Bioinspired Computing Lecture 14

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
© Negnevitsky, Pearson Education, Introduction Introduction Hebbian learning Hebbian learning Generalised Hebbian learning algorithm Generalised.
Advertisements

Bioinspired Computing Lecture 16
2806 Neural Computation Self-Organizing Maps Lecture Ari Visa.
Neural Networks Dr. Peter Phillips. The Human Brain (Recap of week 1)
Un Supervised Learning & Self Organizing Maps. Un Supervised Competitive Learning In Hebbian networks, all neurons can fire at the same time Competitive.
Neural Networks Chapter 9 Joost N. Kok Universiteit Leiden.
Unsupervised learning. Summary from last week We explained what local minima are, and described ways of escaping them. We investigated how the backpropagation.
5/16/2015Intelligent Systems and Soft Computing1 Introduction Introduction Hebbian learning Hebbian learning Generalised Hebbian learning algorithm Generalised.
Kohonen Self Organising Maps Michael J. Watts
Artificial neural networks:
Competitive learning College voor cursus Connectionistische modellen M Meeter.
Unsupervised Networks Closely related to clustering Do not require target outputs for each input vector in the training data Inputs are connected to a.
X0 xn w0 wn o Threshold units SOM.
Bioinspired Computing Lecture 14 Alternative Neural Networks Netta Cohen.
Neural Networks Basic concepts ArchitectureOperation.
Slides are based on Negnevitsky, Pearson Education, Lecture 8 Artificial neural networks: Unsupervised learning n Introduction n Hebbian learning.
1 Chapter 11 Neural Networks. 2 Chapter 11 Contents (1) l Biological Neurons l Artificial Neurons l Perceptrons l Multilayer Neural Networks l Backpropagation.
Un Supervised Learning & Self Organizing Maps Learning From Examples
COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE
Chapter Seven The Network Approach: Mind as a Web.
November 24, 2009Introduction to Cognitive Science Lecture 21: Self-Organizing Maps 1 Self-Organizing Maps (Kohonen Maps) In the BPN, we used supervised.
Un Supervised Learning & Self Organizing Maps Learning From Examples
Lecture 09 Clustering-based Learning
Radial-Basis Function Networks
Machine Learning. Learning agent Any other agent.
KOHONEN SELF ORGANISING MAP SEMINAR BY M.V.MAHENDRAN., Reg no: III SEM, M.E., Control And Instrumentation Engg.
Presentation on Neural Networks.. Basics Of Neural Networks Neural networks refers to a connectionist model that simulates the biophysical information.
Self Organizing Maps (SOM) Unsupervised Learning.
Self Organized Map (SOM)
CZ5225: Modeling and Simulation in Biology Lecture 5: Clustering Analysis for Microarray Data III Prof. Chen Yu Zong Tel:
Artificial Neural Network Theory and Application Ashish Venugopal Sriram Gollapalli Ulas Bardak.
Artificial Neural Nets and AI Connectionism Sub symbolic reasoning.
Introduction to Neural Networks. Neural Networks in the Brain Human brain “computes” in an entirely different way from conventional digital computers.
IE 585 Introduction to Neural Networks. 2 Modeling Continuum Unarticulated Wisdom Articulated Qualitative Models Theoretic (First Principles) Models Empirical.
Artificial Neural Network Unsupervised Learning
Self organizing maps 1 iCSC2014, Juan López González, University of Oviedo Self organizing maps A visualization technique with data dimension reduction.
Chapter 3 Neural Network Xiu-jun GONG (Ph. D) School of Computer Science and Technology, Tianjin University
Modular Neural Networks: SOM, ART, and CALM Jaap Murre University of Amsterdam University of Maastricht
Self-Organising Networks This is DWC-lecture 8 of Biologically Inspired Computing; about Kohonen’s SOM, what it’s useful for, and some applications.
NEURAL NETWORKS FOR DATA MINING
Neural coding (1) LECTURE 8. I.Introduction − Topographic Maps in Cortex − Synesthesia − Firing rates and tuning curves.
Deriving connectivity patterns in the primary visual cortex from spontaneous neuronal activity and feature maps Barak Blumenfeld, Dmitri Bibitchkov, Shmuel.
1 Chapter 11 Neural Networks. 2 Chapter 11 Contents (1) l Biological Neurons l Artificial Neurons l Perceptrons l Multilayer Neural Networks l Backpropagation.
Machine Learning Neural Networks (3). Understanding Supervised and Unsupervised Learning.
Self Organizing Feature Map CS570 인공지능 이대성 Computer Science KAIST.
Bioinspired Computing Lecture 7 Alternative Neural Networks M. De Kamps, Netta Cohen.
Topological Neural Networks Bear, Connors & Paradiso (2001). Neuroscience: Exploring The Brain. Pg. 474.
Neural Networks - Lecture 81 Unsupervised competitive learning Particularities of unsupervised learning Data clustering Neural networks for clustering.
UNSUPERVISED LEARNING NETWORKS
1 Lecture 6 Neural Network Training. 2 Neural Network Training Network training is basic to establishing the functional relationship between the inputs.
Semiconductors, BP&A Planning, DREAM PLAN IDEA IMPLEMENTATION.
Neural Networks Presented by M. Abbasi Course lecturer: Dr.Tohidkhah.
Self Organizing Maps: Clustering With unsupervised learning there is no instruction and the network is left to cluster patterns. All of the patterns within.
Lecture 14, CS5671 Clustering Algorithms Density based clustering Self organizing feature maps Grid based clustering Markov clustering.
COSC 4426 AJ Boulay Julia Johnson Artificial Neural Networks: Introduction to Soft Computing (Textbook)
Supervised Learning – Network is presented with the input and the desired output. – Uses a set of inputs for which the desired outputs results / classes.
Ch. 13 A face in the crowd: which groups of neurons process face stimuli, and how do they interact? KARI L. HOFFMANN 2009/1/13 BI, Population Coding Seminar.
Computational Intelligence: Methods and Applications Lecture 9 Self-Organized Mappings Włodzisław Duch Dept. of Informatics, UMK Google: W Duch.
J. Kubalík, Gerstner Laboratory for Intelligent Decision Making and Control Artificial Neural Networks II - Outline Cascade Nets and Cascade-Correlation.
BIOPHYSICS 6702 – ENCODING NEURAL INFORMATION
Self-Organizing Network Model (SOM) Session 11
Data Mining, Neural Network and Genetic Programming
Unsupervised Learning and Neural Networks
Unsupervised Learning and Autoencoders
Lecture 22 Clustering (3).
Feature mapping: Self-organizing Maps
The Network Approach: Mind as a Web
Unsupervised Networks Closely related to clustering
Co-operative neural networks and ‘integrated’ classification
Presentation transcript:

Bioinspired Computing Lecture 14 Alternative Neural Networks Netta Cohen

Last time Today Biologically inspired associative memories moves away from bio- realistic model Unsupervised learning Working examples and applications Pros, Cons & open questions SOM (Competitive) Nets Neuroscience applications GasNets. Robotic control Attractor neural nets: Other Neural Nets

Spatial Codes Natural neural nets often code similar things close together. The auditory and visual cortex provide examples. Neural Material Low Freq. High Freq. Frequency Sensitivity Orientation Sensitivity Neural Material 0° 359° Another example: touch receptors in the human body. "Almost every region of the body is represented by a corresponding region in both the primary motor cortex and the somatic sensory cortex" (Geschwind 1979:106). "The finger tips of humans have the highest density of receptors: about 2500 per square cm!" (Kandel and Jessell 1991:374). This representation is often dubbed the homunculus (or little man in the brain) Picture from http://www.dubinweb.com/brain/3.html

Kohonen Nets In a Kohonen net, a number of input neurons feed a single lattice of neurons. The output pattern is produced across the lattice surface. Lattice Fully Connected Input Nodes Output Pattern Large volumes of data are compressed using spatial/ topological relationships within the training set. Thus the lattice becomes an efficient distributed representation of the input.

Kohonen Nets also known as self-organising maps (SOMs) Important features: Self-organisation of a distributed representation of inputs. This is a form of unsupervised learning: The underlying learning principle: competition among nodes known as “winner takes all”. Only winners get to “learn” & losers decay. The competition is enforced by the network architecture: each node has a self-excitatory connection and inhibits all its neighbours. Spatial patterns are formed by imposing the learning rule throughout the local neighbourhood of the winner.

Training Self-Organising Maps A simple training algorithm might look like this: Randomly initialise the network input weights Normalise all inputs so they are size-independent Define a local neighbourhood and a learning rate For each item in the training set Find the lattice node most excited by the input Alter the input weights for this node and those nearby such that they more closely resemble the input vector, i.e., at each node, the input weight update rule is: w = r (x-w) Reduce the learning rate & the neighbourhood size Goto 2 (another pass through the training set)

Training Self-Organising Maps (cont) Gradually the net self-organises into a map of the inputs, clustering the input data by recruiting areas of the net for related inputs or features in the inputs. The size of the neighbourhood roughly corresponds to the resolution of the mapped features.

How Does It Work? Imagine a 2D training set with clusters of data points The nodes in the lattice are initially randomly sensitive. Vertical Blue Red Horizontal Gradually, they will “migrate” towards the input data. Nodes that are neighbours in the lattice will tend to become sensitive to similar inputs. Effective resource allocation: dense parts of the input space recruit more nodes than sparse areas. Another example: The travelling salesman problem Applet from http://www.patol.com/java/TSP/index.html

How does the brain perform classification? One area of the cortex (the inferior temporal cortex or IT) has been linked with two important functions: object recognition object classification These tasks seem to be shape/colour specific but independent of object size, position, relative motion or speed, brightness or texture. Indeed, category-specific impairments have been linked to IT injuries.

How does the brain perform classification (cont)? Questions: How do IT neurons encode objects/categories? e.g., local versus distributed representations/coding temporal versus rate coding at the neuronal level Can we recruit ANNs to answer such questions? Can ANNs perform classification as well given similar data? Recently, Elizabeth Thomas and colleagues performed experiments on the activity of IT neurons during an exercise of image classification in monkeys and used a Kohonen net to analyse the data.

The experiment Monkeys were trained to distinguish between a training set of pictures of trees and various other objects. The monkeys were considered trained when they reached a 95% success rate. Trained monkeys were now shown new images of trees and other objects. As they classified the objects, the activity in IT neurons in their brains was recorded. All in all 226 neurons were recorded on various occasions and over many different images. The data collected was the mean firing rate of each neuron in response to each image. 25% of neurons responded only to one category, but 75% were not category specific. All neurons were image-specific. Problem: Not all neurons were recorded for all images & No images were tested across all neurons. In fact, when a Table of neuronal responses for each image was created, it was more than 80% empty. E. Thomas et al, J. Cog. Neurosci. (2001)

Experimental Results Question: Given the partial data, is there sufficient information to classify images as trees or non-trees? Answer: A 2-node Kohonen net trained on the Table of neuronal responses was able to classify new images with an 84% success rate. Question: Are categories encoded by category-specific neurons? Answer: Delete data of category-specific neuron responses from Table. The success rate of the Kohonen net was degraded but only minimally. A control set with random data deletions yielded similar results. Conclusion: Category-specific neurons are not important for categorisation! E. Thomas et al, J. Cog. Neurosci. (2001)

Experimental Results (cont.) Question: Which neurons are important, if any? Answer: An examination of the weights that contribute most to the output in the Kohonen net revealed that a small subset of neurons (<50) that are not category-specific yet respond with different intensities to different categories are crucial for correct classification. Conclusions: The IT employs a distributed representation to encode categories of different images. The redundancy in this encoding allows for graceful degradation so that even with 80% of data missing and many neurons deleted, sufficient information is present for classification purposes. The fact that only rate information was used suggests that temporal information is less important here. E. Thomas et al, J. Cog. Neurosci. (2001)

Space in Neural Nets Kohonen nets teach us an important lesson about the ability of neurons to encode information, not only in weights, but also in spatial organisation. What are the consequences for network dynamics? Can these principles be extended beyond simple centre-surround constraints of self-excitation and neighbour inhibition? Once again, insight may be gained by returning to the biological domain and asking how space affects brain activity. While always aware of the immense richness of neuronal behaviour, we have, until today, considered them to be minimal processors communicating via well-defined circuits. What have we neglected? We have turned our networks into abstract, computing tools, disconnected from the real world in which problems are defined. We have also robbed the networks of enormous freedom by restricting the encoding of information to series of weights.

Neurotransmitters in brain many neurotransmitters do not just excite or inhibit neurons release gases such as nitric oxide (NO) the behaviour of these diffusing gaseous modulators is very different from that of standard neurotransmitters… Unlike standard neurotransmitters which are unable to travel far from their point of origin, NO is a small gas molecule that is free to diffuse slowly away from its origin, unhindered by intervening cellular structures. NO secreted by a neuron affects all neurons within range – regardless of circuitry. Such influences go beyond excitation or inhibition. NO has the potential to modulate many aspects of the neuron’s behaviour.

GasNets Researchers at Sussex’s Centre for Computational Neuroscience and Robotics have been developing an ANN architecture inspired by these findings which they call GasNets. Their model is a generalisation of the dynamic recurrent neural nets. Neurons are organised on a 2D grid, with all-to-all synaptic connections. Active neurons can also secrete gas. input output The concentration of gas at the location of a neuron modulates its sigmoid activity function, either increasing or decreasing the steepness of the curve (and its ability to secrete gas itself). All GasNet figures courtesy of Phil Husbands, Mick O’Shea, Tom Smith, & Nick Jakobi

A Control Task… A robot lives in a walled arena. Its task is to approach a white triangle painted on the wall and avoid a white rectangle, using only very crude visual input (typically a handful of pixels from a camera mounted on the robot). drive wheel caster motor camera bumpers Performing this shape discrimination under noisy lighting conditions is a non-trivial task, especially given the limited visual input available to the controller.

Non-Gaseous Solutions The same researchers had previously evolved more standard dynamical neural nets to solve this task: These controllers took ~6000 generations to discover. What kind of GasNet controllers would evolve? Would they exhibit advantages over other kinds of ANN? all figures courtesy of Sussex CCNR

GasNet Controllers Two kinds of successful GasNet controller were evolved, each taking ~1000 generations to discover. Here’s one: GasNets evolved 3 or 4 times faster and were simpler than the network architectures to which they were compared.

The GasNet Solutions Both GasNets perform robustly despite the noisy lighting conditions and “outrageously low bandwidth” vision. Far: Ballistic Contrast between two offset visual inputs is used to detect triangle edge. Near: Closed-loop “Scanning behaviour” continually modulates approach The evolved visual morphology always played a crucial role. Active visual strategies solved the task, rather than central reasoning.

Why Does Gas Make It Better? While still an open question, several possibilities include: Gas diffuses widely, allowing large parts of the network to be inhibited or excited simultaneously. Gas concentration varies much slower than the flow of ‘electrical’ activation around the synaptic connections. There may be useful interactions between the slow gas and fast activation dynamics. Combination of these ideas may explain why solutions appear easier to build from GasNets than from non-gas dynamical ANNs. Research into these possibilities is currently ongoing by Chris Buckley in the Biosystems group of the School of Computing.

From Biology to ANNs & Back Neuroscience and studies of animal behaviour have led to new ideas for artificial learning, communication, cooperation & competition. Simplistic cartoon models of these mechanisms can lead to new paradigms and impressive technologies. Dynamic Neural Nets are helping us understand real-time adaptation and problem-solving under changing conditions. Hopfield nets shed new insight on mechanisms of association and the benefits of unsupervised learning. Thomas’ work helps unravel coding structures in the cortex. Husbands et al.’s GasNets are helping neuroscientists to understand the behaviour of NO and other local influences in real nervous systems and for improved robot control.

Next time… Guest lecture series on Genetic Evolution and Genetic Programming. Reading Elizabeth Thomas et al (2001) “Encoding of categories by noncategory-specific neurons in the inferior temporal cortex”, J. Cog. Neurosci. 13: 190-200. Phil Husbands, Tom Smith, Nick Jakobi & Michael O’Shea (1998). “Better living through chemistry: Evolving GasNets for robot control”, Connection Science, 10:185-210. Ezequiel Di Paolo (2003). Organismically-inspired robotics: Homeostatic adaptation and natural teleology beyond the closed sensorimotor loop, in: K. Murase & T. Asakura (Eds) Dynamical Systems Approach to Embodiment and Sociality, Advanced Knowledge International., Adelaide, pp 19 - 42. Ezequiel Di Paolo (2000) “Homeostatic adaptation to inversion of the visual field and other sensorimotor disruptions”, SAB2000, MIT Press.