Spatial Learning and Memory in the Sand Maze: Does WIN-2 Produce State Dependent Retention in Rats? Ashley R. Smith and Gretchen Hanson Gotthard Randolph-Macon.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Animal model of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD): Effects of propranolol on exploration in an elevated plus maze in rats G.H. Gotthard, J. Marini,
Advertisements

Reconsolidation Experiment Psychology 4870 Winter 2008.
Research Methods in Psychology Spring 2012, PSYC I. A brief description of the Morris water maze task used to measure spatial navigation in rodents.
Protein Synthesis Inhibition and Appetitive Memory Reconsolidation: Effects of Cycloheximide on Retrained Odor Discrimination Memory in Long-Evans Rats.
Retrograde Amnesia: Is Memory Gone For Good Or Just Hiding Out? Gretchen Hanson Gotthard, Ph.D. Randolph-Macon Woman’s College Lynchburg, VA.
DECLARATIVE MEMORY IN ANIMALS 1.Research aims of animal models a. Neuropsychological aim b.Comparative aim c.Neurobiological mechanisms 2. Limitations.
The Effects of Vicarious Learning with Experienced Rats Jessie Young and Lyndsey Wells Randolph College Lynchburg, VA Introduction Vicarious learning.
Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Blindly Biased: Restricting Cue Access Impacts Spatial Orientation in a Human Analogue of the Water Maze B.P. Apger, D.M.
Experiment 2: Context generalization following delayed context shift.
Examination of the State Dependent Properties of WIN on Spatial Learning and Memory in Rats in the Sand Maze Ashley R. Smith and Gretchen Hanson.
Unilateral lesions of the medial agranular cortex produce a persistent disruption in the organization of food protection behaviors S. J. WAGNER, M. J.
 Left Panel. The two highest nicotine doses (0.8 and 2.0 mg/kg) produced a CTA on several sessions and the test session in the WKY rats. (* = 0.8 mg/kg.
Working & Reference Memory Following Fimbra-Fornix Lesions.
Protein synthesis inhibition and extinction: Does cycloheximide produce amnesia for extinction of an odor discrimination in rats? Alexandra Knoppel, Katherine.
Selective impairment of learning and blockade of long-term potentiantion by an NMDA receptor antagonist, AP5 Presentation by: David Alexanian Ellen Almirol.
Self-Protective Memory of Interpersonal Events Margaret Wile, Angela Neal, Christine Coyne, and Edward Lemay Department of Psychology, University of New.
Protein Synthesis is Not Required for Extinction of Paired-Associate Odor Discriminations Sarah Linderman, Elizabeth Nguyen, and Gretchen Hanson Gotthard.
Blocking frustrating memories using the protein synthesis inhibitor cycloheximide and the β- adrenergic receptor antagonist propranolol in an appetitive.
From Mechanisms of Memory, second edition
Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models
Lecture 22: Animal Cognition I (Memory) Learning, Psychology 5310 Spring, 2015 Professor Delamater.
Figure 3: H5 Probe and Control Data Bar 1: Percent correct on regular probe trials. Bar 2: Percent correct on probes with blind exp. Bar 3: Percent correct.
A vanishing sex difference Effects of environmental contingencies in a virtual Morris water task on male and female spatial navigation Mühl, Griego, Kabisch,
Competitive Place Task
A Pro-Inflammatory Agent, Lipopolysaccharide, Can Mimic the Effects of Aging on Spatial Reference Memory E. R. Escobedo 1, K. Magnusson 2,3 — 1 BioResource.
Left and Right Panels. Mice treated with prazosin were less active than their vehicle-paired counterparts (left panel). Time course analysis shows paired.
Early Arbitrary Object Memory May Set the Stage for Episodic Memory in Toddlers Frances Balcomb, Nora S. Newcombe, Katrina Ferrara, Jule Grant, Sarah M.
CP-154,526, a CRF type-1 receptor antagonist, attenuates the cue-and methamphetamine-induced reinstatement of extinguished methamphetamine- seeking behavior.
Does Scheduled Intermittent Access to a Highly Palatable Carbohydrate-Rich Food Trigger Binge Eating in Laboratory Rats? Robynn Mackechnie, Kishan Govind,
Determinants of Olfactory Memory Span in Rats MacQueen, D. A., Bullard, L. A., & Galizio, M. University of North Carolina Wilmington Introduction Dudchenko,
Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research
Twenty male Sprague-Dawley rats previously used for other purposes (Mackechnie et al., 2008 and Govind et al., 2008) were used in this pilot study. Rats.
Effect of Cycloheximide on Extinction of Odor Discrimination Learning in Rats Alexandra Knoppel, Katherine Janson, Sonnett White, and Gretchen Hanson Gotthard.
Role of muscarinic M1 receptors in inhibitory avoidance and contextual fear conditioning Juliana Carlota Kramer Soares, Raquel Vecchio Fornari, Maria Gabriela.
D ISTINCT C OMPONENTS OF S PATIAL L EARNING R EVEALED BY P RIOR T RAINING AND NMDA R ECEPTOR B LOCKADE Group B3 Abdullah, Barbara, Charles, Charmaine,
Effects of age on navigation strategy 期刊: Neurobiology of Aging 作者: M. Kirk Rodgers, Joseph A. Sindone III, Scott D. Moffat 學生:林怡儒 M
Three animals (F3, F6, G13) developed high levels of accuracy and showed rapid acquisition during baseline sessions. Three animals (F3, F6, G13) have shown.
Learning like a girl? “Estrogen modulates learning in female rats by acting directly at distinct memory systems” L. Zurkovsky et al. Neuroscience (2007)
Anthony S. Rauhut 1,2 and André White 2, Department of Psychology 1 and Neuroscience Program 2, Dickinson College, Carlisle, PA References Discussion Abstract.
The Use of Chaining and Reinforcement in an Obstacle Course Vivian Scott & Elishia Webster Randolph-Macon Woman’s College Lynchburg, VA Introduction.
The Effects of Different Types of Food Reinforcers on Performance in the T-Maze Sarah A. Linderman, Fahima Vakalia, and Laura Revetta Randolph-Macon Woman’s.
Genetic enhancement of learning and memory in mice. Tang YP et al.
“Enhancement of extinction memory consolidation: The role of the noradrenergic and GABAergic systems within the basolateral amygdala” Neurobiology of Learning.
Margaret Della Vecchia 2 and Anthony S. Rauhut 1,2, Department of Psychology 1 and Neuroscience Program 2, Dickinson College, Carlisle, PA References Discussion.
Memory Mechanisms One. Spatial Memory The ability to remember the position or location of objects and places. – important in foraging for food in the.
 Endocannabinoids: Learning, & Memory Clarissa Staton Endocannabinoids Seminar 2015.
Timing of SCH23390 Administration Influences Extinction of Conditioned Hyperactivity in Mice Anthony S. Rauhut 1,2, Kristen Ratner2, Sandy Buck2, and Ee-Rah.
Jiang Wu, Bihua Bie, Hui Yang, Jijun J. Xu, David L
J. Ruiz-Medina, I. Morgado-Bernal, D. Redolar-Ripoll, L
BEHAVIOR AND MEMORY EVALUATION OF RATS TREATED WITH SIMVASTATIN IN THE SCOPOLAMINE ANIMAL MODEL TONIN, F. S.; BARBIERO, J. K. Departamento de Farmacologia,
Justin Yates & Michael Bardo
Supplementary Figure 1. Deficits in cognitive function persist up to 11 wks post-injury in moderate-level CCI TBI mice. Sham-injured and CCI TBI mice.
Examination of the State Dependent Properties of WIN on Spatial Learning and Memory in Rats in the Sand Maze   Ashley R. Smith and Gretchen Hanson.
Introduction Results Discussion Method References
Protein synthesis inhibition and extinction:
Vicarious Learning in Laboratory Rats
Randolph-Macon Woman’s College
Volume 21, Issue 2, Pages (August 1998)
Volume 60, Issue 2, Pages (October 2008)
Hill-Aina Steffenach, Menno Witter, May-Britt Moser, Edvard I. Moser 
Acetylcholine-secreting cells improve age-induced memory deficits
Volume 11, Issue 2, Pages (April 2015)
Results from the Morris water task.
Volume 17, Issue 1, Pages (February 2010)
Do Rats Have the Ability to Discriminate Between Words?
Volume 50, Issue 3, Pages (May 2006)
Volume 42, Issue 1, Pages (April 2004)
Single reward vs. two different rewards:
Hippocampus? OR....
Fig. 3. Effects of RP on SPS-CF stress-induced memory impairment of spatial memory. (A) Latency to find hidden platform in Morris water maze during 5 daily.
Presentation transcript:

Spatial Learning and Memory in the Sand Maze: Does WIN-2 Produce State Dependent Retention in Rats? Ashley R. Smith and Gretchen Hanson Gotthard Randolph-Macon Woman’s College Lynchburg, VA Introduction Naturally occurring cannabinoids (e.g., THC, as found in marijuana) and synthetic cannabinoids (e.g., WIN , used in the present study), have been linked with hippocampal function, and therefore, spatial learning and memory. Although a number of studies have shown a link between cannabinoid administration and learning and memory deficits (e.g., Lichtman, et al., 1995), no studies have explicitly examined the state dependent properties of cannabinoids. State dependent retention refers to the tendency of organisms to recall information better when in the same “state” they were in during learning. In fact, if cannabinoid administration produces state dependent effects, then a memory impairment following cannabinoid administration may not be due to decreased hippocampal function alone, but may be due to a switched state between acquisition and testing. The sand maze is an appetitive open-field spatial task that may serve as an alternative to the water maze in some studies. While the water maze requires rats to swim in a pool of water to locate a hidden platform (Morris, 1981), the sand maze requires rats to dig in a pool of sand to retrieve buried cereal rewards (Hanson, 2003). The sand maze may be a better alternative for studies that aim to examine spatial behavior without the potential side effects of aversive tasks (e.g., increased amygdala activity and/or fight or flight responses). The synthetic cannabinoid, WIN (WIN-2), was examined in the present study using the sand maze. It was hypothesized that WIN-2 would produce anterograde amnesia. Additionally, it was predicted that the anterograde amnesic effect would be diminished with re-administration of the drug at testing (i.e., state dependent retention). Method Subjects The subjects were 90-day old, male Long-Evans rats (N=10). Rats were reduced to and maintained at 85% of their free-feeding weights one week prior to and during the experiment. Water was available ad libitum. Apparatus The sand maze was a plastic pool (36 inches wide by 6 inches deep) filled with a sand/crushed Froot Loops (FL) cereal mixture that was 2 inches deep (approximately 11 ounces of FL was crushed and mixed with 100 pounds of play sand to make the mixture). The maze was elevated 36 inches off the floor. Procedure Each rat was placed in the sand maze and required to find Froot Loops cereal (FL), which was located in one consistent location during shaping and training (i.e., NW, NE, SW, or SE quadrant). Rats were started from a different location in the maze on each trial (i.e., N, S, E, or W). After finding the reward on any given trial, the rat was allowed to consume approximately three to four FL prior to termination of the trial. Rats were handled prior to shaping. Rats were shaped, trained, and tested on Location 1 followed by shaping, training and testing on Location 2 (see Figure 1). Shaping and training on Location 1 was distributed (i.e., one trial per day for six days), while shaping and training on Location 2 was massed (i.e., three trials per day for two days). All rats received intraperitoneal (I.P.) injections of WIN-2 (1.5 mg/kg) 30 minutes prior to shaping and training on Location 2. Rats were then randomly assigned to either the WIN-2 test group (n=5) or the Vehicle (VEH) test group (n=5), which were used to test the state dependent effects of WIN-2 on the second probe trial. Initial maze behavior, latency, and quadrant preference were measured. Discussion Latency to find the FL increased significantly across reversal training trials. All rats learned to find the correct locations and showed preferences for those locations. WIN-2 did not produce anterograde amnesia and no state dependent effects were exhibited. There were no differences between WIN-2 and Vehicle groups. Future Research: increase WIN-2 dosage to produce more salient drug state. References Hanson, G.R. (2003). The sand maze: An appetitive alternative to the Morris water maze (Doctoral dissertation, Kent State University, 2003). Dissertation Abstracts International, 63, Lichtman, A. H., Dimen, K. R., & Martin, B. R. (1995). Systemic or intrahippocampal cannabinoid administration impairs spatial memory in rats. Psychopharmacology,119(3), Morris, R.G.M. (1981). Spatial localization does not require the presence of local cues. Learning and Motivation, 12, ˚ Day 1: Exposed Day 2: Partially Buried Day 3: Shallow Buried Day 4: Buried (Shallow) Day 5: Buried (Medium) Day 6: Buried (Deep) Day 7: Probe Trial Trial 1: Exposed Trial 2: Partially Buried Trial 3: Shallow Buried Trial 1: Buried (Shallow) Trial 2: Buried (Medium) Trial 3: Buried (Deep) Shaping: Location 1 Day 1-3 Training: Location 1 Days 4-6 Testing: Location 1 Shaping: Location 2 Day 8 Training: Location 2 Day 9 Testing: Location 2 Day 10: Probe Trial Results Rats took longer to find the FL on later training trials during reversal learning. Additionally, WIN-2 did not produce anterograde amnesia and no state dependent effects were exhibited (i.e., both WIN-2 and VEH rats showed above chance preferences for the correct quadrants during testing). Latency Latency to find the FL increased significantly across reversal learning trials for both groups, t(9)=-5.57, p 0.05; Day 8, Trial 2: t(8)=-0.25, p>0.05; Day 8, Trial 3: t(8)=-0.50, p>0.05; Day 9, Trial 1: t(8)=-0.73, p>0.05; Day 9, Trial 2: t(8)=-0.80, p>0.05; Day 9, Trial 3: t(8)=-0.28, p>0.05]. Quadrant Preference As can be seen in Figure 3, all rats learned to find the correct locations and showed a preference for those locations both on Probe Trial 1 (WIN-2: t(4)=4.62, p 0.05; VEH: t(4)=0.99, p>0.05). However, there were no significant differences between WIN-2 and VEH groups on quadrant preference (Location 1: t(8)=0.44, p>0.05; Location 2: t(8)=-0.103, p>0.05; Location 1 after reversal training: t(8)=0.940, p>0.05).