English-speaking children who are typically developing first acquire item-specific patterns (e.g. put it in) and their meanings as a whole, then develop.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The filler-gap hypothesis and the acquisition of German relative clauses Holger Diessel University of Jena
Advertisements

Developmental Sequences in Second Language Learning Presenters: Jacqueline dos Anjos, Hanna Heseker, Dana Meyer.
Contrastive Analysis, Error Analysis, Interlanguage
18 and 24-month-olds use syntactic knowledge of functional categories for determining meaning and reference Yarden Kedar Marianella Casasola Barbara Lust.
Passive comprehension by Slovak typically and atypically developing children Master thesis in psychology, Author: Radka Antalíková, Supervisor: Kristine.
Measuring Referring Expressions in a Story Context Phyllis Schneider, Speech Pathology & Audiology, University of Alberta Denyse Hayward, University of.
Theeraporn Ratitamkul, University of Illinois and Adele E. Goldberg, Princeton University Introduction How do young children learn verb meanings? Scene.
Language, Mind, and Brain by Ewa Dabrowska Chapter 9: Syntactic constructions, pt. 1.
Constructions at Work The Nature of Generalization in Language
Acknowledgments This study was funded in part by a grant from the National Institutes of Health-Institute on Deafness and other Communicative Disorders.
Nonword Repetition and Sentence Repetition as Clinical Markers of SLI: The Case of Cantonese Stokes, F. S., Wong, M.Y.A., Fletcher, P., & Leonard, B. L.
Nature of the Input Dana Hughes. What is the Nature of Input in Regards to Language Acquisition?
Discussion Future Research Although there was no statistical significance between prelinguistic and linguistic levels of communicative acts, canonical.
L2 Acquisition: The Social Perspective Guadalupe Valdés Stanford University.
Extracting an Inventory of English Verb Constructions from Language Corpora Matthew Brook O’Donnell Nick C. Ellis Presentation.
LOT 5: jan06 1 Language Acquisition 5. Elena Lieven, MPI-EVA, Leipzig School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester.
Explaining first language acquisition
Topics in Cognition and Language: Theory, Data and Models *Perceptual scene analysis: extraction of meaning events, causality, intentionality, Theory of.
From linear sequences to abstract structures: Distributional information in infant-direct speech Hao Wang & Toby Mintz Department of Psychology University.
1 The syntactic abilities of children with SLI: The Passive.
Language, Mind, and Brain by Ewa Dabrowska Chapter 10: The cognitive enterprise.
Non-Word Repetition Challenges to Language Acquisition: Bilingualism and Language Impairment Dr. Sharon Armon-Lotem Bar Ilan University.
Casenhiser and Goldberg (2005) Ability to learn to pair novel constructional meaning with novel form Known nouns and nonsense verb arranged in non- English.
Affectionate Touching and Intentional Communication in 9, 12, and 18 Month-Old Deaf Infants with Deaf and Hearing Mothers By: Sherri Ewing Introduction.
Input-Output Relations in Syntactic Development Reflected in Large Corpora Anat Ninio The Hebrew University, Jerusalem The 2009 Biennial Meeting of SRCD,
Young Children Learn a Native English Anat Ninio The Hebrew University, Jerusalem 2010 Conference of Human Development, Fordham University, New York Background:
Time to Play? Playtime and Language Development
The Effects of Enhanced Milieu Teaching on Children with Down Syndrome
Complementary roles of AAC and speech. AAC and Speech AAC is useful for anyone who is unable to use speech to meet all communication needs – Across all.
CSD 5400 REHABILITATION PROCEDURES FOR THE HARD OF HEARING Language and Speech of Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Characteristics and Concerns Language Acquisition.
Maternal Scaffolding of Play and Thinking in the Infant/Toddler Period Relationships to Later IQ.
Learning the passive in natural(istic) settings Katie Alcock, Ken Rimba, Manizha Tellaie, and Charles Newton Thanks to Kamil ud Deen.
“Language Intervention with Young Children” March 28, 2000 Bonnie W. Johnson, PhD, CCC-SLP University of Illinois Postdoctoral Fellow Special Education.
X Language Acquisition
A single case series of narrative interaction between children who use speech generating devices and their educational staff Pippa Bailey*, Karen Bunning,
Infant Directed Speech with infants at risk of Autism Spectrum Disorder Jean Quigley School of Psychology Trinity College Dublin Funded by IRCHSS.
Single Subject Research (Richards et al.) Chapter 8.
The 12 th International Cognitive Linguistics Conference (June 25, 2013) Mothers’ Speech for Children’s Intention-reading: a Cross-linguistic Study of.
Assessment of Morphology & Syntax Expression. Objectives What is MLU Stages of Syntactic Development Examples of Difficulties in Syntax Why preferring.
Parental Educational Level, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences Vanderbilt.
Chapter 10 - Language 4 Components of Language 1.Phonology Understanding & producing speech sounds Phoneme - smallest sound unit Number of phonemes varies.
Adele E. Goldberg. How argument structure constructions are learned.
Objective The current study examined whether the timing of recovery from late onset of productive vocabulary (e.g., either earlier or later blooming) was.
Specific Language Impairment & Cognition: A Meta-Analysis Michael W. Casby Communicative Sciences & Disorders Michigan State University imail:
INTRODUCTION : DESCRIBING AND EXPLAINING L2 ACQUISITION Ellis 2003, Chapter 1 PP By. Annisa Rizqi Handayani.
Parent Education, Language Characteristics, and Children Who Are Late to Talk Celeste Domsch, Ph.D. Baylor University Stephen Camarata, Ph.D. Edward G.
THE RESPONSES OF CHILDREN WITH DOWN SYNDROME TO COMMUNICATION BREAKDOWNS Alyssa Armster-Wik Alyssa Armster-Wik Department of Speech Communication Arts.
CHAPTER 1 Description and Explaining L2 Acquisition Source: Rod Ellis 2003 Second Language Acquisition Name: Sekar Katon Wijayanti NIM :
A Longitudinal Study of Complex Syntax Production in Children with SLI There are relatively few studies of complex syntax (CS) in children with SLI (Schuele.
Language Intervention and the Effect on Passive Comprehension – A pilot-study Lone Sundahl Olsen, ph.D. Student, University of Aalborg, Denmark Master.
J UMPING AROUND AND LEAVING THINGS OUT : A PROFILE OF THE NARRATIVES ABILITIES OF CHILDREN WITH SPECIFIC LANGUAGE IMPAIRMENT M IRANDA, A., M C C ABE, A.,
Teaching the Control of Variables Strategy in Fourth Grade Classrooms Robert F. Lorch, Jr., William J. Calderhead, Emily E. Dunlap, Emily C. Hodell, Benjamin.
Reinforcement Look at matched picture after sound ends & it moves 10 trials (5 of each pairing) 2 or 4 blocks (2 pairs of words, 2 pairs of swoops) Participants.
Method. Input to Learning Two groups of learners each learn one of two new Semi-Artificial Languages. Both Languages: Example sentences: glim lion bee.
Introduction : describing and explaining L2 acquisition Ellis, R Second Language Acquisition (3 – 14)
Child Directed Speech. What is CDS? A specialised way of speaking to young children/a way of direct teaching A specialised way of speaking to young children/a.
Introduction: describing and explaining L2 acquisition By : Annisa Mustikanthi.
1 The acquisition of the morphosyntax and pragmatics of reference Evidence from the use of indefinite/definite determiners and pronouns in English Margot.
Chapter 10 Language acquisition Language acquisition----refers to the child’s acquisition of his mother tongue, i.e. how the child comes to understand.
CLS July EYE GAZE IN TURNTAKING IN SIGN LANGUAGE INTERACTION Anne Baker & Beppie van den Bogaerde.
Faculty Adviser: Dr. Deborah Elledge  Student Researchers: Leah Carpenter – Jacqueline Oakes – Jillian Utz Communication Sciences and Disorders Department.
Gaze cues in mother-child dyads Heather Bell and Meredith Meyer University of Oregon INTRODUCTION RESULTS CONCLUSIONS METHODS REFERENCES ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS.
Use of Concordancers A corpus (plural corpora) – a large collection of texts, written or spoken, stored on a computer. A concordancer – a computer programme.
Effects of Reading on Word Learning
PSYC 206 Lifespan Development Bilge Yagmurlu.
State University of New York at Geneseo
Language, Mind, and Brain by Ewa Dabrowska
Consonant variegations in first words: Infants’ actual productions of
Saidna Zulfiqar bin Tahir STATE UNIVERSITY OF MAKASSAR
References (2016) (New brain and neuroscience research)
Presentation transcript:

English-speaking children who are typically developing first acquire item-specific patterns (e.g. put it in) and their meanings as a whole, then develop semi-fixed patterns with open slots (e.g., X it in), which they may eventually analyze into more abstract grammatical constructions ((Subj), V, Obj, Oblique path/location ). According to usage-based theorists (e.g., Clark and Kelly, 2006; Cameron-Faulkner et al, 2003; Tomasello, 2003; Lieven, 2006; Peters, in press), both the content of constructions in the input and their frequencies are critical to acquisition. Goldberg and Casenhiser (2006) presented evidence that skewed input, which initially exposes children to less diversity within particular constructions, may facilitate learning of those constructions. Much less is known about the information available in the input to children with language impairments (but see Riches, Faragher & Conti- Ramsden, 2006). The research question addressed in the current study is whether children with SLI experience a more diverse linguistic input than typically developing children. This diversity could produce difficulty in learning. Specifically, the current study examined two linguistic constructions, intransitive motion e.g. Mary goes to the store, and caused motion, e.g. Mary put the baby in the crib, in the child-directed speech of mothers to their children with SLI and their younger MLU-matched siblings. What is a construction? A construction is a form-meaning pairing that expresses the basic propositions of a language. It is any linguistic pattern that is not fully predictable from its component parts (Goldberg, 2006). Constructions themselves carry meaning independent of the lexical items they contain. For example, the transitive-motion construction, He kicked the ball into the wall, can be interpreted to mean that He caused the ball to move into the wall. However, there is nothing in the verb kick that requires that its object be caused to move, as shown in the pattern He kicked the wall. Conclusions Differences in the Language Input of Children with Specific Language Impairments Compared to their MLU-Matched Siblings Roberta Corrigan, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee References Transitive and Intransitive Motion Constructions Transitive (Caused) Motion Construction = (Subj), V, Obj, Oblique path/location X causes Y to move Z path/location Example: We’ve got to put it on here. Intransitive Motion Construction = (Subj), V, Oblique path/location X moves to Y Example: This goes there. Methods Participants There were 9 mothers in the data set, taken from the CHILDES (MacWhinney, 2000) clinical corpora, Conti-Ramsden 1 and 2 (Conti- Ramsden & Dykins, 1991; Conti-Ramsden, Hutcheson, & Grove, 1995; Conti-Ramsden & Jones, 1997). Mother child interactions had been videotaped during free play. Child-directed speech (CDS) of the mothers to their children with SLI (mean age = 6;3, mean MLU = 1.93) was compared to CDS to the younger MLU matched siblings (mean age= 2;8 mean MLU = 1.81 ). Data Reduction All sessions from the cross-sectional data set were included. The first session in the longitudinal data set where the MLU was approximately 2.00 for the child with SLI and the closest sibling session in which the MLU was within.5 MLU of the target child were included. Data for the three children who had two sessions in which their MLUs were were combined in order to increase the size of the total sample of data. Maternal single-word utterances were excluded, as were fragments that did not contain verbs. Utterances that were complex sentences containing more than one main verb were split into clauses. All maternal utterances were coded by the author as to whether they were transitive or intransitive motion constructions, other transitive or intransitive constructions, or copula constructions. A second coder examined a sample of the utterances. The average reliability between the two coders was.95. Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2003). A construction based analysis of child directed speech. Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 27(6), Clark, E. and Kelly, S. (2006) Constructions and acquisition. In E. Clark and S. Kelly (eds). Constructions and acquisition. Stanford: CSLI. Conti-Ramsden, G., & Dykins, J. (1991). Mother–child interactions with language-impaired children and their siblings. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 26, 337–354. Conti-Ramsden, G., Hutcheson, G. D., & Grove, J. (1995). Contingency and breakdown: Specific Language Impaired children’s conversations with their mothers and fathers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, Conti-Ramsden, G., & Jones, M. (1997). Verb use in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 40, Goldberg, A. (2006) Constructions at work. The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford U. Press. Goldberg, A. & Casenhiser, D. (2006) Learning argument structure constructions. In E. Clark and B. Kelly (eds.) Constructions in Acquisition. Stanford, CSLI. Lieven, L. (2006) Producing multiword utterances. In E. Clark and B. Kelly (eds.) Constructions in Acquisition. Stanford, CSLI. MacWhinney, B. (2000) The Childes project. 3 rd edition. NJ: Erlbaum. Peters, A. (in press) Connecting the dots to unpack the language. In R.Corrigan, E. Moravcsik, H. Ouali, and K. Wheatley (eds). Formulaic language, vol. 2, Acquisition, loss, psychological reality, and functional explanations. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Riches, N., Faragher, B., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2006). Verb schema use and input dependence in 5-year-old children with specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41(2), Tomasello, M. (2003) Constructing a language. A usage based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard U. Press. Acknowledgements Thanks to Suraiya Khandakar for her assistance coding the data and to John Surber for his encouragement on all phases of the study. Comparison of Transitive and Intransitive Motion Constructions Mothers produced fewer intransitive motion constructions than expected to Sibs and more than expected to the group with SLI. Mothers produced more transitive motion constructions than expected to Sibs and fewer than expected to the group with SLI. Χ 2 (1) = 8.61, p =.003. Type-Token Ratios for Intransitive Motion Constructions Mothers’ type-token ratios for intransitive motion constructions were not significantly different between the two groups (means=.38 SLI,.29 SIB). Neither the number of types of intransitive verbs used in the constructions (means = 6.11 SLI, 6.67 SIB) nor the number of tokens (mean=19.33 SLI, SIB) differed between groups. Type-Token Ratios for Transitive Motion Constructions In contrast, mothers’ type token ratios for the transitive motion constructions did differ, t (16) = 2.83, p =.01, with the SLI group hearing significantly more diverse speech (means=.52 SLI,.29 SIB). The number of types was approximately equal across the two groups (means = 5.78 SLI, 7.22 SIB), but the siblings heard almost twice as many tokens as the SLI group (Means = SLI, SIB). The results raise intriguing questions about the effect of the input on children with SLI. Children with SLI in this sample heard fewer tokens of the transitive motion construction while hearing the same number of types as their younger, typically developing siblings. This had the effect of producing more diversity in the input, which could conceivably hamper language acquisition. The effect was construction specific, occurring with the transitive motion construction, but not the intransitive motion construction. Introduction Results