the UK to evaluate the success of various fertilizer treatments.

Fisher’s solution:  In the same field and same year, apply fertilizer to randomly spaced plots within the field. Analyze plants from similarly treated plots together.  This minimizes the effect of variation of drainage and soil composition within the field on yield as well as controlling for weather. FFFFFF FFFFFFFF FFFFF FFFFFFFF FFFFF FFFF Randomized comparative experiments

The best way to exclude biases in an experiment is to randomize the design. Both the individuals and treatments are assigned randomly. A double-blind experiment is one in which neither the subjects nor the experimenter know which individuals got which treatment until the experiment is completed. Getting rid of sampling biases Another way to make sure your conclusions are robust is to replicate your experiment — do it over. Replication ensures that particular results are not due to uncontrolled factors or errors of manipulation.

Is the treatment appropriate for the response you want to study?  Is studying the effects of eating red meat on cholesterol values in a group of middle-aged men a realistic way to study factors affecting heart disease problem in humans?  What about studying the effects of hair spray on rats to determine what will happen to women with big hair? Lack of realism Random sampling is meant to gain information about the larger population from which we sample. population sample

In a completely randomized experimental design, individuals are randomly assigned to groups, then the groups are randomly assigned to treatments. Completely randomized designs

Matched pairs: Choose pairs of subjects that are closely matched — e.g., same sex, height, weight, age, and race. Within each pair, randomly assign who will receive which treatment. It is also possible to just use a single person and give the two treatments to this person over time in random order. In this case, the “matched pair” is just the same person at different points in time. The most closely matched pair studies use identical twins. Matched pairs designs

In a block design, subjects are divided into groups, or blocks, prior to the experiment to test hypotheses about differences between the groups. The blocking, or stratification, here is by gender. Block designs