Figure 12: Each group’s average correlations are plotted for cued, uncued and dark probes. Group differences were observed during the uncued and dark probes.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The bead maze: Adapting the water maze task to the human manipulatory scale E.L. Loda, A.B. Thoennes, J.R. Köppen, S.S. Winter, D.A. Hamilton, D.G. Wallace.
Advertisements

192-IgG Saporin lesions of the medial septum or nucleus basalis magnocellularis disrupt exploratory trip organization. D.G. Wallace*, S.S. Winter, M.M.
Methods of Studying Behavior. Open Field Test A measure of general activity. In automated versions infrared sensors or video tracking is used to obtain.
Research Methods in Psychology Spring 2012, PSYC I. A brief description of the Morris water maze task used to measure spatial navigation in rodents.
AMPHETAMINE AND HALOPERIDOL MAY ALTER TEMPORAL PROCESSING DURING A NATURALLY OCCURRING BEHAVIOR P.S.Wallace 1* ; E.F.Field 2 ; I.Q.Whishaw 2 ; D.G.Wallace.
BNST lesions aggravate behavioral despair but do not impair navigational learning in rats Pezük, Göz, Aksoy, Canbeyli Brain Research Bulletin-2006.
Methods Results Conclusions Selective cholinergic lesions of the medial septum disrupt dead reckoning based navigation M.M. Martin*; M.D. Schultz; S.S.
Deficits in Spatial Cognition After TBI Revealed By Virtual Morris Water Maze R.W. Skelton & S.P. Ross, Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC,
Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Blindly Biased: Restricting Cue Access Impacts Spatial Orientation in a Human Analogue of the Water Maze B.P. Apger, D.M.
Methods Results Conclusions Selective hippocampal cholinergic deafferentation disrupts exploratory trip organization D.G. Wallace*; S.K. Knapp; J.A. Silver;
Methods Results Conclusions Walking the plank: Role of the medial septum in distance estimation S.S. Winter; M.M. Martin; D.G. Wallace Dept Psychology,
Humans were able to accurately use dead-reckoning to estimate distance and direction on a smaller scale. Participants modulated their peak speed on the.
A Human Analogue of Rat Food Hoarding Behavior: Searching Path Kinematics Influence the Accuracy of Dead Reckoning S.N. Choudhry; M.M. Martin; D.G. Wallace*
Conclusions Both groups set up a home base under novel-dark and light conditions and display a similar topographical organization of exploratory trips.
Unilateral lesions of the medial agranular cortex produce a persistent disruption in the organization of food protection behaviors S. J. WAGNER, M. J.
Methods Results Conclusions Mammillothalamic tract lesions disrupt self-movement cue processing in a food hoarding task. *S.S. Winter; J.L. McMillin, D.G.
Methods Results Conclusions Getting lost in the dark: Disruption of directional but not distance estimates following mammillothalamic tract lesions *S.S.
PART 4: BEHAVIORAL PLASTICITY #24: SPATIAL NAVIGATION IN RATS I
Navigating with fingers and feet: Comparative analysis of human and rat movement kinematics during non-visual spatial tasks J.R. Raines, N.D. McNeal, J.L.
Figure 4: Average dodger speeds are shown for MS (left panel) and NB (right panel) groups. Dodging (circles) and bracing (triangles) speeds are plotted.
Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Running on rungs: Serial pattern learning on the horizontal ladder *M.J Hylin 1 ; S.S. Winter 1 ; T. L. Marsiglio 1 ;
Figure 5: Initial (left panel) and terminal (right panel) distances between the noses of the dodger and robber rats are plotted separately for dodging.
Methods Experiment 1 Conclusions Does the home base anchor spatial mapping? *D.G. Wallace 1 ; D.A. Hamilton 2 ; A.D. Choate 1 1 Department of Psychology,
Do Rats with Fimbria-Fornix Lesions Ever Return Home? S.L.Bennett 1* ; D.A.Hamilton 2 ; D.G.Wallace 1 1. Dept Psychology, Northern Illinois Univ., DeKalb,
A vanishing sex difference Effects of environmental contingencies in a virtual Morris water task on male and female spatial navigation Mühl, Griego, Kabisch,
Neural Progenitor Cells as Replacement Therapy for Diseased and Aging Brains. R.G. Jarman, E. Alveraz, C.R. Freed; Division of Clinical Pharmacology, Dept.
Neural mechanisms of Spatial Learning. Spatial Learning Materials covered in previous lectures Historical development –Tolman and cognitive maps the classic.
A Pro-Inflammatory Agent, Lipopolysaccharide, Can Mimic the Effects of Aging on Spatial Reference Memory E. R. Escobedo 1, K. Magnusson 2,3 — 1 BioResource.
Dendritic complexity is reduced in neurons of nucleus accumbens after social isolation Ue-Cheung Ho 1,2, Yu-Chun Wang 1,2, Chun-Chieh Liao 2 and Li-Jen.
Lesions of the Basal Forebrain Cholinergic System Impair Task Acquisition and Abolish Cortical Plasticity Associated with Motor Skill Learning James M.
Children Getting Lost: Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans.
A new neural framework for visuospatial processing Group #4 Alicia Iafonaro Alyona Koneva Barbara Kim Isaac Del Rio.
Chapter 38 Diffraction Patterns and Polarization.
Learning like a girl? “Estrogen modulates learning in female rats by acting directly at distinct memory systems” L. Zurkovsky et al. Neuroscience (2007)
The Basics of Experimentation: Concepts, Definitions and Examples.
Neglecting to protect: DCS lesions disrupt food protection behavior organization. 1 Blankenship, P.A., 1 Wolf, C. N., 2 Cheatwood, J.L., & 1 Wallace, D.G.
Prior cocaine exposure disrupts extinction of fear conditioning
Making sense of the senseless: Evaluating sex differences of ASO
Dmitriy Aronov, David W. Tank  Neuron 
Within a Mixed-Frequency Visual Environment
11/24/2018 Sensory Re-Weighting In Human Postural Control During Moving-Scene Perturbations A. Mahboobin1, P. Loughlin1,2, Ph.D., M. Redfern3,2, Ph.D.,
Volume 25, Issue 10, Pages (May 2015)
Methods Objectives Results Conclusions References
Hiroaki Norimoto, Yuji Ikegaya  Current Biology 
Volume 27, Issue 9, Pages (May 2017)
Jung Hoon Sul, Hoseok Kim, Namjung Huh, Daeyeol Lee, Min Whan Jung 
Volume 96, Issue 5, Pages e4 (December 2017)
E.H. Baeg, Y.B. Kim, K. Huh, I. Mook-Jung, H.T. Kim, M.W. Jung  Neuron 
Sensory Detection of Food Rapidly Modulates Arcuate Feeding Circuits
Hill-Aina Steffenach, Menno Witter, May-Britt Moser, Edvard I. Moser 
Acetylcholine-secreting cells improve age-induced memory deficits
Space Psychology 3926.
Spatially Periodic Activation Patterns of Retrosplenial Cortex Encode Route Sub-spaces and Distance Traveled  Andrew S. Alexander, Douglas A. Nitz  Current.
Cognition and Immunity
Volume 80, Issue 2, Pages (October 2013)
Sharon C. Furtak, Omar J. Ahmed, Rebecca D. Burwell  Neuron 
Mosquitoes Use Vision to Associate Odor Plumes with Thermal Targets
Attentive Tracking of Sound Sources
Grid and Nongrid Cells in Medial Entorhinal Cortex Represent Spatial Location and Environmental Features with Complementary Coding Schemes  Geoffrey W.
Development of Cue Integration in Human Navigation
Tracking Route Progression in the Posterior Parietal Cortex
Volume 48, Issue 2, Pages (October 2005)
Traces of Experience in the Lateral Entorhinal Cortex
Environmental Consistency Determines the Rate of Motor Adaptation
Application of microwave energy in cardiac tissue ablation: from in vitro analyses to clinical use  Mathew R Williams, MD, Michael Knaut, MD, Dany Bérubé,
A Data Partitioning Scheme for Spatial Regression
The Hippocampus, Memory, and Place Cells
Fabian Chersi, Neil Burgess  Neuron 
Volume 25, Issue 10, Pages (May 2015)
Earth and Space Science
Presentation transcript:

Figure 12: Each group’s average correlations are plotted for cued, uncued and dark probes. Group differences were observed during the uncued and dark probes. The septohippocampal GABAergic system is selectively involved in spatial orientation. Direction estimation When provided access to environmental cues both groups made direct returns to the refuge. GAT1-Sap lesions disrupted homeward segment topography when access was limited to self-movement cues or environmental and self-movement cues were placed in conflict. Distance estimation When provided access to proximal environmental cues both groups modulated their peak speed relative to the distance to the refuge. GAT1-Sap lesions disrupted homeward segment kinematics when access was limited to distal environmental cues or self- movement cues. Self-movement cue processing depends on septohippocampal GABAergic function. These results add to growing literature demonstrating a selective role for limbic system structures in spatial orientation. Spatial orientation depends on a system of processes that an animal uses to navigate through space. Spatial disorientation results when there is damage to one of the neural systems that sustain these processes. For example, wandering is a behavior associated with being lost and occurs during the progression of Alzheimer’s disease (AD). AD effects the neurobiology of the central nervous system, which results in deterioration of the cholinergic projection to the hippocampus. The correlation between AD neuropathology and cognitive function has led to the development of the cholinergic hypothesis, which posits that the predominant features observed in AD may be due to a dysfunction of the cholinergic system. The cholinergic hypothesis has led researchers to develop therapies that enhance cholinergic function in those with AD. These treatments are at best, only mildly effective; therefore, an investigation of the role of other systems in AD neuropathology is needed. Identifying the function of other neurotransmitter systems will provide a more comprehensive understanding of spatial orientation, allowing for a better analysis of wandering behavior in AD and the related neuropathology. Methods Results Conclusions Role of the septohippocampal GABAergic system in spatial orientation J.R. Köppen; M. M. Sheehan; S.S. Winter; J.L. Cheatwood; D.G. Wallace Dept Psychology, Northern Illinois Univ., DeKalb, IL, USA Dept of Anatomy, Southern Illinois Univ. School of Medicine, Carbondale, IL, USA Female Long-Evans rats (n=7) were trained in a food hoarding paradigm. Initial training involved placing a “cued” refuge at the periphery of a circular table and shaping rats to carry randomly placed 1 g banana pellets back to a cued refuge. Three probes were used to dissociate environmental and self-movement cue use: 1) The uncued probe involved placing the refuge below the surface of the table, limiting rats to use distal environmental or self-movement cues to locate the refuge; 2) The dark probe involved using the hidden refuge with the room lights off, limiting rats to use self-movement cues to locate the refuge; 3) The new probe involved placing the hidden refuge on the opposite side of table, placing environmental and self-movement cues in conflict. The rats experienced the cued, hidden and dark probes twice before surgery. The rats then received either GABAergic lesions (n=2) or sham lesions (n=5). The rats were tested again in the same probes as in pre-surgery for two days. The “new” probe was given post- surgery on the last day of testing. Rats received 2 trials in the new condition. Figure 1: Photographs of the testing room are shown for dark (left) and light conditions (right). The cued refuge (17 x 26 x 12cm) is positioned at the northern edge of the table (200cm) and the uncued refuge is positioned at southern edge. Figure 3: Post-surgery homeward segment topographic profiles are plotted for representative sham (left) and GAT1-Sap (right) rats during the Cued probe. Figure 4: Each group’s average homeward segment path circuity is plotted for pre- and post- surgery testing during the Cued probe. Both groups made direct returns to refuge during post-surgery testing. Figure 7: Post-surgery homeward segment topographic profiles are plotted for representative sham (left) and GAT1-Sap (right) rats during the Dark probe. Figure 5: Post-surgery homeward segment topographic profiles are plotted for representative sham (left) and GAT1-Sap (right) rats during the Uncued probe. Figure 8: Each group’s average homeward segment path circuity is plotted for pre- and post-surgery testing during the Dark probe. The GAT1- Sap group followed circuitous paths to the refuge during post-surgery testing, relative to the Sham group. Figure 6: Each group’s average homeward segment path circuity is plotted for pre- and post- surgery testing during the Uncued probe. Both groups made direct returns to refuge during post-surgery testing. Figure 9: Post-surgery outward and homeward segment topographic profiles are plotted for representative sham and GAT1-Sap rats during the New probe. Both rats perseverate to the former refuge location (solid square) on trip 1; whereas, the GAT1-Sap rat continues to perseverate to at the former refuge location during trip 2. Trip 1 Trip 2 Correspondence: J. D. Wallace /KKK21 Introduction Sham GAT1-Saporin Figure 11: Homeward segment peak speeds are plotted relative to the homeward segment minimum distance for a representative sham and GAT1-Sap rat under cued, uncued and dark conditions. CuedUncued Dark Sham GAT1-Saporin Figure 10: Each group’s average number of progressions to the former refuge location is plotted for both trips during the New probe. Although the Sham group decreased in the number of progressions observed from Trip 1 to Trip 2, no change was observed in the GAT1-Sap group. Figure 1: Photographs of coronal sections stained for AChE or parvalbumin are presented for representative sham (left) and GAT1-Saporin (right) rats. Sham GAT1-Saporin