Cognitive models of spelling and writing Types of dysgraphia

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
PS2011 & PS2016 Cognitive Psychology. Dr John Beech
Advertisements

Associate Professor Lyndsey Nickels
PS2011 & PS2016 Cognitive Psychology – Dr John Beech The Acquired Dyslexias.
Levels of breakdown in impaired word retrieval
ADULT LANGUAGE EVIDENCE BASED PRACTICE GROUP 2008 Extravaganza ADULT LANGUAGE EVIDENCE BASED PRACTICE GROUP Anika Roseby and Kate Schuj Group Co- Leaders.
What phonological deficit?
Perception and Dyslexia Mr Patrick Mulcahy, Chair ASASA
Dyslexia Parent Meeting
Strategies for spelling? What strategies do you use to spell words? Do these strategies work - sometimes -all the time -never? What do you think you can.
Sound – Print Connection. Learning to read entails… Normally developed language skills Normally developed language skills Knowledge of phonological structures.
Phonological dyslexia Introduction Phonological dyslexia was first described by Beauvois and Derouesne (1979); other early case are in Shallice and Warrington.
Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg. Beginning readers in the USA Looked at predictors of reading success or failure Pre-readers aged 3-5 yrs Looked at variety.
Visual Word Recognition II Language Use and Understanding Class 4.
Language and Cognition Colombo, June 2011 Day 8 Aphasia: disorders of comprehension.
Cognitive Psychology, 2 nd Ed. Chapter 12 Language Comprehension.
Language and Cognition Colombo 2011 Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia – Word comprehension With acknowledgement to Jane Marshall.
Chapter 13 Language
Chapter 16: In the Steps of the ‘Diagram Makers’ Amy Menendez.
Human Communication.
Language Ref: Banich pp Classical Model of Language 1.Predicted Patterns Never "Absolute“ 2.Symptoms can dissociate BUT:
Cognitive Neuroscience of Language 1. Premise 1: Constituent Cognitive Processes Phonological analysis Syntactic analysis Semantic analysis Premise 2:
Language Disorders October 12, Types of Disorders Aphasia: acquired disorder of language due to brain damage Dysarthria: disorder of motor apparatus.
Communication after a Stroke
Themes in production Producing speech Reading and writing.
Cognitive Psychology, 2 nd Ed. Chapter 11 Language Production.
Models of Language Language and Cognition Colombo 2011.
Sample Written Expression Lesson for Dusty Rhodes
Reading. Reading Research Processes involved in reading –Orthography (the spelling of words) –Phonology (the sound of words) –Word meaning –Syntax –Higher-level.
Aphasias: Language Disturbances Associated with Brain Injury The Classic View: based on symptoms and associated with particular brain areas The Major Syndromes:
Anne Castles and Genevieve McArthur UNDERSTANDING DYSLEXIA.
Reading from a neurolinguistic perspective Deep dyslexia Several models have been proposed to explain deep dyslexia reading performance. Now follows a.
Language Comprehension reading
Language and Cognition Colombo 2011 Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia - Writing With acknowledgement to Jane Marshall.
Despite adjustments to the Wernicke-Lichtheim model, there remained disorders which could not be explained. Later models (e.g., Heilman’s) have included.
1 Language disorders We can learn a lot by looking at system failure –Which parts are connected to which Examine the relation between listening/speaking.
Speech and Language Test Language.
Eight Constructs The 90 Minute Dysgraphia Evaluation
Writing Disabilities Sousa Chapter 6. Spoken & Written Language Researchers used to think that the left brain housed both processing centers Now know.
+ Treatment of Aphasia Week 12 April 1 st, Review Involvement of semantic and phonological stages in naming. Differentiating features of naming.
Last Lecture Dichotic Listening Dichotic Listening The corpus callosum & resource allocation The corpus callosum & resource allocation Handedness Handedness.
Reading Disabilities. Terminolgy Congenital word blindness Dyslexia Developmental dyslexia Specific reading disability Reading disability Disability-disorder,
Reading disorders in mental retardation. Dyslexia or not ? Annick COMBLAIN, University of Liege – FAPSE Department of Cognitive Sciences Speech and language.
PSYC4080D Learning Disabilities 1. PSYC4080D Learning Disabilities 2 A word on definitions… A “learning disability” (LD) is an administrative term Each.
Bookheimer 2003 Annual Rev. Neurosci.. Phonology in IFG Gelfand and Bookheimer, Neuron 2002.
Age of acquisition and frequency of occurrence: Implications for experience based models of word processing and sentence parsing Marc Brysbaert.
Aphasia and Intelligence Pierre Marie They hear and comprehend all that one says to them; they all have their intelligence; they emit vocal sounds with.
CSC321: Neural Networks Lecture 19: Simulating Brain Damage Geoffrey Hinton.
Computation in the Brain Neuropsychological Inference with an Interactive Brain: A Critique of the “Locality” Assumption Sang Joon Park.
DYSGRAPHIA. What is dysgraphia It is a learning disability resulting from the difficulty in expressing thoughts in writing. People who have this difficulty.
Mental Organs. Phrenology was an important part of popular culture in Victorian England and in Europe during the 19th century.
Semantic Processing and Irregularly Inflected Forms Michele Miozzo & Peter Gordon Columbia University Introduction Recent models of lexical representation.
Language and Cognition Colombo, June 2011
Informational Guide Prema Long SED 527 SED 527.  Specific learning disabilities (SLD) is a term that refers to a wide variety of learning problems. SLD.
Decoding Dyslexia Parent Support Group October,
Comparing the effectiveness of orthographic and phonological cues in the treatment of anomia. Lyndsey Nickels 1, Antje Lorenz 1,2, 1 Macquarie Centre for.
The Cross-Script Length Effect: Evidence for Serial Processing in Reading Aloud Kathleen Rastle (Royal Holloway University of London), Linda Bayliss (Royal.
Neurologically Based Communicative Disorders. Disorders Aphasia Apraxia Dysarthria.
1 Applying Principles To Reading Presented By Anne Davidson Michelle Diamond.
Cerebral Cortex 2.
Do teachers know what the essential literacy skills are? Do teachers know what the essential literacy skills are? Presenters: Ansie Lessing & Marike de.
VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION. What is Word Recognition? Features, letters & word interactions Interactive Activation Model Lexical and Sublexical Approach.
GTN 301 Community Nutrition & Dietetics Services Practicum Nurul Hidayah Binti Mohd Junaidi Nutrition 3.
Assessment. Issues related to Phonemic awareness assessment  Is it a conceptual understanding about language or is it a skill?
Dyslexia & reading disorders
Chapter 8 Reading and Writing
Child Psychopathology
Linguistics: Neurolinguistics
Dysgraphia.
2008 Extravaganza ADULT LANGUAGE EVIDENCE BASED PRACTICE GROUP
Learning Disabilities in the Classroom
Presentation transcript:

Cognitive models of spelling and writing Types of dysgraphia What is dysgraphia? Cognitive models of spelling and writing Types of dysgraphia Central dysgraphias Peripheral dysgraphias Nonlexical spelling? Summary

What is dysgraphia? A broad division: Central and peripheral acquired dysgraphias (or agraphias) refer to disorders of spelling and/or writing. Dysgraphias that affect spelling are central and those that affect writing are peripheral. Will consider acquired disorders following brain damage (not developmental difficulties).

Cognitive model of writing and spelling Three routes to writing from speech input: 1) Lexical-semantic route. 2) Lexical-nonsemantic route. 3) Nonlexical route. Inner speech? Need to posit separate spoken and written output lexicons in the model because of the many cases of patients who have anomia but not agraphia (and agraphia without anomia). See Ellis & Young, Chapter 7.

The lexical semantic route A word can be recognised as familiar via the auditory input lexicon (PWD patients). The meaning of the word is accessed from the auditory lexicon via the semantic system. The orthographic form of a written word is retrieved via the graphemic output lexicon. The letter representations of a word are assembled via the graphemic output buffer. This route can also be called “spelling via meaning" as only applicable to familiar words.

The lexical nonsemantic route A word is recognised as familiar by activation of representations in the auditory input lexicon. The spoken form of the word is accessed directly via the phonological output lexicon. The written form is retrieved via the graphemic output lexicon using the spoken word form. The letter representations of a word are assembled via the graphemic output buffer. Again, this is only applicable to familiar words.

The nonlexical (or sublexical) route This is assumed to be activated when we spell unfamiliar words such as invented nonwords. It is a metaphor for the application of knowledge about sound-spelling correspondences in order to assemble an oral or a written spelling. Will misspell words that have irregular spellings: e.g.,CASTLE -> "cassel” TABLE -> "taybul” these are called ‘regularisation errors’.

Peripheral dysgraphias Types of dysgraphia Central dysgraphias Phonological dysgraphia Surface dysgraphia Nonsemantic spelling Deep dysgraphia Peripheral dysgraphias Grapheme level impairment Allograph level impairment Motor pattern impairment

Phonological dysgraphia Shallice (1981) reported patient PR. PR’s ability to spell low frequency familiar words was at an above-average level - for example he could spell the word "coniferous". Very poor at spelling nonwords. 2/10 four letter nonwords and 0/10 six-letter words. Errors to nonwords tended to be similar to real words 'lexicalisations': "na" -> gn (from "gnat") "sult" -> ault (from "assault") Had trouble generating letter sounds (/b/ for B).

Loss of nonlexical route Spelling in the absence of phonological mediation (i.e., poor phoneme awareness). Vocabulary or lexically based spelling. Spelling can thus proceed via an autonomous module called the graphemic output lexicon and does not require phonological mediation. Other patients (Baxter & Warrington, 1986; Nolan and Caramazza, 1982; Roeltgen et al., 1986).

Beauvois and Derousne (1981) patient RG. Surface dysgraphia Beauvois and Derousne (1981) patient RG. Spelling unambiguous words GOOD (90%). Spelling irregular words POOR (40%). Spelling nonwords GOOD (100%). RG showed a regularity effect on spelling. e.g., “yacht” --> yot. Could not spell homophones correctly either e.g., “steak” --> stake.

Loss of lexical routes Spelling by sound. There is a modular phoneme to grapheme system for spelling words (nonwords OK). Over-reliance on nonlexical route due to impairment of lexical routes. Difficulties with exception words ("island") and ambiguous words ("team") because they require semantic knowledge (Graham, 2000). Other patients (Hatfield & Patterson, 1983; Parkin, 1993b; Weekes and Coltheart, 1996).

Nonsemantic spelling? Bramwell’s case of word meaning deafness. This patient could spell to dictation words she could not understand from spoken word input. These words included irregularly spelled words such as the word ‘Edinburgh’. Patterson (1986) reported a similar patient GK. Some patients with dementia show the same pattern i.e. an absence of knowledge about the meaning of a word from any modality but have intact spelling of irregular words (Graham, 2000).

Loss of semantic and nonlexical routes Patterson (1986) proposed the direct route from the auditory input lexicon to the graphemic output lexicon (via the speech output lexicon) thus bypassing the semantic system. This could account for cases of people who can spell irregular words without comprehension. Recent evidence suggests that the semantic system supports the spelling of irregular words - the semantic glue hypothesis (Graham, 2000).

Bub & Kertesz (1982) reported patient JC. Deep dysgraphia Bub & Kertesz (1982) reported patient JC. Made semantic errors when spelling words. e.g., “time” -> clock e.g., “sky” -> sun JC had better spelling of concrete than abstract nouns and this is called an imageability effect. JC also displayed impaired nonword spelling. 5/20 four letter nonwords, 0/17 eight letter nonwords.

Loss of nonlexical and nonsemantic routes Suggests that the route from semantics to the graphemic output lexicon was inoperative. JC did not make semantic errors when reading aloud so this is evidence in favor of separate reading and spelling systems in the brain. JC recovered completely (after 6mths) thus ruling out a developmental explanation of the dysgraphia and also showing the plasticity of spelling skills.

One orthographic lexicon or two? This dissociation between reading and writing underpins contemporary models of written language comprehension and production. Unitary model of writing/reading (Behrmann & Bub, 1992; Allport & Funnell, 1988). Dual lexicon model of writing/reading (Ellis & Young, 1996; Weekes and Coltheart, 1996). Weekes (1996) reported patient NW who was surface dysgraphic and surface dyslexic. Rehabilitation of spelling skills did not affect his reading performance and vice versa.

Grapheme level impairment Miceli, et al (1985) reported patient FV. No aphasia but frequent spelling errors such as additions, deletions, transpositions and letter substitutions in writing to dictation (e.g b for p). Affected oral spelling aloud and handwriting. Affected both word and nonword spelling. Copying was intact. Normal letter formation (intact motor skills). Problem at the grapheme level where abstract letter identity forms are stored and ordered.

Allograph level impairment Goodman & Caramazza (1986). MW. Letter substitutions in handwriting but not spelling aloud (so graphemic level is OK) “starve” -> starze “pierce” -> tierce Letters were well formed - (graphic motor OK) The assignment of shape of written letter form from a graphemic unit is impaired - this is called the allograph level of representation.

Other patients Patient JEC (Weekes, 1994). He made frequent case errors (e -> E). Wrote upper case letters well but some lower case letters were written poorly (e.g. b p q d). The spatial similarity of the letters was the best predictor of substitutions, omissions etc. JEC also displayed some surface dysgraphia leading some to argue that peripheral writing errors result from loss of semantic memory (Patterson, Hodges, 1997; Graham, 2000).

Types of error letter substitution errors “apple” --> aBBel letter omission errors “swing” --> swin letter addition errors “across -> acccross case substitution errors Queen --> quEEn Letter substitution errors were predicted by the spatial similarity of the lower case target to other lower case letters e.g., b, p, d, & q. crab --> craB; ladder --> laDDer; apple --> aBBel

Graphic motor pattern impairments Selecting letters for written output: Baxter & Warrington (1986): patient I.D.T. Problem in retrieving graphic motor patterns. Could spell aloud (so grapheme level OK). Could copy words and letters well and describe the shape of letters (so allograph level OK). Couldn't write at all - not even three letter words.

Other patients Executing (spatial dysgraphia): Ellis, Young & Flude (1987): patient VB. Know the sequence of movements but problem in executing this sequence. Patient tends to write down one side of page. Patient omits or duplicates letters and strokes. Problem attending to visual and kinaesthetic feedback during writing?

Summary Studies of dysgraphic patients reveal that spelling and writing depend on multiple routes. and the functional architecture of the normal spelling/writing system is highly modular. The evidence from surface dyslexic and dysgraphic patients suggests that there is more than one orthographic lexicon used for reading and writing although this hypothesis awaits converging data from brain imaging.

Readings Parkin Chapter 8. Ellis & Young (Chapter 7&8). Graham, N. (2000). Dysgraphia in dementia. Neurocase, 6, 365-376. Graham, N. Patterson, K & Hodges, J (2000). The impact of semantic memory loss on spelling: evidence from semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia,38, 143-163. Weekes, B (1994). A cognitive neuropsychological analysis of allograph errors from a patient with acquired dysgraphia. Aphasiology, 8(5), 409-425. Weekes, B.S. & Coltheart, M. (1996). Surface dyslexia and surface dysgraphia: Treatment studies and their theoretical implications. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13(2), 277-315.

Weekes and Coltheart (1996)

One example of this is the fact that the dissociation between letter naming and word recognition provides strong evidence for the modularity of letter specific and word level representations (Berhmann and McLeod, 1995). Similarly, the dissociation between impaired reading and preserved writing (the latter being a more difficult task) is among the strongest pieces of evidence in favor of the view that reading and writing are modular skills. The phenomenon of alexia without agraphia is also germane to the issue of whether there is one lexicon (Behrmann and Bub, 1992) or two lexica (Weekes and Coltheart, 1996) for reading and writing.